User:Jczhu/arapaho merged
Arapaho | |
---|---|
Hinónoʼeitíít | |
Native to | United States |
Region | Wind River Indian Reservation, Wyoming; Oklahoma |
Ethnicity | Arapaho |
Native speakers | 1,087, 10% of ethnic population (2009-2013)[1] |
Algic
| |
Dialects |
|
Language codes | |
ISO 639-2 | arp |
ISO 639-3 | arp |
Glottolog | arap1274 |
The Arapaho (Arapahoe) language (Hinónoʼeitíít)[2] is one of the Plains Algonquian languages, closely related to Gros Ventre and other Arapahoan languages. It is spoken by the Arapaho of Wyoming and Oklahoma. Speakers of Arapaho primarily live on the Wind River Indian Reservation in Wyoming, though some have affiliation with the Cheyenne living in western Oklahoma.[citation needed] (infobox to the right also from original page)
Classification
[edit]Arapaho is an Algonquian language of the Algic family.[3]
History
[edit]By the 1850s, Arapaho bands formed two tribes: the Northern Arapaho and Southern Arapaho. Since 1878 the Northern Arapaho have lived with the Eastern Shoshone on the Wind River Reservation in Wyoming and are federally recognized as the Arapahoe Tribe of the Wind River Reservation. The Southern Arapaho live with the Southern Cheyenne in Oklahoma. Together their members are enrolled as the federally recognized Cheyenne and Arapaho Tribes.[citation needed]
After World War 2, the Northern Arapaho tribe tended to use English, not Arapaho, when raising their children. However, Arapaho speakers within the tribe still primarily speak Arapaho amongst each other. The Northern Arapaho additionally have had relatively less intermingling with other tribes and non-Native Americans compared to the Southern Arapaho who live amongst a predominantly non-Native American population.[4]: 2
Current status
[edit]The exact number of Arapaho speakers is not precisely known; however it has been estimated that the language currently retains between 250 [5] and 1,000 [6] active users. Arapaho has limited development outside of the home; however, it is used in some films[7] and the Bible was translated into the language in 1903.[8] According to one source, under 300 people over the age of 50 speak the language in Wyoming, and in Oklahoma the language is used by "only a handful of people . . . all near eighty or older".[5] As of 1996, there were approximately 1,000 speakers among the Northern Arapaho.[9] As of 2008, the authors of a newly published grammar estimated that there were slightly over 250 fluent speakers, plus "quite a few near-fluent passive understanders".[5] In 2008, it was reported that a school had been opened to teach the language to children.[10] Arapaho language camps were held in Summer 2015 at Wind River Tribal College and in St. Stephens, Wyoming.[11] Currently, the language may be acquired by children, for a population estimate as recent as 2007 lists an increase to 1,000 speakers and notes that the language is in use in schools, bilingual education efforts begun on Wind River Reservation in the 1980s and the Arapaho Language Lodge, a successful immersion program, was established in 1993.[12] "The Arapaho Project" is an effort made by the Arapaho people to promote and restore their traditional language and culture.[13] Despite hope for the language, its relatively few active users and the fact that it has seen recent population decreases render Arapaho an endangered language. Ethnologue deems it "threatened," meaning that some children are learning it but it is threatened by other languages and it may be losing speakers.
Dialects
[edit]Besawunena, only attested from a wordlist collected by Kroeber, differs only slightly from Arapaho, though a few of its sound changes resemble those seen in Gros Ventre. It had speakers among the Northern Arapaho as recently as the late 1920s.[citation needed]
Phonology
[edit]Among the sound changes in the evolution from Proto-Algonquian to Arapaho are the loss of Proto-Algonquian *k, followed by *p becoming either /k/ or /tʃ/; the two Proto-Algonquian semivowels merging to either /n/ or /j/; the change from *s to /n/ in word-initial position, and *m becoming /b/ or /w/ depending on the following vowel. Arapaho is unusual among Algonquian languages in retaining the contrast between the reconstructed phonemes *r and *θ (generally as /n/ and /θ/, respectively).[14][15][16] These and other changes serve to give Arapaho a phonological system very divergent from that of Proto-Algonquian and other Algonquian languages, and even from languages spoken in the adjacent Great Basin. Some examples comparing Arapaho words with their cognates in Proto-Algonquian can illustrate this:[17][18][16]
(table below from the original page)
Proto-Algonquian | Arapaho | Translation |
---|---|---|
*erenyiwa | hinén | 'man' |
*wa•poswa | nóːku | 'hare' |
*nepyi | nétʃ | 'water' |
*weθkweni | hís | '(his) liver' |
*mexka•či | wóʔoːθ | 'leg' |
*si•pi•wi | níːtʃíː | 'river' |
*sakime•wa | nóúbeː | 'mosquito' > 'fly' |
*akweHmi | hóú | 'blanket, robe' |
*ka•ka•kiwa | hóuu | 'raven' > 'crow' |
*aθemwa | héθ | 'dog' |
Vowels
[edit]At the level of pronunciation, Arapaho words cannot begin with a vowel, so where the underlying form of a word begins with a vowel, a prothetic /h/ is added.[19]
Arapaho has a series of four short vowels /i e o u/ (pronounced [ɪ ɛ ɔ ʊ]) and four long vowels /iː eː oː uː/ (customarily written ⟨ii ee oo uu⟩ and pronounced [iː ɛː ɔː uː]). The difference in length is phonemically distinctive: compare hísiʼ, "tick" with híísiʼ, "day", and hócoo, "steak" with hóócoo, "devil".[19] /i/ and /u/ are mostly in complementary distribution, as, with very few exceptions, the former does not occur after velar consonants, and the latter only occurs after them. /u/ does have some exceptions as in the free variants kokíy ~ kokúy, "gun"; kookiyón ~ kookuyón, "for no reason"; and bííʼoxíyoo ~ bííʼoxúyoo, "Found in the Grass" (a mythological character). There is only one minimal pair to illustrate the contrast in distribution: núhuʼ, "this" versus níhiʼ-, "X was done with Y", in which níhiʼ- only occurs in bound form.[20]
Remarkably, unlike more than 98% of the world's languages, Arapaho has no low vowels, such as /a/.[21]
In addition, there are four diphthongs, /ei ou oe ie/, and several triphthongs, /eii oee ouu/ as well as extended sequences of vowels such as /eee/ with stress on either the first or the last vowel in the combination.[22] Each diphthong also has a lengthened, contrastive counterpart that lengthens the second vowel. For example, hou "blanket" versus houu "crow".[4]: 15
(the table below also from the original page)
Front | Back | |
---|---|---|
High | ɪ | ʊ |
Mid | ɛ | ɔ |
Consonants
[edit]<deleted their consonant section and table due to disagreement with the table found in my grammar and lack of citation>
The consonants in Arapaho are given in the table below in their orthographical form along with their IPA equivalent if it differs.[4]: 14
Labial | Dental | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | Glottal | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stop | b | t | k | ' [ʔ] | ||
Affricate | c [t͡ʃ] | |||||
Fricative | 3 [θ] | s | x | h | ||
Nasal | n | |||||
Semivowel | w | y [j] |
The glottal consonants appear less pervasively word-initially. The glottal stop /'/ only occurs at the start of a subset of particles and the glottal fricative /h/ is only pronounced initially before an underlying vowel-initial word as vowel-initial words do not occur in Arapaho.[4]: 14
Allophony
[edit]The phoneme /b/ (the voiced bilabial stop) has a voiceless allophone [p] that occurs before other consonants or at the end of a word. The plosives /tʃ/, /k/, and /t/ are pronounced without aspiration in most environments, but are aspirated before other consonants or at the end of a word, or when preceding a syllable-final sequence of short vowel + /h/. In this same environment /b/ is aspirated and devoiced. For example, the grammatical prefix cih- is pronounced [tʃʰɪh], the grammatical prefix tih- is pronounced [tʰɪh], and the word héétbihʼínkúútiinoo, "I will turn out the lights" is het[b̥ʰ]ihʼínkúútiinoo.[19]
Prosody
[edit]Arapaho is a pitch accent language. There are two phonemic tones: high (marked with an acute accent) or "normal" (unmarked). The contrast can be illustrated with the pair hónoosóóʼ, "it is fancy" and honoosóóʼ, "it is raining." Long vowels and vowel sequences can carry a contour tone from high to low, as in hou3íne-, "to hang" (where the first syllable has a normal tone) versus hóu3íne-, "to float" (where the first syllable has a high+normal, or falling, tone). Although tonal contrasts are distinctive in Arapaho, minimal pairs such as those listed above are rare.[23]
Syllable Structures
[edit]Syllables tend to have the structure CV(C), where the final consonant, if it is present, is either a single consonant, or /hC/.[24]: 45 In general, [c]onsonant clusters in Arapaho can only be two consonants long. Consonant clusters do not occur word initially, and /hC/ is the only that occurs word finally. The only consonant cluster that is "base generated" (exists in the most underlying representation of words) is /hC/. At the "surface" (at the level of actual pronunciation), other clusters arise by phonological processes including vowel syncope, or by juxtaposition of morphemes.[citation needed]
Vowel-initial, onset-less syllables, however, can occur due to partitioning of vowel clusters. An example of partitioning a cluster of 3 identical vowels into syllables is ní.ii.non ('tepee'). The vowel cluster is not always split into short vowel followed by long vowel; the location of the partition depends on Arapaho's complex pitch accent system. For example, another word with a sequence of 3 vowels, but with a different partitioning of vowels into syllables is hóo.ó ('bed').[4]: 45
However, sometimes the vowel cluster does not divide and the whole cluster becomes the nucleus of the syllable. One example is hi.héio ('his/her aunt (obviative)').[4]: 46
Morphology
[edit]Arapaho is polysynthetic; verbs in particular take multiple morphemes.[4]: 7
Inflectional
[edit]Nouns
[edit]Animacy
[edit]Nouns in Arapaho come in two classes: animate and inanimate. Which category a noun belongs to is part of the lexicon. Being animate does not necessitate “aliveness” (but aliveness does mean animate): doors, planets, ghosts, etc. are considered animate. Some nouns can also be both animate and inanimate, but in these situations, the animate version is more “active” (e.g. a log is inanimate, but a rolling log is animate).[4]: 51–53
Obviation
[edit]Animate nouns can be made obviative or proximate.[4]
When the underlying noun is consonant final, two general patterns can occur. One pattern occurs for the class of nouns that have /ii/ or /uu/ (depending on vowel harmony) as their plural marker. These nouns reuse the plural marker to mark obviative singular and both obviative and proximate plural. For example, /iwoxuuh/, a stem meaning 'elk,' is hiwóxuu in the proximate singular, but becomes hiwóxuuh-uu in obviative singular, proximate plural, and obviative plural.[4]: 58
The other pattern occurs for most other consonant final noun stems and is summarized in the table below. C denotes the final consonant and the bracketed [C] denotes either consonant mutation of C or deletion of some number of stem-final phonemes. /siisiik/ is 'duck'.[4]: 57
Proximate | Obviative | |
---|---|---|
Singular | [C] (siisííc) | C (siisíík) |
Plural | C-o' (siisíík-o’) | C-o (siisíík-o) |
For vowel-final stems, the general pattern is a variation of the first consonant final pattern. Namely, a single marker is used to mark all plural forms and the obviative singular form. For example, /ote/, a stem meaning 'sheep, bighorn sheep,' becomes hóte-’ in proximate singular but hóte-ii in both plural forms and the obviative singular.[4]: 59
Verbs
[edit]Verb categorization
[edit]Verbs are divided into classes depending on the transitivity and animacy of their argument(s). Transitivity of a verb affects how many arguments are affixed to the verb. Notice in the examples below the usage of the transitive form requires the addition of INAN, the inanimate marker for the object (the shoes). Verbal inflection also depends on "orders" like imperative.[4]: 74–75
Intransitive, Animate Subject (AI)[4]: 354
otoonee-noo
IC.buy(AI)-1S
wo’ohn-o
shoe-PL
'I am buying shoes.'/'I am shopping for shoes.'
Transitive, Inanimate Object (TI)[4]: 355
Initial Change
[edit]Initial change (IC) can mark tense and aspect (in particular, "present tense and ongoing aspect or present perfect tense and aspect") under affirmative and conjunct orders.[4]: 73 Differing phonological changes occur depending on the first vowel of the stem. If the vowel is short, it is lengthened. For example, be’éé- 'to be red' becomes bee’éé’ 'it is red'.[4]: 73 Otherwise, an infix is placed before the first vowel. The infix is either /en/ or /on/ and is determined based on harmony with the long vowel. For example, hoowúsee- ('to walk downward') becomes honoowúseenoo ('I am walking downward').[4]: 74
If the first vowel is short and is followed by an /h/, some speakers treat the /h/ as a vowel and use the infix /en/ or /on/ to mark initial change. Other speakers treat the /h/ as a consonant and perform the vowel lengthening process instead.[4]: 74
An irregular form of initial change affects some vowel-initial preverbs by appending an /n/ before the first vowel, rather than the ordinary /h/ that would be prepended to avoid a vowel-initial word. For example, the imperfective /ii/ morpheme becomes nii- instead of the expected hii- when prefixing verbs that would undergo initial change.[4]: 74
Agreement
[edit]In sentences with an explicit noun phrase, separate from the verb, the verb agrees with the noun in terms of animacy, number, and whether the noun is proximate or obviative. The grammatical category, including person, of the noun also needs to agree with the verb.[4]: 59 Note that the categories of subject and object do not affect agreement inflection.[4]: 349 As an example of animacy agreement, the intransitive verb for "to fall" has a form that takes an inanimate subject, nihtéésceníse’ (PAST-on top-fall(II)-0S)[4]: 53 and a form that takes an animate subject, nihtéés'cenísi.[4]: 53
If a verb has a single noun argument that is composed of two different types of noun, most speakers default to the obviative (over proximate) and inanimate (over animate) forms to refer to the composite noun argument in case of conflict. This can be seen in the example below where 'walk' takes an argument that is composed of a composite proximate and obviative noun. Both nouns are animate, but there is conflict regarding proximate or obviative. The verb thus defaults to the obviative plural (4PL). [4]: 60
ne-si[h]
1S-uncle(PROX)
noh
and
i-betebihew-o
3S-wife-OBV
cebisee-ni3i
IC.walk(AI)-4PL
'My uncle and his wife are walking by.'
Derivational
[edit]Nouns
[edit]Arapaho has a number of derivational affixes and processes. Some operate on nouns to form verb-like clauses. For example, the morpheme /tohúút/ can prefix a noun to ask 'what kind of <noun>'. A specific example is hoséíno’ ('meat') when prefixed becomes tohúút-oséíno’ ('What kind of meat is this?') [4]: 107
Verbs
[edit]Derivational morphology on verbs can be grouped into abstract and concrete. Abstract morphemes mark transitivity and the animacy of subject/object for the verb. For example, the basic root /be'/ "red" can be marked with abstract morphemes as follows[4]: 121–122
- /be'-ee/ "to be red," intransitive and takes an inanimate subject
- /be'-eihi/ "to be red," intransitive and takes an animate subject
Concrete morphemes tend to add three types of meanings to the verb[4]: 167–168
- patients and undergoers; attach particularly common nouns after a verb with a transitive meaning to give it an object, resulting in an intransitive verb
- Example: to add /-oox-/ "wood" as an object, transform it to /-ooxu-/ before appending to verb such as /no’ooxu-/ ('haul wood here') and /cowooxu-/ ('haul wood along') [4]: 169
- topics concerning nouns that lack volition; examples include body parts, weather and nature, sensations
- Example: the underlying noun be-sonon ('neck') becomes /-isono-/ before attaching to a verb such as /enisono-/ ('have a long neck') [4]: 170
- methods of achieving action; examples include tools, means of transportation, non-manmade forces such as wind
- Example: /-see/ ('walking') can be added to create an AI (animate subject, intransitive) verb such as no’usee- ('to arrive on foot') and oowusee- ('to descend on foot') [4]: 177
Reduplication
[edit]Reduplication is prefixal and is formed by taking the first consonant (if there is one) and the first vowel and then adding /:n/, where the colon indicates that the preceding vowel is elongated. The /n/ is deleted in the presence of a subsequent consonant.[4]: 197–198 For example, cebísee- ('to walk past') after reduplication becomes cée[n]cebísee- ('to walk back and forth past').[4]: 198
There are multiple usages of reduplication in Arapaho including pluralizing implied, secondary, and inanimate objects of (di)transitive verbs; indicating repeating and habitual action (extend the space and time a verb occurs in general), and intensifying. One example of marking repeating action is as follows [4]: 201
There can be multiple reduplications in compound words, where each reduplication can have an independent effect. Some verbs appear to be only in a reduplicated form; these verbs tend to describe repeating, iterative action.[4]: 203–204
Syntax
[edit]Arapaho has no canonical word order. Some sentences/clauses consist of only the verb like below [4]: 194
Single Noun Phrase
[edit]When a sentence contains a verb and a single noun phrase, the noun phrase can either precede or follow the verb. Preposing the noun phrase, however, gives it more importance and salience. Some instances where noun phrases are preposed include introducing a new referent (or reintroducing one that was inactive) and creating contrast.[4]: 400
V-NP order[4]: 331
i’i-bebiisitii-t
IC.INSTR(PREVERB)-fix(AI)-3S
kohuyohoen-i
glue-S
'He is fixing it with glue.'
NP-V order[4]: 401 (additional context: a new referent, "stone monuments," is being introduced)
3i’eyoon-o’
stone monument-PL
'There were just some stone monuments located there.' [O[25]:Woman Captive, 4]
Two Noun Phrases
[edit]For a basic sentence with a single verb that takes two noun phrases as arguments, all orderings are possible, but having the verb final is less common.[4]: 410
Noun Phrase Hierarchy
[edit]A hierarchy exists in determining which noun phrase goes in which position. In the listing below, the first in the pair is treated as "higher" in the hierarchy and tends therefore to be the leftmost NP.[4]: 411
- subject (object)
- proximate (obviative)
- actor (undergoer)
- marked object (unmarked object)
- animate (inanimate)
Subject-verb-object order[4]: 411
e’iiteihi3i
someone
eebeh-notinoohoot
POTENT-look for(TI)
niiteheibetiiton-i
help(AI.PART)-S
'Someone might be looking for help.' [O[25]:Scouts, 64]
Verb-subject-(implied) object order[4]: 412
nuhu’
this
o3-ii
arrow-PL
'Their arrows went right through the buffalos.' [O[25]:Scouts, 60]
Saliency
[edit]Saliency determines whether a noun phrase can precede its corresponding verb. Noun phrases are deemed salient if they are referring to something new, something that is being reintroduced, something contrastive, or something that is being emphasized.[4]: 403
Preposed NP[4]: 404 (here, the noun phrase meaning "where the attack was taking place" precedes the verb "see" to create emphasis)
eetoh-bisiitooni-’
where-attack.PART.IMPERS-0S
noonoohoot-o’u
see.REDUP(TI)-3PL
te’iyoonehe’-o’
child-PL
isein-o’
woman-PL
'Women and children watched where the attack was taking place.' [O[25]:Scouts, 70]
Both NPs preceding Verb (uncommon)[4]: 402 (additional context: occurs under "contrastive focus")
oo3oo’o’
some
tece’-ii
night-PL
nih-e’in-o’u
PAST-know(TI)-3PL
'Some knew the nighttime' [O[25]:Eagles, 93]
Syntax of Noun/Verb Phrases
[edit]Modifying Nouns
[edit]Generally, noun modifiers occur before the noun. These modifiers additionally tend to occur in a particular order relative each other. For example, in the example below, note that the presentative 'here is' occurs before the demonstrative 'this'[4]: 324
’oh
but
owoo
also
niiyou-no
here is-PL
nihii
well
nuhu’
this
nihii
well
yookox-ii
willow-PL
'And then there are these, well . . . these, well . . . willows.' [O[25]:Apache Captive, 7]
Particles and Verb Stems
[edit]Some particles are more closely linked to verbs; these particles generally precede the verb and are often neighboring the corresponding verb.[4]: 328
Particle expressing potential[4]: 329
nooxeihi’
maybe
esowobeihi-t
IC.be sick(AI)-3S
'Maybe he’s sick.'
Particle expressing recent past[4]: 329
Adverbials
[edit]Adverbials are a type of particle. Unlike other particles in Arapaho, however, they are not a closed class and are instead derived from or composed of other morphemes. One purpose of adverbial construction is to emphasize a morpheme by extracting it from a verb and having it stand alone. Another purpose is to convey meaning outside of what can normally be attached to a verb.[4]: 330–332
Adverbials are constructed by appending /iihi'/ (which can become /uuhu'/ after vowel harmony) to the end of the root.[4]: 333
A common usage of adverbials is to modify verbs. Adverbials can also act like prepositions and modify noun phrases; such adverbials can occur before or after the noun phrase and are thus exceptions to the rule that nominal modifiers prepose the noun (see example below).[4]: 334
sosoni’-ii
Shoshone-PL
neeyou
there it is
ohe’en-i’
mountain-LOC
niih-iihi’
along-ADV
nih-ii-3i’
PAST-say(AI)-3PL
'“We were chasing the Shoshones along the mountain there,” they said.' [O[25]:Shade Trees, 15]
(the Notes, References, and Further Readings sections below are from the original page. The only additions by me were for sources that I cite in my sections, such as my grammar)
Notes
[edit]- ^ "Detailed Languages Spoken at Home and Ability to Speak English". www.census.gov. US Census Bureau. Retrieved 2017-11-17.
- ^ Conathan 2006, 'A'.
- ^ "Arapaho". Ethnologue.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z aa ab ac ad ae af ag ah ai aj ak al am an ao ap aq ar as at au av aw ax ay az ba Cowell & Moss 2008b.
- ^ a b c Cowell & Moss 2008a, p. 1.
- ^ http://www.ethnologue.com/language/arp
- ^ http://www.ethnologue.com/language/arp
- ^ http://www.ethnologue.com/language/arp
- ^ Greymorning 2001, p. 287.
- ^ Frosch 2008.
- ^ Over, Ernie. "Northern Arapaho Language Camp wrapped up Wednesday at St. Stephens". County 10. Retrieved 2015-10-03.
- ^ http://www.ethnologue.com/language/arp
- ^ http://www.colorado.edu/UCB/AcademicAffairs/ArtsSciences/linguistics/csilw/newarapproj2.htm
- ^ Hale 2001, pp. 283–284.
- ^ Goddard 1974.
- ^ a b Goddard 1990, p. 103.
- ^ Goddard 1974, pp. 1974:104, 106, 107, 108.
- ^ Goddard 2001, p. 75.
- ^ a b c Cowell & Moss 2008a, p. 14.
- ^ Cowell & Moss 2008a, pp. 14–16.
- ^ Search the UPSID database for languages not having a low vowel
- ^ Salzman et al. 1998.
- ^ Cowell & Moss 2008a, pp. 22–23.
- ^ Cite error: The named reference
:0
was invoked but never defined (see the help page). - ^ a b c d e f g Cowell & Moss 2005.
References
[edit]- Campbell, Lyle (1997). American Indian Languages: The Historical Linguistics of Native America. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-514050-8.
- Conathan, Lisa (2006). "English-Arapaho dictionary (draft)". The Arapaho Language: Documentation and Revitalization Project. University of California at Berkeley. Retrieved 8 March 2012.
- Cowell, Andrew; Moss, Alonzo, Sr. (2008a). The Arapaho Language. University Press of Colorado. ISBN 978-0-87081-901-8.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Cowell, Andrew; Moss, Alonzo, Sr. (2008b). The Arapaho language. Moss, Alonzo, Sr. Boulder: University Press of Colorado. ISBN 9780870819940. OCLC 506069183.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Cowell, Andrew; Moss, Alonzo, Sr. (2005). Hinóno'éínoo3ítoono / Arapaho historical traditions. Winnipeg, Man.: University of Manitoba Press. ISBN 0887556833. OCLC 243614307.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Frosch, Dan (16 October 2008). "Its Native Tongue Facing Extinction, Arapaho Tribe Teaches the Young". New York Times.
- Goddard, Ives (1974). "An Outline of the Historical Phonology of Arapaho and Atsina". International Journal of American Linguistics. 40 (2): 102–16. doi:10.1086/465292.
- Goddard, Ives (1990). "Algonquian Linguistic Change and Reconstruction". In Philip Baldi (ed.). Linguistic Change and Reconstruction Methodology. Trends in Linguistics, Studies and Monographs 45. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 99–114.
- Goddard, Ives (2001). "The Algonquian Languages of the Plains". Handbook of North American Indians. Vol. 13. Washington: the Smithsonian Institution. pp. 71–79.
- Greymorning, Steven (2001). "Reflections on the Arapaho Language Project". In Ken Hale; Leanne Hinton (eds.). The Green Book of Language Revitalization in Practice. San Diego, California: Academic Press.
- Hale, Ken (2001). "The Arapaho Language". In Ken Hale; Leanne Hinton (eds.). The Green Book of Language Revitalization in Practice. San Diego, California: Academic Press.
- Kroeber, Alfred Louis (1917). Arapaho dialects. University of California Press. Retrieved 24 August 2012.
- Salzmann, Zdeněk and The Northern Arapaho Tribe and Anderson, Jeffrey. 1998. Dictionary of the Northern Arapaho Language.
Further reading
[edit]- Goddard, Ives. 1998. "Recovering Arapaho etymologies by reconstructing forwards". In Melchert, Craig & Jasanoff, Jay H. (eds.) Mír Curad: Studies in Honor of Calvert Watkins, Innsbruck: Institut für Sprachwissenschaftder Universität Innsbruck, Innsbrucker Beiträge zur Sprachwissenschaft, pp. 183–200.
- Jacques, Guillaume 2013. "The sound change s>n in Arapaho", Folia Linguistica Historica 34:43-57
- Pentland, David. 1997. [review of] Principles and Methods in Historical Phonology: From Proto-Algonkian to Arapaho, by Marc Picard, 1994. Diachronica 14.2: 383–386.
- Pentland, David. 1998. "Initial *s > n in Arapaho-Atsina". Diachronica 15.2:309–321.
- Picard, Marc. 1994. Principles and Methods in Historical Phonology: From Proto-Algonkian to Arapaho. Montreal and Kingston: McGill—Queen's University Press.
- Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The Languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
External links
[edit]- The Arapaho Language (U. of Colo. Arapaho Project)
- Arapaho Language Archives (U. of Colo. Arapaho Project), with many dialogues and narratives in Arapaho with glosses
- Nun-na-a-in-ah Ve-vith-ha Hin-nen-nau Hin-nen-it-dah-need (1895) Portions of the Book of Common Prayer in Arapaho
- OLAC resources in and about the Arapaho language
- Let's Learn Arapaho
- Constantine Scollen
- Arapaho Text Corpus