skip to main content
10.1145/2492494.2492516acmconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication PagessapConference Proceedingsconference-collections
research-article

Does neck viewing angle affect spatial orientation in an HMD-based VE?

Published: 22 August 2013 Publication History

Abstract

In this study, we examine subjects' spatial orientation in a head--mounted display (HMD) based virtual environment (VE) when objects are placed at different vertical viewing angles. More specifically, we examine spatial orientation when subjects memorize the location of objects on the ground, at eyelevel, and up in the air. Interestingly, we found that spatial orientation was the same across all conditions. This finding is important because it informs researchers who use similar spatial orientation tasks that variations in viewing neck angle to targets is not necessarily an issue.

References

[1]
Bian, Z., and Andersen, G. J. 2010. The advantage of a ground surface in the representation of visual scenes. J. of Vision 10, 8.
[2]
Bian, Z., Braunstein, M. L., and Andersen, G. J. 2006. The ground dominance effect in the perception of relative distance in 3-d scenes is mainly due to characteristics of the ground surface. Perc. & Psycho. 68, 8, 1297--1309.
[3]
Bowman, D. A., Gabbard, J. L., and Hix, D. 2002. A survey of usability evaluation in ves: Classification and comparison of methods. Presence 11, 4, 404--424.
[4]
Chance, S. S., Gaunet, F., Beall, A. C., and Loomis, J. M. 1998. Locomotion mode affects updating of objects encountered during travel: The contribution of vestibular and proprioceptive inputs to path integration. Presence 7, 2, 168--178.
[5]
Creem-Regehr, S. H., Willemsen, P., Gooch, A. A., and Thompson, W. B. 2005. The influence of restricted viewing conditions on egocentric distance perception: Implications for real and virtual environmentss. Perception 34, 2, 191--204.
[6]
Gibson, J. 1950. The perception of the visual world. Boston. Houghton-Mifflin.
[7]
Gogel, W. 1977. Stability and constancy in visual perception: Mechanisms and processes. Wiley, New York, ch. The metric of visual space, 129--182.
[8]
Imura, T., and Tomonaga, M. 2007. Visual search on the ground-like surface defined by texture gradients in chimpanzees and humans. J. of Vision 7, 9, 284--284.
[9]
Kavšek, M., and Granrud, C. E. 2013. The ground is dominant in infants perception of relative distance. Attn., Perc., & Psycho. 75, 2, 341--348.
[10]
Loomis, J. M., and Knapp, J. M. 2003. Virtual and Adaptive Environments. ErlBaum, Mahwah, NJ, ch. Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and VEs, 21--46.
[11]
Mark, L. 1987. Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances. a study of sitting and stair climbing. jhpp 13, 361--370.
[12]
Matin, L., and Li, W. 1995. Multimodal basis for egocentric spatial localization and orientation. J. of Vest. Res: equilibrium & orientation 5, 6, 499.
[13]
May, M. 1996. Thinking outside the body: An advantage for spatial updating during imagined versus physical self-rotation. Psychologische Beitrge 38, 418--434.
[14]
McCarley, J. S., and He, Z. J. 2000. Asymmetry in 3-d perceptual organization: Ground-like surface superior to ceiling-like surface. Perception & Psychophysics 62, 3, 540--549.
[15]
Meng, J. C., and Sedgwick, H. 2001. Distance perception mediated through nested contact relations among surfaces. Percep. Psycho. 63, 1, 1--15.
[16]
Ozkan, K., and Braunstein, M. L. 2006. Background surface and horizon effects in the perception of relative size. J. of Vision 6, 6, 421--421.
[17]
Ozkan, K., and Braunstein, M. L. 2009. Predominance of ground over ceiling surfaces in binocular rivalry. Attn., Perc., & Psycho. 71, 6, 1305--1312.
[18]
Péruch, P., Belingard, L., and Thinus-Blanc, C. 2000. Transfer of spatial knowledge from virtual to real environments. In Spatial Cognition II, C. Freska, Ed. Springer, Berlin.
[19]
Rock, I. 1975. An introduction to perception. MacMillan.
[20]
Thompson, W. B., Dilda, V., and Creem-Regehr, S. H. 2007. Absolute distance perception to locations off the ground plane. Perception 36, 11, 1559.
[21]
Waller, D., Hunt, E., and Knapp, D. 1998. The transfer of spatial knowledge in ve training. Presence 7, 2, 129--143.
[22]
Warren, W. H. 1984. Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. J. Exp. Psych: Hum. Perc. Perf. 10, 683--703.
[23]
Wartenberg, F., May, M., and Péruch, P. 1998. Spatial orientation in ves: Background considerations and experiments. In Spatial Cognition, An Interdisciplinary Approach to Representing and Processing Spatial Knowledge, Springer, London, 469--489.
[24]
Willemsen, P., and Gooch, A. A. 2002. Perceived egocentric distances in real, image-based and traditional ves. In IEEE Virtual Reality Conference, 79--86.
[25]
Williams, B., Narasimham, G., McNamara, T. P., Carr, T. H., journal Rieser, J., and Bodenheimer, B. 2006. Updating orientation in large ves using scaled translational gain. In Symp. on App. Perc. in Gr. and Vis., ACM, 21--28.
[26]
Williams, B. 2008. Design and Evaluation of Methods for Motor Exploration in Large Virtual Environments with Head-Mounted Display Technology. PhD thesis, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee, USA.
[27]
Witmer, B. G., and Sadowski, W. J. J. 1998. Nonvisually guided locomotion to a previously viewed target in real and virutal environments. Human Factors 40, 478--488.
[28]
Wraga, M. 1999. Using eye height in different postures to scale the heights of objects. J. Exp. Psych: Hum. Perc. Perf. 25, 518--530.

Cited By

View all

Index Terms

  1. Does neck viewing angle affect spatial orientation in an HMD-based VE?

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Information & Contributors

    Information

    Published In

    cover image ACM Conferences
    SAP '13: Proceedings of the ACM Symposium on Applied Perception
    August 2013
    150 pages
    ISBN:9781450322621
    DOI:10.1145/2492494
    Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

    Sponsors

    Publisher

    Association for Computing Machinery

    New York, NY, United States

    Publication History

    Published: 22 August 2013

    Permissions

    Request permissions for this article.

    Check for updates

    Author Tags

    1. spatial orientation
    2. virtual environments

    Qualifiers

    • Research-article

    Conference

    SAP' 13
    Sponsor:
    SAP' 13: ACM Symposium on Applied Perception 2013
    August 22 - 23, 2013
    Dublin, Ireland

    Acceptance Rates

    SAP '13 Paper Acceptance Rate 22 of 54 submissions, 41%;
    Overall Acceptance Rate 43 of 94 submissions, 46%

    Contributors

    Other Metrics

    Bibliometrics & Citations

    Bibliometrics

    Article Metrics

    • Downloads (Last 12 months)3
    • Downloads (Last 6 weeks)0
    Reflects downloads up to 09 Feb 2025

    Other Metrics

    Citations

    Cited By

    View all

    View Options

    Login options

    View options

    PDF

    View or Download as a PDF file.

    PDF

    eReader

    View online with eReader.

    eReader

    Figures

    Tables

    Media

    Share

    Share

    Share this Publication link

    Share on social media