7th Young Linguists' Meeting in Poznań
Book of Abstracts
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
7th Young Linguists' Meeting in Poznań
23-25th April 2021
organized by the Faculty of English,
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland
YLMP 2021 Book of Abstracts:
Issued on the occasion of the 7th Young Linguists' Meeting in Poznań
“Rethinking language and identity in the multilingual world”
Due to standardization processes, formatting of the abstracts contained herein was
changed in accordance with the YLMP abstract stylesheet. However, no alternations
with regard to language and contents were introduced.
1
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
YLMP 2021 Organizing Committee:
Honorary Organizer:
Katarzyna Dziubalska-Kołaczyk
Organizing Committee:
Joanna Pawelczyk
Katarzyna Jankowiak
Patryk Dobkiewicz
Tomasz Dyrmo
Contact details:
www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
ylmp(at)wa.amu.edu.pl
2
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
YLMP 2021 Best Presentation Award
sponsored by
Cambridge University Press
3
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
YLMP 2021 Advisory Board
Prof. Marc Brysbaert, Ghent University, Belgium
Prof. Katarzyna Bromberek-Dyzman, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Piotr Cegłowski, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Agnieszka Chmiel, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Alan Cienki, VU University Amsterdam, Netherlands
Prof. Anna Cieślicka, Texas A&M International University, USA
Dr. Kamila Dębowska-Kozłowska, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Krystyna Droździał-Szelest, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Katarzyna Dziubalska-Kołaczyk, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Anna Ewert, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Małgorzata Fabiszak, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Eva-Maria Graf, Alpen-Adria University, Klagenfurt, Austria
Prof. Walter van Heuven, University of Nottingham, UK
Prof. Agnieszka Kiełkiewicz-Janowiak, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Ronald Kim, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Dr. Paweł Korpal, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Marcin Krygier, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Małgorzata Kul, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Dr. Olha Lehka-Paul, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Dr. Iwona Łęska-Drajerczak, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Gary Massey, ZHAW Zurich University of Applied Sciences, Switzerland
Dr. Grzegorz Michalski, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Heiko Motschenbacher, Goethe-Universität, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
Dr. Katharina Oster, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, Germany
Prof. Joanna Pawelczyk, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Dr. Monika Połczyńska-Bletsos, University of California, Los Angeles, USA
Prof. Dennis Preston, Oklahoma State University, USA
Prof. Gro-Renée Rambø, University of Agder, Norway
Dr. Jacek Rysiewicz, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Dr. Stephanie Schnurr, Warwick University, UK
Prof. Paweł Stachura, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Dr. Jane Sunderland, Lancaster University, UK
Prof. Aleksandra Wach, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Bartosz Wiland, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Prof. Magdalena Wrembel, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Dr. Hanna Wysocka, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
Dr. Magdalena Zabielska, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
4
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Author index
Ambroziak, 22
Andernatt, 23
Baranowska, 24
Berthele, 17, 18
Bertin, 25
Bučková, 26
Buczak, 19
Buntinx, 28
Cal, 29
Calafato, 30
Carrere, 81
Cen, 31
Centner, 83
Cerqueglini, 32
Chevrot, 82
Chowdhury, 33
Cierpich, 54
Coady, 15
Cowell, 34
Cseh, 35
Czopek, 54
Del Vecchio, 36
Dunin-Borkowski, 37
Dyrmo, 19, 38
Dziubalska-Kołaczyk, 17
Earls, 39
Eldho, 40
Feindt, 41
Filipova, 42
Forsyth, 43
Gabadadze, 44
Galac, 45
Ghane, 46, 99
Ghiselli, 47
Gieshoff, 48
Gnitiev, 50
Grabiger, 51
Grabovskaya, 52
Gridneva, 52
Hartsuiker, 8
Herda, 53
Ivanenko, 52
Jankowiak, 8, 80
Jarynowski, 54, 55
Jelec, 105
Jelińska, 54, 55
Jose, 56
Juhász, 57
Jurczenia, 58
Jurewicz, 59
Kakalic, 60
Kakuba, 19
Kamiński, 61
Karácsonyi, 63
Kiełkiewicz-Janowiak,
19, 79
Korenar, 64
Korpal, 18, 80
Kotowicz, 65
Kubicha, 66
Kutylowska, 91
Ladinskaya, 52
Lara, 67
Laszloczki, 69
Lehka-Paul, 18
Lew, 13
Liu, 107
Lorenz, 70
Lujić, 71
Łuszczek, 72
Malanoski, 73
Mayr, 74
Meletis, 75
Morgan, 65
Nandi, 76
Naranowicz, 77
Nardy, 82
Nasef, 78
Nosal, 19, 79
Oster, 18, 80
5
Otwinowska, 91
Paradowski, 54, 55
Peake, 81
Peng, 105
Perrez, 9, 13
Peuzin, 82
Podboj, 71
Prawdzic, 83
Puthran, 84
R’boul, 85
Rogalski, 86
Rosenberg, 87
Rybova, 88
Sauntson, 11, 14
Senda, 89
Sikatskaya, 90
Silva, 91
Skałba, 92
Skóra, 93
Smith-Christmas, 14
Szabó, 94
Szyszka, 95
Tardel, 97
Tóth, 98
Vasheghani Farahani, 46,
99
Verthein, 80
Vodneva, 101
Walla, 102
Wockelmann, 103
Woll, 65
Wrembel, 17
Xu, 104
Yan, 105
Zachorska, 105
Zafar, 56
Zaychenko, 106
Zdorova, 52
Zhang, 107
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
PLENARY TALKS
6
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Syntactic blocking: When syntactic representations are not shared
across languages
Robert J. Hartsuiker
(Ghent University)
Cross-linguistic structural priming suggests that proficient bilinguals share syntactic
representations across their languages (e.g., Hartsuiker, Pickering, & Veltkamp, 2004). Such
priming can occur even when the syntactic structures are not fully identical in the two
languages. The shared representations must therefore abstract across such differences. What
then determines whether representations are or are not shared across languages? In this talk, I
will propose the syntactic blocking hypothesis, according to which a structure S in one language
and a counterpart S’ in another language can only have a shared representation if neither
language distinguishes between S and S’. Thus, priming can occur between a passive with SVO
structure in one language and one with SOV structure in another language, but not if either
language allows both word orders. In that case, the need to distinguish between different word
orders blocks the formation of an abstract representation for the passive that is independent of
word order. I will discuss structural priming studies using picture description, translation, and
artificial language learning tasks that are consistent with the syntactic preemption hypothesis. I
will conclude with suggestions to further test this hypothesis.
Rethinking the role of the native and non-native language in the
bilingual brain
Katarzyna Jankowiak
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
Much psycholinguistic research has recently been conducted to examine different aspects of
bilingual language processing, with a view to providing insights into how language information
is stored in the bilingual brain as well as what variables influence the automaticity of non-native
language (L2) processing. Less attention has, however, been devoted to showing how studying
bilingual populations can broaden our knowledge also on cognitive mechanisms that govern
native language (L1) processing. In this talk, I will report on behavioral (reaction times) and
psychophysiological (skin conductance and EEG) studies that were aimed to examine different
aspects of language processing in L1 and L2, including figurative and emotionally-laden
language comprehension. I will discuss how testing bilingual populations on different aspects
of language comprehension can provide us with novel and valuable information regarding the
nature of cognitive mechanisms engaged in meaning processing in both L1 and L2.
7
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Metaphor studies as a bridge between linguistics and political
science: Theoretical implications and methodological challenges
Julien Perrez
(University of Liège)
Metaphor is a central component of human cognition and communication. While metaphors
can be studied for their own sake from a linguistic perspective as a process of meaning extension
at various levels (be it morphological, lexical or syntactical) or as a rhetorical device used in
argumentation, metaphor studies have broadened their scope and touched upon many other
scientific disciplines among the cognitive and social sciences. As a result, metaphors have
become a central topic in many disciplines, including linguistics, philosophy, psychology,
psycholinguistics, neurolinguistics, communication studies, political science, education science
or translation studies.
Among these disciplines, politics remains one prominent area where to find metaphors,
as has been highlighted by many scholars (see among others Carver & Pikalo, 2008; CharterisBlack, 2011, 2013; Lakoff, 2002; Musolff, 2004, 2016). This can be explained by two main
factors. On the one hand, most of our political concepts are metaphorical in nature (Lakoff,
2002, 2004). This means that our understanding of complex and abstract political concepts and
processes relies on conceptual metaphors that ‘provide ways of simplifying complexities and
making abstractions accessible’ (Semino, 2008:90). On the other hand, metaphors are central
to the domain of politics because they have the potential to frame the debate (Lakoff, 2004) and
indirectly convey hidden ideologies (Goatly, 2007). Using a particular metaphor to depict a
given reality will activate a particular frame and highlight some aspects of this reality while
hiding others. Through framing, ‘metaphor helps construct particular aspects of reality and
reproduce (or subvert) dominant schemas’ (Koller, 2009: 121). This framing function of
metaphors suggests that they influence or even determine the representations of a given reality
in the receiver’s mind. This framing function of metaphors can be associated with their
persuasive role in rhetoric (see Charteris-Black, 2011).
In this talk I will report on the results of two sets of studies we have been conducting as
part of an interdisciplinary project on the evolution of the discourse about Belgian federalism.
The first set of studies (Perrez & Reuchamps, 2015b; Reuchamps et al., 2018) tackle the framing
effect of metaphors and aim at understanding under which conditions metaphors might
influence citizens’ representations and preferences about Belgian federalism. Among other
things, I will highlight the methodological issues related to these types of behavioral
experiments and discuss the mediating role of political knowledge on the framing effect of
metaphors.
The second set of studies propose semiotic analyses of metaphor use in different types
of political corpora, including citizen discourse, institutional discourse and media discourse
about Belgian federalism (see for instance Heyvaert, 2019; Perrez & Reuchamps, 2015a). In
this part of the presentation, I will question the notion of political discourse itself (see Randour
et al., 2020) and focus on the notion of variation in political metaphor use, e.g. understanding
why particular metaphors emerge in particular political contexts (or which type of metaphors
are produced by which of type political actors?) and how they evolve in one or several discourse
communities. Building on the results of these studies, I will also argue that Deliberate Metaphor
Theory (Steen, 2008, 2017) provides an appropriate theoretical framework to account for the
rhetorical potential of political metaphors.
8
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
By focusing on these two interrelated dimensions of metaphor use in political discourse,
I will try and highlight the cross-fertilization potential of such interdisciplinary research, by
showing what metaphor studies can gain, as well theoretically as methodologically, from
privileged contact with political science, in the hope that this can be a source of inspiration for
any kind of interdisciplinary enterprise.
References:
Carver, T., & Pikalo, J. (2008). Political Language and Metaphor: Interpreting and changing
the world (1st ed.). Routledge.
Charteris-Black, J. (2011). Politicians and Rhetoric. Palgrave Macmillan UK.
Charteris-Black, J. (2013). Analysing political speeches: rhetoric, discourse and metaphor.
Palgrave Macmillan.
Goatly, A. (2007). Washing the Brain – Metaphor and Hidden Ideology (Vol. 23). John
Benjamins Publishing Company.
Heyvaert, P. (2019). Chapter 2. Fairies, Christmas miracles and sham marriages: A diachronic
analysis of deliberate metaphors in Belgian political discourse. In J. Perrez, M. Reuchamps,
& P. H. Thibodeau (Eds.), Discourse Approaches to Politics, Society and Culture (Vol. 85,
pp. 35–59). John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Koller, V. (2009). Missions and Empires: Religious and Political Metaphors in Corporate
Discourse. In A. Musolff & J. Zinken (Eds.), Metaphor and Discourse (pp. 116–134).
Palgrave Macmillan UK.
Lakoff, G. (2002). Moral politics: how liberals and conservatives think (2nd ed). University of
Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G. (2004). Don’t think of an elephant! know your values and frame the debate: the
essential guide for progressives. Chelsea Green Pub. Co.
Musolff, A. (2004). Metaphor and Political Discourse. Palgrave Macmillan UK.
Musolff, A. (2016). Political metaphor analysis: discourse and scenarios. Bloomsbury
Academic, an imprint of Bloomsbury Publishing Plc.
Perrez, J., & Reuchamps, M. (2015a). A crazy machine or a strong ‘living apart together’
relationship? Mots. Les Langages Du Politique, 109 (3), 126–126.
Perrez, J., & Reuchamps, M. (2015b). The ‘Belgian Tetris’: Assessing the political impact of
metaphors on citizens’ representations of Belgian federalism. Metaphor and the Social
World, 5 (2), 223–244.
Randour, F., Perrez, J., & Reuchamps, M. (2020). Twenty years of research on political
discourse: A systematic review and directions for future research: Discourse & Society.
Reuchamps, M., Dodeigne, J., & Perrez, J. (2018). Changing your political mind: The impact
of a metaphor on citizens’ representations and preferences for federalism. Regional &
Federal Studies, 28 (2), 151–175.
Semino, E. (2008). Metaphor in discourse. Cambridge University Press.
Steen, G. (2008). The Paradox of Metaphor: Why We Need a Three-Dimensional Model of
Metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 23 (4), 213–241.
Steen, G. (2017). Deliberate Metaphor Theory: Basic assumptions, main tenets, urgent issues.
Intercultural Pragmatics, 14 (1), 1–24.
9
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Rethinking gender and sexuality in the classroom: A corpus-based
discourse analysis of classroom interaction
Helen Sauntson
(York St John University)
This presentation explores language-based sexism and homophobia in educational
settings. Schools have been identified as places where girls, gender variant and LGBT+identifying young people report routinely experiencing discrimination through discursive
practices in schools. This talk explores these issues by drawing on a recent research project
which conducts a detailed and systematic examination of the diverse ways that language can
play a role in constructions of gender and sexual identities in British school contexts.
Using corpus-based discourse analysis (Baker, 2006; Baker et al, 2013), I analyse data
comprising spoken interactional data taken from a series of Relationships and Sex Education
(RSE) lessons in two British secondary schools. The corpus methods used in the first part of
the analysis comprise keywords and concordance analyses which are used to broadly identify
what kinds of gender and sexual identities are constructed in the lessons. Critical discourse
analysis of the interaction is then used to explore how those identities are constructed
interactionally. The critical discourse analysis element draws on Pakula et al’s (2015) and
Sauntson’s (2019) notions of ‘gender and sexuality triggered points’ in classroom discourse.
Overall, the analysis focuses on how language in the data works as a form of social practice
which can include and exclude certain gender and sexual identities in classroom settings. The
presentation concludes by considering the implications of the analysis with a specific focus on
how to make visible gender and sexual diversity issues in schools and other educational settings
and how to support the needs of learners of diverse sexual identities.
References:
Baker, P. 2006. Using Corpora in Discourse Analysis. London: Continuum.
Baker, P., Gabrielatos, C. and McEnery, T. 2013. Discourse Analysis and Media Attitudes: The
Representation of Islam in the British Press. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Pakula, L., Pawelczyk, J. and Sunderland, J. 2015. Gender and Sexuality in English Language
Education: Focus on Poland. London: British Council.
Sauntson, H. 2019. Language-based discrimination in schools: Intersections of gender and
sexuality. In C. R. Caldas-Coulthard (ed) Innovations and Challenges: Women, Language
and Sexism. London: Routledge.
10
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
WORKSHOPS
11
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Gentle introduction to R for language analysis
Robert Lew
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
R is an open-source, community-driven, and cost-free environment for statistical computing. In
many areas of academic research, it has recently become (or is becoming) a de facto standard
for data analysis and visualization. It is also making steady inroads into the realm of
linguistic research. However, young linguists aspiring to try it out often find it daunting and get
discouraged.
This workshop will offer a gentle, non-threatening introduction to R for language
analysis. I plan to work with you through some of the many possible applications for analysis
and visualization. Upon completion, you should be able to follow up on your own using the
many available learning resources.
Introduction to Corpus Linguistics: Theoretical and methodological
basics
Julien Perrez
(University of Liège)
Since the 1980’s and the rise of computer-assisted technologies, Corpus Linguistics (CL) has
become a mainstream methodology in linguistics, making it possible to analyze ‘very extensive
collections of transcribed utterances or written texts’ (McEnery & Hardie, 2012: i). This
workshop will be devoted to main theoretical and methodological basics of Corpus Linguistics.
It will be composed of three main parts. Firstly, we will address the process of corpus
construction, with a focus on data collection, balance and representativeness. Secondly, we will
discuss essential notions of CL, such as tokens, types, concordances, collocations, corpus
annotation and the distinction between corpus-based and corpus-driven approaches. Finally, we
will present various types of specialized corpora (for example, monolingual and bilingual
corpora, learner corpora and political corpora) to give an overview of the research questions
that can be addressed thanks to Corpus Linguistics in a variety of disciplines.
This workshop will also include a hands-on session during which the participants will
have the opportunity to apply the notions that have been discussed to their own corpus, using
the free corpus processing softwares AntConc (Anthony, 2019) and Unitex (Paumier, 2020).
References:
Anthony, L. (2019). AntConc (Version 3.5.8) [Computer Software].
McEnery, T., & Hardie, A. (2012). Corpus Linguistics: Method, Theory and Practice.
Cambridge University Press.
Paumier, S. (2020). Unitex 3.2 User Manual.
12
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Language and sexuality in schools: A practice-oriented workshop
Helen Sauntson
(York St John University)
Much current research in the field of language, sexuality and education finds that school-based
language practices are overwhelmingly experienced as sites which foster and reproduce genderand sexuality-based discrimination. However, research also reveals that there is scope for
challenging and changing these practices. This workshop draws on this recent and current
research to explore some of the practical ways in which gender and sexual diversity and
inequalities may be challenged through language and discourse in schools. Topics explored
through the workshop may include (but are not restricted to): specialized training for teachers;
the production of handbooks and briefing documents for school inspectorates and governing
bodies; the role of teacher-student-parent dialogue; language ‘audits’ in schools. The workshop
is designed to be participant-led and dialogic with participants sharing experiences, ideas and
suggestions in a supportive and inclusive environment.
The negotiated nature of language and identity
Cassandra Smith-Christmas
(National University of Ireland, Galway)
It is well-established within sociolinguistics that the relationship between language and identity
is complex, multi-layered, and continuously negotiated (e.g. LePage and Tabouret-Keller;
Omoniyi, 2004). This workshop will seek to build on these concepts and will guide workshop
participants through exploring identity negotiations in everyday interactions. It will use
recorded conversations of multilingual families as its main lens to do so and will examine the
multi-scalar way in which family members use language to index particular identities, from the
macro-level (e.g. language as a marker of national identity) to the more micro-level (e.g.
enacting the family roles, such as ‘parent’ or ‘child’; multilingualism as a component of family
identity-making processes). Workshop participants are invited (but not required) to bring their
own data to this workshop to explore how ‘acts of identity’ (LePage and Tabouret-Keller, 1985)
may take various forms over space and time. Time will be allotted for small and large group
discussions of some of the data that participants may wish to share.
References:
LePage, R. and Tabouret-Keller, A. (1985). Acts of Identity: Creole-Based Approaches to
Language and Ethnicity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Omoniyi, T. (2004). The Sociolinguistics of Borderlands: Two Nations, One Community.
Trenton, New Jersey: Africa World Press.
13
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
YLMP Special Event
Why place matters: In- and exclusion of language minoritized
students during COVID-19
Maria Coady
(University of Florida)
The global pandemic has affected students, families, and educators worldwide but in
disproportionate ways. Minoritized students and families of color have faced tremendous
barriers to healthcare and technology. Rural communities and schools, for example, have
struggled to provide access to learning technologies and broadband communication with little
lead time and insufficient resources. Even more illuminating are the limitations of this placed
on students and families in the United States whose first language is not English (language
minorized students, often referred to as emergent bilingual). In other words, “place” matters in
the education of students, and this issue has become illuminated during the current pandemic.
This presentation provides an overview of language minoritized students in the U.S. and
how places (urban, suburban, small town, and rural) have been affected by the current
pandemic. I then describe challenges that educators face working with rural, language
minoritized students and how these have been exacerbated in the past year. I offer examples of
what rural educators are doing to address exclusion of language minoritized students. I end the
discussion with suggestions for educators, scholars, and policymakers to consider when looking
forward at equity issues in educational policies for language minoritized students and families.
14
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
INVITED
THEMATIC
SESSIONS
15
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Multilingualism Matters
Organized by: Bilingualism Matters @Poznań
Conveners: Katarzyna Dziubalska-Kołaczyk and Magdalena Wrembel (Adam Mickiewicz
University, Poznań)
Special guest: Raphael Berthele (University of Fribourg)
This session aims to exchange ideas and expertise on how bi- and multilingualism have changed
and developed linguistic research. Empirical language studies are currently adapting to the fact
that nowadays the majority of the world’s population is bi- or multilingual by introducing a
number of new methodological standards, including a more thorough screening of language
background of participants tested or more rigorous criteria for selecting appropriate language
stimuli to be used in a study. Furthermore, bi-/multilingualism has also significantly contributed
to applied linguistics, introducing novel research avenues, such as cross-linguistic influence,
multicompetence, or code-switching investigated across different linguistic domains.
The session will approach bi-/multilingualism from two correlated perspectives. Firstly,
it will focus on methodological issues related to the study of bi-/multilingual populations.
Secondly, it will discuss the contributions of multilingual research to applied linguistics. In this
two-part session, we welcome presentations devoted to addressing methodological challenges
related to the study of bi-/multilingual populations, as well as to exploring the bi-/multilingual
perspective while researching foreign language learning.
16
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Language learning aptitude and social differences in multilingual
language learning
Raphael Berthele
(University of Fribourg)
In this talk I discuss evidence on two related issues in current multilingualism research: On the
one hand, scholars observe considerable interindividual differences in aptitude for language
learning, and on the other hand, there are well-known social determinants of learning
achievement in particular in the school context.
First, I focus on cognitive, motivational and other causes for and correlates of the
observable variability in learning additional (foreign) languages. Focussing on data from Swiss
elementary school learners of French and English as additional languages, I show evidence for
a general cognitive factor as the main predictor for skill learning in the target language.
Furthermore, additional variance is explained by certain motivational and affective constructs.
In the second part of my talk, I present evidence on an inquiry into the social correlates
(e.g. linguistic, economic, educational family background) of these dimensions. The analyses
are an attempt to account for the potential complexity in the interplay of various factors while
still producing interpretable results. In the discussion I provide my current state of ignorance
on adequate and feasible ways of investigating determinants that shape additional language skill
learning.
The psychology of translation and interpreting
Conveners: Katharina Oster (TRA&CO-Center), Paweł Korpal (Adam Mickiewicz
University, Poznań), Olha Lehka-Paul (Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
Recent decades have shown a growing interest in cognitive and psycho-affective aspects of
translation and interpreting, also among young translation scholars. Among the topics discussed
in Translation and Interpreting Studies were: language processing, working memory in
translation and interpreting, control mechanisms, uncertainty management, cognitive effort,
mental representations, personality, stress experienced by interpreters, emotional intelligence,
and ergonomics. To empirically test various assumptions related to the complexity of the
translator’s and interpreter’s work, several research methods were applied, which include, but
are not limited to, Think Aloud Protocols (TAPs), reaction times, eye-tracking, key-logging,
EEG, fMRI, and skin conductance.
In this panel we are going to explore the interplay between psychology and translation
as a form of language mediation. The aim of our session is to contribute to the discussion of
various perspectives on the psychology of translation and interpreting, as well as to share and
discuss relevant research in Translation and Interpreting Studies. The session will be convened
by Dr. Katharina Oster (TRA&CO-Center), Dr. Paweł Korpal (Faculty of English, Adam
Mickiewicz University, Poznań), and Dr. Olha Lehka-Paul (Faculty of English, Adam
Mickiewicz University, Poznań). We welcome submissions which report on empirical research;
speakers should be able to present at least preliminary findings of their projects.
17
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Gender(ed) identities: Diversity and attitudes
Conveners: Tomasz Dyrmo, Jagoda Nosal, Patrycja Kakuba, Mikołaj Buczak, Agnieszka
Kiełkiewicz-Janowiak (Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
For a very long time language has been regarded as a medium that unites people, that brings
them together, but, undeniably, divides them too (e.g. Wee 2005, Sczesny et al. 2015). This
dichotomy comes with a premise that language may be a tool for empowering those whose
voices have been silenced (Cameron et al. 1993). In this context, linguistics, in particular, seems
suitably equipped to give a platform to marginalised groups, whose identities unquestionably
hinge on their power to speak for themselves and the communities they represent.
Diversity in gender(ed) identities as well as ingroup and outgroup attitudes are the point
of interest for many researchers in linguistics and beyond. Recent studies have focused on the
notions of visibility and empowerment (Cameron 2005, Zimman 2017), which is a drastic
departure from the previous locus of scholarly attention, that is difference and discrimination
(Crawford 1995, Pauwels 2003). What is more, current analyses favour a more open and
inclusive approach to language and identity, which is exemplified by a growing body of
literature on non-binary identities (Zimman et al. 2014, Konnelly and Cowper 2020).
Clearly, gender and sexual identity are related notions. This relationship is visible, for
instance, in how the LGBT+ community expresses their identity (Bucholtz and Hall 2004), how
they inform of their varied understanding of sexuality (Zimman 2009), or how they
communicate their sexual orientation and gender identity multimodally (Lederer 2019). Given
the complexity of the issue in question, we aim to highlight the need for understanding and
acceptance in the ever-changing landscape of gender identities.
References:
Bucholtz, M., & Hall, K. (2004). Theorizing identity in language and sexuality research.
Language in Society, 33(04).
Cameron, D., Frazer, E., Harvey, P., Rampton, B., & Richardson, K. (1993). Ethics, advocacy
and empowerment: Issues of method in researching language. Language &
Communication, 13(2), 81–94. Cameron, D. (2005). Language, gender, and sexuality:
Current issues and new directions. Applied Linguistics, 26(4), 482–502.
Crawford, M. (1995). Talking difference: On gender and language. SAGE.
Konnelly, L., & Cowper, E. (2020). Gender diversity and morphosyntax: An account of singular
they. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 5(1), 40.
Leap, W. (2013). Queer linguistics, international perspectives and the Lavender Language
Conference: Rethinking alterity. In Queering Paradigms III: Queer Impact and Practices.
Kathleen O’Meara and Liz Morrish, eds., 179-200. Berlin: Peter Lang.
Lederer, J. (2019). Gesturing the source domain: The role of co-speech gesture in the
metaphorical models of gender transition. Metaphor and the Social World, 9(1).
Motschenbacher, H. (2011). Taking Queer Linguistics further: Sociolinguistics and critical
heteronormativity research. International Journal of the Sociology of Language,
2011(212).
Pauwels, A. (2003). Linguistic Sexism and Feminist Linguistic Activism. In: Holmes, J., &
Meyerhoff, M. (Eds.), The Handbook of Language and Gender. pp. 550-570.
18
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Sczesny, S., Moser, F., & Wood, W. (2015). Beyond sexist beliefs: How do people decide to
use gender-inclusive language? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(7), 943–
954.
Wee, L. (2005). Intra-language discrimination and linguistic human rights: The case of singlish.
Applied Linguistics, 26(1), 48–69.
Zimman, L. (2009). ‘The other kind of coming out’: Transgender people and the coming out
narrative genre. Gender and Language, 3(1), 53–80.
Zimman, L., Davis, J. L., & Raclaw, J. (Eds.). (2014). Queer excursions: Retheorizing binaries
in language, gender, and sexuality. Oxford University Press.
Zimman, L. (2017). Transgender language reform. Journal of Language and Discrimination, 1
(1), 84–105.
19
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
ORAL AND POSTER
PRESENTATIONS
20
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
L2 Motivational Self System in a Polish context, and its connection
to L2 Willingness To Communicate (WTC)
Agata Ambroziak
(University of Warsaw)
Individual learner differences, such as motivation or willingness to communicate (WTC ), are
considered strong predictors of second language (L2) achievement. According to the theoretical
framework of L2 motivation developed by Dörnyei (2009), the L2 Motivational Self System
comprises three main components: Ideal L2 Self, Ought-to L2 Self, and L2 Learning Experience.
It was proposed that Ideal L2 Self was closely linked to integrativeness, and Ought-to L2 Self
corresponded to instrumental motive. However, the interplay of motivational variables and their
links to WTC still need deeper exploring. The present correlational study aimed to establish the
relationships between L2 motivational dimensions and L2 WTC. Moreover, it aimed at
validating the theoretical framework of L2 Motivational Self System proposed by Dörnyei
(2009).
The data were collected via a self-report motivation questionnaire (Taguchi, Magid, &
Papi, 2009) and WTC questionnaire (MacIntyre et al., 2001 ). The ultimate sample of this study
consists of c.a. 100 teenage native speakers of Polish who learn English as their L2, but here,
the interim results are presented (N = 47). As for the interplay between motivational variables
and L2 WTC, Ideal L2 self predicted 14% of variance in L2 WTC. This suggests that promoting
the development of strong Ideal L2 self among learners may increase their level of WTC, which
is in line with Munezane (2013). Also, high level of language anxiety predicted a decreased
level of willingness to speak, both inside and outside classroom environment. Focusing solely
on motivational variables, the results indicate that although integrativeness predicts the
intended effort to learn English, explaining 14% of its variance, it does not correlate with the
Ideal L2-Self (p > .05), which stands in contrast to some previous studies (e.g., Taguchi et al.,
2009). Although such results may speak against the idea of equating integrativeness with Ideal
L2 Self in Dörnyei’s (2009) model, one may argue that the lack of that relationship may stem
from the low reliability of integrativeness factor in the motivation questionnaire developed by
Taguchi et al. (2009), as based on data in a current study (α = .44). I will discuss the issues
related to the items measuring integrativeness (Dörnyei & Csizér, 2002) used in the current
questionnaire (Taguchi, et al. 2009) in the light of Polish socio-educational context and the use
of English as a global language.
References:
Dörnyei, Z., & Csizer, K. (2002). Motivational Dynamics in Second Language Acquisition:
Results of a Longitudinal Nationwide Survey. Applied Linguistics, 23, 421-462.
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The L2 Motivational Self System. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda
(Eds.), Motivation, language identity and the L2 self (pp. 9-42). Bristol: Multilingual
Matters.
Macintyre, P. D., Baker, S., Clément, R. & Conrod, S. (2001). Willingness to
Communicate,Social Support, and Language-Learning Orientations of Immersion
Students. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23(3), 369-388.
Munezane, Y. (2013) Attitudes, affect and ideal L2 self as predictors of willingness to
communicate. In R. Leah, A. Ewert, M. Pawlak and M. Wrembel (Eds.) EUROSLA
Yearbook: Volume 13 (pp. 176-198). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
21
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Taguchi, T., Magid, M., & Papi, M. (2009). The L2 Motivational Self System among Japanese,
Chinese and Iranian Learners of English: A Comparative Study. In Z. Dornyei, & E.
Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, Language Identity and the L2 Self (pp. 66-97). Bristol: Multilingual
Matters.
A relevance-theoretic approach to interpreting ELF input
Katrin Andermatt
(ZHAW Zurich University of Applied Sciences)
Although the use of English as a lingua franca (ELF) has become a new professional reality for
translators and interpreters, its implications for translating and interpreting have only recently
started to be investigated scientifically (Albl-Mikasa, 2018). This PhD project focuses on the
impact of (the often hybrid nature of) ELF on simultaneous interpreters' processing of source
text from a pragmatics perspective. Findings from Reithofer's (2013) comprehension-testing
study, namely that an ELF speech was understood less well by the audience than its
simultaneous interpretation into the audience's first language, suggest that interpreters manage
to produce coherent target texts from less coherent source text input, presumably levelling out
ELF-induced problems. To date, no research has been done on how exactly this is achieved.
Drawing on the pragmatic framework of Relevance Theory (Sperber & Wilson, 1995),
transcriptions of professional interpreters' renditions of an ELF source text are subjected to a
qualitative product analysis and compared with renditions of an edited English version of the
same source text to assess the cognitive effects generated by the interpreters. More specifically,
the explicatures identified in the interpretations are compared with those in the source texts with
regard to the enrichment processes interpreters may have engaged in. According to Relevance
Theory, these include "disambiguation, reference assignment, the resolution of vagueness, and
the recovery of ellipsed or unexpressed material" (Blakemore, 1987, p. 72). As the relation
between cognitive effects and processing effort has been shown to be crucial for the description
of the translation process as a cognitive activity (Alves, 2007; Alves & Gonçalves, 2015, 2003),
the product analysis is triangulated with process data to gauge the cognitive effort involved in
the interpreting processes.
This contribution will present an overview of the study design as well as preliminary
results of a pilot study conducted as part of the PhD project on two interpretations of the ELF
source text and two interpretations of the edited English source text, and report on possible
differences in the interpretations of ELF as opposed to edited English input.
References:
Albl-Mikasa, M. (2018). ELF and translation/interpreting. In J. Jenkins, W. Baker, & M. Dewey
(Eds.), The Routledge Handbook of English as a Lingua Franca (pp. 369–383).
Routledge.
Alves, F. (2007). Cognitive effort and contextual effect in translation: A relevance-theoretic
approach. Journal of Translation Studies, 10(1), 57–76.
Alves, F., & Gonçalves, J. L. (2015). Investigating the conceptual-procedural distinction in the
translation process. A relevance-theoretic analysis of micro and macro translation units.
In M. Ehrensberger-Dow, S. Göpferich, & S. O’Brien (Eds.), Interdisciplinarity in
translation and interpreting process research (Vol. 1, pp. 109–126). John Benjamins.
22
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Alves, F., & Gonçalves, J. L. V. R. (2003). A relevance theory approach to the investigation of
inferential processes in translation. In F. Alves (Ed.), Triangulating translation:
Perspectives in process oriented research (Vol. 45, pp. 3–24). John Benjamins.
Blakemore, D. (1987). Semantic constraints on relevance. Blackwell.
Reithofer, K. (2013). Comparing modes of communication: The effect of English as a lingua
franca vs. Interpreting. Interpreting, 15(1), 48–73.
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition (2nd ed).
Blackwell Publishers.
Modality and its impact on comprehension, foreign language
vocabulary learning, and cognitive load
Karolina Baranowska
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
In the light of Dual Coding Theory (Paivio 1971) and Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning
(Mayer 2009), the integration of audio and video is more effective for information processing,
comprehension, and language acquisition than a single-modality presentation. This is evidenced
by a bulk of experimental research (e.g. Whiting and Granoff 2010, Safarali and Hamidi 2012,
Kivrak and Gökmen 2019). However, there are also studies which challenge the effectiveness
of audiovisual input and demonstrate that audio-only input is more beneficial for learners (e.g.
Pusey and Lenz 2014 , Hsu 2017).
Because the results of previous research on the effectiveness of dual modality input are
inconclusive, the current study seeks to find whether foreign language films, which are
annotated with both verbal and visual input, facilitate or hinder comprehension, vocabulary
learning, and whether they lead to cognitive overload. Using a between-subject design, 65
upper-intermediate Polish learners of English were divided into three experimental groups and
viewed a movie clip in English in the audiovisual, audio-only, or video-only version. During
the viewing session, the level of cognitive load was measured by means of reaction time to
sound stimuli occurring throughout the movie. Immediately after the viewing session, the
participants completed a post-test on comprehension, and a post test on vocabulary acquisition,
which was a vocabulary knowledge scale test adapted from Zhao and Macaro (2016).
Moreover, as a subjective measurement of cognitive load, a self-report on the cognitive load
experienced was administered. The questionnaire was adapted from NASA Task Load Index
(Hart 1986) and Kruger et al. (2014), and it measured intrinsic, extraneous, and germane
cognitive load separately. A month after the experiment, the participants completed a delayed
post-test on vocabulary, which consisted of two parts: production (the same as the immediate
post-test), and recognition (multiple choice). The results challenge the dual channel assumption
in that no significant differences were found between the audiovisual and the audio-only groups
in terms of comprehension, vocabulary learning, and cognitive load.
References:
Hart, S.G. (1986). NASA Task Load Index. NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett Field, CA
United States.
Hsu, W. (2017). ‘The modality effect of input repetition on vocabulary consolidation’, Konin
Language Studies 5(4): 403-424.
23
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Kıvrak, C.& Gökmen, D. U. (2019). ‘The effects of task-induced involvement load and input
modality on incidental vocabulary learning’, Journal of Language and Linguistic
Studies 15(4): 1355-1375.
Kruger, J. L., Hefer, E.,& Matthew, G. (2014). ‘Attention distribution and cognitive load in a
subtitled academic lecture: L1 vs. L2’, Journal of Eye Movement Research 7(5)4: 1-15.
Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia learning (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Pusey, K., & Lenz, K. (2014). ‘Investigating the Interaction of Visual Input, Working Memory,
and Listening Comprehension’, Language Education in Asia, 5(1): 66-80.
Safarali, S. K., & Hamidi, H. (2012). ‘The Impact of Videos Presenting Speakers’ Gestures and
Facial Clues on Iranian EFL Learners’ Listening Comprehension’, International
Journal of Applied Linguistics & English Literature 1(6): 106-114.
Whiting, J., & Granoff, S. (2010). ‘The Effects of Multimedia Input on Comprehension of a
Short Story’, The Electronic Journal for English as a Second Language 14(2):1-10.
Zhao, T., & Macaro, E. (2016). ‘What works better for the learning of concrete and abstract
words: teachers’ L1 use or L2-only explanations?’, International Journal of Applied
Linguistics 26(1): 75-98.
Extreme hybrids in Norwegian: natural gender representation
through NP-internal semantic agreement
Alice Bertin
(University of Milan)
Norwegian is a gendered language. This means that grammatical gender is a fundamental
feature of every noun and that it must consequently be expressed through agreement by any
element which is sensible to it. Grammatical gender is primarily assigned through core semantic
assignment rules, which consider in the first place the natural gender of the referent. Yet, they
are not equally representative toward women and men, and women are generally less
represented in the grammatical gender of Norwegian nouns. A new tendence is, however, on
the rise and extreme hybrid are now attested in the language and accepted by Norwegian
speakers, even though, it has been found, to a limited extent and to different degrees depending
on the gender of the speaker. These hybrid nouns are special in that they can express NP-internal
semantic agreement with the indefinite article, which can display feminine gender despite the
masculine grammatical gender of the controller noun. This is a complete innovation, as NPinternal agreement is of the most rare and restricted type. Furthermore, they can be inscribed in
a broader tendence which takes into account the semantic core of animacy and natural gender
and expresses it through new agreement patterns. A representative instance of these new
agreement patterns is the introduction of the anaphoric pronoun den, which caused a more
definite partition of nouns into different anaphoric pronouns according to their syntactic and,
especially, semantic properties, as animacy and referential gender are. This innovation led the
way to the re-semantization and reinforcement of grammatical gender, of which extreme
hybrids are an important representative, while it can also be further interpreted as a renovation,
as previously important features of the gender system are newly brought forward through new
agreement patterns. A more inclusive language, which gives equal space to the representation
of both women and men is thus now possible and allowed even by strict linguistic principles.
24
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Aikhenvald, A. (2016). How gender shapes the world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Corbett, G.G. (1991). Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Enger, H.O. (2004). Tre endringer i det skandinaviske genussystemet i lys av
grammatikaliseringsteori. Arkiv för nordisk filologi, (119), 125-146. Eslöv: Trycktjänst
i Eslöv.
Enger, H.O. (2009). The role of core and non-core semantic rules in gender assignment. Lingua,
(119), 1281-1299.
Enger, H.O. (2015). When friends and teachers become hybrids. In Fleischer, J., Rieken, E. &
Widmer, P. (eds) Agreement from a Diachronic Perspective. Berlin: Mouton De
Gruyter, pp 215-35.
Enger, H.O., Haugen, T.A. (2017). Styrking og svekking av genussemantikk: Ei diakron
drøfting av (in)animathet ved pannekakesetninger og anaforisk den. Maal og Minne (2),
43-72. http://urn.nb.no/URN:NBN:no-63949
Lepschy, A.L., Lepschy, G. and Sanson, H. (2001). Lingua italiana e femminile. Quaderns
d’Italià (6), 9-18.
Trosterud, T. (2001). Genustilordning i norsk er regelstyrt. Norsk Lingvistisk Tidsskrift, (19),
29-59.
Zubin, D., Köpcke, K. (1986). Gender and Folk Taxonomy. Typological studies in language,
(7), 139-180.
Linguistic creativity in the language production of bilinguals
Aneta Bučková
(Universität Regensburg)
My paper deals with linguistic creativity in Czech-German bilinguals. The aim is to determine
a typology of deviations in the language production of bilinguals compared to the homeland
variety. I will show that these deviations often reflect the linguistic creativity of the speakers.
The analysis is based on narrative texts collected within the project “Language across
generations: contact induced change in morphosyntax in German-Slavic bilingual speech”
supported by the German Research Foundation (Kosciolek 2019). The corpus consists of
narrative interviews conducted in German and Czech (Nekvapil 2003). The focus lies on phrasal
structures and syntactic (verbal) patterns.
The investigation of linguistic creativity in these patterns draws on a corpus study on
heritage speakers of Russian in the USA by Rakhilina et al. (2016). The notion of creativity as
“speakers’ ability to create novel expressions” (ibid: 2) is based on Chomsky (2009 [1966]). In
this sense, linguistic creativity is also being studied in the context of language contact (Matras
2009).
This study concentrates on two annotation types used to indicate deviations from the
baseline of comparison, i. e. the homeland variety. Firstly, pattern replications (PAT) are such
cases of contact-induced replications where, in contrast to material borrowing, “only the
patterns from one language are replicated” (Sakel 2007: 15) – see (1).
25
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
(1) a. bilingual speech
a
ve škole
si
to
všimli
and in school.LOC REFL DEM.ACC notice.3SG.PTCP
(MS_GAU_MI_CZ_0016, 00:05:29)
b. homeland variety
a
ve škole
si
toho
všimli
and in school.LOC REFL
and they noticed it at school
DEM.GEN
notice.3SG.PTCP
Secondly, ‘other deviations’ (andere Abweichungen, AA) stand for deviations which
cannot be explained by the reference to a concrete pattern from the other language, as in (2).
For this group of deviations, explanations related to the cognitive processes during speech
production are possible.
(2) a. bilingual speech
tak jsme @ udělali ..
kříž
so AUX
make.1PL.PTCP cross.ACC
(BH_BUB_SP_CZ_0002, 00:03:41)
b. homeland variety
tak jsme
se
pokřižovali
so AUX
REFL
cross.1PL.PTCP
so we crossed ourselves
These deviations can be understood as a strategy of how the bilinguals deal with the
progressive linguistic attrition of their secondary language.
References:
Kosciolek, I. M. (2019). DFG Projekt - Language across generations - Universität
Regensburg. https://www.uni-regensburg.de/forschung/language-acrossgenerations/startseite/index.html
Matras, Y. (2009). Language contact. Cambridge textbooks in linguistics. Cambridge
University Press.
Nekvapil, J. (2003). Language biographies and the analysis of language situations: on the life
of the German community in the Czech Republic. International Journal of the Sociology
of Language, 2003(162), 63–83.
Rakhilina, E., Vyrenkova, A., & Polinsky, M. (2016). Linguistic Creativity in Heritage
Speakers.
Glossa:
A
Journal
of
General
Linguistics,
1(1),
1–29.
https://doaj.org/article/64ce196235c84656971a9f5bf37fad64
Sakel, J. (2007). Types of loan: Matter and pattern. In Y. Matras & J. Sakel (Eds.), Empirical
approaches to language typology: Vol. 38. Grammatical borrowing in cross-linguistic
perspective (pp. 15–31). Mouton de Gruyter.
26
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Reading to write an MA thesis in languages and literatures: Are we
educating multilinguals with a monolingual bias?
Natacha Buntinx
(Université Catholique de Louvain)
If multilingualism has long been seen as the co-existence of several monolingualisms, recent
studies have highlighted the importance of different models, such as multicompetence
(Cummins, 2012) or the plea for a translingual agenda (Seloni, 2014). However, in academic
writing, monolingualism seems to be the norm as authors, who often write in English, tend to
mainly read and cite works published in English, even in the field of multilingualism (Horner
et al., 2011). For scholars, this could in part be attributed to a citation advantage when
references are in English (Gong et al., 2019), but students should ideally be free from these
bibliometric pressures. The present paper reports the results of an exploratory study on the role
of pluriliteracy in students’ research process for the writing of their MA thesis.
The eclectic coding (Saldaña, 2016) of 5 fully-transcribed focus groups (n = 28)
highlights how language and literature students at a French-speaking university engage in their
MA thesis project under the form of a cognitive map that allows for a global understanding of
the processes and beliefs leading students to search for sources in one or several languages.
The results show that, surprisingly for a programme that requires students to develop an
expertise in two (often foreign) languages, most students report reading solely in their language
of writing. Students generally lack confidence and face numerous challenges in the first steps
of their project. To define their topic, they seem to rely primarily on knowledge previously
acquired in content classes and/or on sources proposed by their supervisors. These sources of
knowledge then seem to guide their judgment throughout their research process. Monoliterate
practices are arguably inherited from their teachers who might promote monoliterate research
strategies without being aware of it, but also from the structure of their study programme, in
which languages are kept apart and in which multilingual and multicultural competences do not
seem central. This presentation will make a case for the promotion of plurilingual competences
in higher education, as an effective way to develop multicultural competences and to prevent
bibliographical amnesia (Chevrier, 2016, p. 20).
References:
Chevrier, S. (2016). Table ronde n°1: Quel avenir pour la recherche et la publication en
français? La Revue des Sciences de Gestion, 277(1), 13-26.
Cummins, J. (2012). Rethinking monolingual instructional strategies in multilingual
classrooms. RCLA/CJAL, 10(2), 221-40.
Gong, K., Xie, J., Cheng, Y., Larivière, V., & Sugimoto, C. (2019). The citation advantage of
foreign language references for Chinese social science papers. Scientometrics, 120,
1439-60.
Horner, B., NeCamp, S., & Donahue, C. (2011). Toward a multilingual composition
scholarship: From English only to a translingual norm. CCC, 63(2), 269-300. Retrieved
May 20, 2020, from www.jstor.org/stable/23131585
Saldaña, J. (2016). The Coding Manual for Qualitative Researchers (3rd ed.). Sage.
Seloni, L. (2014). 'I'm an artist and a scholar who is trying to find a middle point': A textographic
analysis of a Colombian art historian's thesis writing. Journal of Second Language
Writing, 25, 79-99.
27
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Cross-language phonological similarity of nasal vowels – the case of
French and Polish
Zuzanna Cal
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
Although cross-language phonological similarity is the fundamental property of research on L2
speech, the phenomenon itself is rather unexplored and problematic. There exist prominent
frameworks of second language speech acquisition, such as Speech Learning Model (SLM;
Flege, 1995; 2007) or Perceptual Assimilation Model (PAM; Best, 1995; Best & Tyler, 2007)
which use cross-language similarity as a predictor of success in the process of learning a foreign
language. Yet, one of the major concerns while working with these theories is the fact that there
exists no objective measure of similarity that would be helpful in formulating and testing
predictions about the second language acquisition process. This paper aims to further explore
the field of cross-language phonological similarity on the basis of nasal vowels in Polish and
French. To this end, the paper proposes a new methodology for assessing similarity across
languages, namely it strives to use the Onset Prominence representational framework (OP;
Schwartz, 2010 et seq), as a tool for describing cross-language similarity.
Polish and French nasal vowels differ on phonetic as well as phonological levels and
these discrepancies can be captured in the OP representations. Thus, it is suggested that nasal
vowels in these languages are phonologically different and, therefore, according to the SLM
(Flege, 1995; 2007), speakers of the two languages should not evince any signs of crosslinguistic influence while producing nasal vowels in one of the languages. An experiment has
been designed to confirm these hypotheses by means of analysing L1 phonetic drift in nasal
vowels of L1 Polish, L2 French speakers. The sounds under investigation are the vowels
represented in Polish by ą and ę and their French counterparts [ɛ̃] and [ɔ]̃ in pre-fricative and
pre-stop contexts. It is predicted that the participants should not experience L1 phonetic drift in
nasal vowels, if the vowels are phonologically distinct.
References:
Best, Catherine T. (1995). “A direct realist perspective on cross-language speech perception”.
In: Winifred Strange (Ed.) Speech perception and linguistic experience: Theoretical and
methodological issues in cross-language speech research (pp. 167-200). York:
Timonium, MD.
Best, Catherine T. & Tyler, Michael D. (2007). Nonnative and second-language speech
perception: Commonalities and omplementarities”. In Ocke-Schwen Bohn & Munro,
Murray J. (Eds.) Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning: In Honor
of James Emil Flege (pp. 13-34). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Flege, James E. (1995). “Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems”.
In Winifred Strange (Ed.) Speech perception and linguistic experience: Theoretical and
methodological issues in cross-language speech research (pp. 229-273). Timonium,
MD: York Press.
Flege, James E. (2007). Language contact in bilingualism: Phonetic system interactions. In
Jennifer S. Cole & Hualde, José Ignacio. (Eds.) Laboratory phonology 9 (pp. 353-382).
Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
Schwartz, Geoffrey. (2010). Auditory representations and the structures of GP 2.0. Acta
Linguistica Hungarica 57, 381-397.
28
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
English, Arabic and French teachers’ implementation of a
multilingual pedagogy in UAE secondary schools: Opportunities
and challenges
Raees Calafato
(University of Bergen)
Developing multilingual citizens has become a key goal of language education in many
countries, especially in light of the advent of a multipolar world order where knowledge of
English should be complemented by proficiency in other languages. In order to accomplish this,
it is first important to ensure that teachers are willing and able to motivate their students and
develop their desire to become multilingual. This can be done through the implementation of
multilingual teaching practices where teachers and students draw on their knowledge of other
languages to boost the learning process and their language awareness (Haukås, 2016). This is
especially important in super-diverse contexts where language teachers and students come from
different cultures and might not have a common first language. At present, few studies have
investigated the implementation of multilingual teaching practices as this concerns the teaching
of non-European languages, especially in foreign and second language education contexts. My
presentation will discuss the findings from a mixed-methods study on multilingual language
teachers of English, Arabic, and French in secondary schools in the United Arab Emirates. The
study explored their multilingualism through a dynamic systems framework (Jessner, 2008)
while also investigating their reflexive positioning (Kayı-Aydar, 2019) as teachers and
multilingual individuals and their awareness and use of their multilingual affordances (Aronin,
2014). Drawing upon data collected via a questionnaire and open-ended interviews, the study’s
findings indicated that language education policy in the Emirates can be disjointed and does not
sufficiently focus on developing language teachers’ ability to effectively implement
multilingual teaching practices, which can have negative consequences for both teacher
motivation and learner achievement. The study also identified key factors, beyond language
policy, that appeared to strongly influence the extent to which the participants drew on their
and their students’ knowledge of other languages during lessons. My presentation will also
discuss the implementation of specific measures for developing teachers’ ability to implement
multilingual teaching practices that might lead to language lessons more accurately reflecting
the superdiverse nature of the UAE, with implications for other countries that have similar
levels of super-diversity.
References:
Aronin, L. (2014). The concept of affordances in applied linguistics and multilingualism. In M.
Pawlak, & L. Aronin (Eds.), Essential Topics in Applied Linguistics and Multilingualism
(pp. 157-173). Cham: Springer.
Haukås, Å. (2016). Teachers’ beliefs about multilingualism and a multilingual pedagogical
approach. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(1), 1-18.
Jessner, U. (2008). A DST model of multilingualism and the role of metalinguistic awareness.
The Modern Language Journal, 92(2), 270-283.
Kayı-Aydar, H. (2019). Positioning Theory in Applied Linguistics: Research Design and
Applications. Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave Macmillan.
29
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Interpreting as translanguaging: An SF-MDA approach on
bilingual communication
Xinqiao Cen
(University of Nottingham, Ningbo, China)
“Translanguaging is the act performed by bilinguals of accessing different linguistic features or
various modes of what are described as autonomous languages, in order to maximize
communicative potential.” (García, 2009, p. 140) Various modes are semiotic resources
(Halliday, 1984) including linguistic, prosodic and gestural expressions. Given the multimodal
nature of translanguaging, it gives an implication on bilateral interpreting as an inter-semiotic
communication activity. Interpreters as bilinguals, shoulder the responsibility of meaning
transfer via multiple semiotic resources in the communication process. This paper examines
how interpreter transfers multimodal semiotic resources into those in another language system
from both cognitive and functional perspectives.
We argue that linguistic, prosodic and gestural semiotic resources have communicative
purposes. This research observes the video recordings of China-US diplomatic talks mediated
by consecutive interpreters with a total time duration of 9470 minutes. To find how the
multimodal semiotic resources achieve communicative purposes, we utilise systemic functional
multimodal discourse analysis as a toolkit in exploring the functions of linguistic, prosodic and
gestural modes. A multimodal corpus is established to corroborate that the interpreters and the
audiences perceive meaning through the interaction of multiple semiotic resources. We
approach the meaning transfer mechanism from a cognitive perspective to investigate how the
interpreter achieve the communicative goals via using multimodal semiotic resources.
We find that both language and multimodal cues are semiotic resources serving certain
communicative purposes in consecutive interpreting. Based on the understanding of the
multimodal semiotic resources from source language, interpreter transfer them into those from
another language system to achieve the communicative goal.
This paper offers consecutive interpreting studies a new insight from translanguaging
perspective, exploring the meaning transfer mechanism by the interpreter as a bilingual. It also
reveals the multimodal nature in interpreting which gives a panoramic view on the two-way
communication.
References:
García, O. (2009). Education, multilingualism and translanguaging in the 21st century. In T.
Skutnabb–Kangas, R. Phillipson, A. K. Mohanty & M. Panda (Eds.) Social justice through
multilingual education (pp. 140–158). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Halliday, M. A. (1984). Language as code and language as behaviour: a systemic-functional
interpretation of the nature and ontogenesis of dialogue. The semiotics of culture and
language, 1, 3-35.
O’Halloran, K. L. (2007). Systemic functional multimodal discourse analysis (SF-MDA)
approach to mathematics, grammar and literacy. Advances in language and education, 77102.
Poyatos, F. (1997a). Nonverbal Communication and Translation: New perspectives and
challenges in literature, interpretation and the media (Vol. 17): John Benjamins
Publishing.
Poyatos, F. (1997b). The reality of multichannel verbal-nonverbal communication in
simultaneous and consecutive interpretation. Benjamins Translation Library, 17, 249-282.
30
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
“Brother” and “sister” as terms of address in bilingual
Arabic/Hebrew speakers in Israel
Letizia Cerqueglini
(Tel Aviv University)
I examine the use of the kinship terms (KTs) “brother” and “sister” as terms of address for nonkin among native Muṯallaṯ Arabic (MA) speakers who are bilingual in MA and Hebrew
(MAHEs). MA is a Palestinian Arabic dialect spoken in central Israel. KTs are used as terms
of address for non-kin in both MA and Hebrew politeness practices (Alenizi, 2019; Borg, 2019;
Brown & Levinson, 1978), yet with different norms. The Hebrew KTs “brother” axi/ “sister”
axoti are avoided across gender boundaries. Axi is used in greeting and requests, axoti to preface
critiques; both are used as discourse markers to disapprove of something said by the
interlocutor. MA “brother” āxy/ “sister” uxty are used to express emotional closeness and
surprise, without gender boundaries.
I investigate whether the use of KTs as terms of address among MAHEs is influenced
by Hebrew pragmatic norms, and whether MAHEs use Hebrew KTs to address native Hebrew
speakers. I tested 24 MAHEs, 12 women/12 men, aged 19 to 30, who work and/or study with
native Hebrew speakers. I distributed a questionnaire modified from Braun (1988) to check KT
use with neighbors, work/study colleagues, and strangers, within the MA speaking community
and toward native Hebrew speakers. I hypothesized that the socio-cultural boundary that divides
Arabic-speaking society from Hebrew-speaking society would prevent MAHEs from using
KTs with native Hebrew speakers and that MAHEs’ use of KTs would not be influenced by
Hebrew. Five monolingual MA speakers over age 65 were tested as a control group for MA
traditional KT practices. Results show that MAHEs used KTs as terms of address differently
than elderly MA speakers; informants also used MA āxy/uxty within the MA community to
express polite critique and disagreement, as in Hebrew. MAHEs used Hebrew axi/axoti toward
native Hebrew speakers, regardless of gender boundaries and degree of intimacy, with
increasing frequency of use among less formally educated people.
References:
Alenizi, A. (2019). The norms of address terms in Arabic: The case of Saudi speech community.
International Journal of English Linguistics, 9(5), 227-241.
Borg, A. (2019). From etymology to diachrony. The semantics of ḫwj ‘to protect’ in Old
Egyptian and Bedouin Arabic. Lingua Aegyptia, 27, 1-16.
Braun, F. (1988). Terms of address: Problems of patterns and usage in various languages and
cultures. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Brown, P., Levinson, S. (1978). Universals in language usage: Politeness phenomena.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
31
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Categorizing emotions based on phrase order patterns in Bengali
and Hindi
Spandan Chowdhury
(Jadavpur University)
The conceptualization of experience, when given a particular syntactic structure, is called
construal (Croft & Cruse, 2004). Components of a conceptualized experience, namely stimulus,
response and the associated emotion, are reflected in the construals. Based on six basic emotion
types of Happiness, Surprise, Anger, Sadness, Fear and Disgust proposed by (Ekman, 1993),
this study aims to find whether we can categorize emotions by virtue of their preferences of
phrase orders patterns between the available construals in language describing an event
involving some emotional component.
Two typologically different SOV word-ordered Indo-Aryan languages, Hindi and
Bengali, have been considered for this study. Data has been collected from 100 healthy native
Bengali-speaking participants [male:female=1:1, mean age = 26.41 years, SD = 6.84 years] and
56 healthy native Hindi-speaking participants [male:female=1:1, mean age = 25.46 years, SD
= 6.66 years] after taking their consent. Construals of situations involving a Stimulus Phrase
(S-phrase), a Response Phrase (R-phrase) and an Emotion Phrase (E-phrase) were presented
and a grammaticality judgement test was carried out. Taking into account that stimulus always
precedes response, the position of the E-phrase was varied sentence-initially (ESR ordering),
sentence-medially (SER ordering) and sentence-finally (SRE ordering) for each of the six basic
emotions. As an example, the Hindi sentence for the emotion fear is shown with phrase-marking
as followsː
S-phrase
wo
ʃer
E-phrase
d̪ekʰ kər
3.SG tiger see
do-CP
dər
kər
afraid do.be-CP
R-phrase
bʰag gəja
run
[SER ordering]
go-PERF
‘He saw the tiger and fled in fear.’
It is observed that for both languages, the SER ordering is the most preferred structure.
Statistical test results (ANOVA and post-hoc analysis) were found to be significant [pvalue<<0.01]. Furthermore, emotions could be categorized into groups, based on number of
orders and the relative pattern of preferences for different orders. Other interesting findings
have also been discussed.
References:
Croft, W., & Cruse, D. A. (2004). Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge University Press.
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error. Emotion, reason, and the human brain. London: Picador.
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American psychologist, 48(4), 384.
Ekman, P. (1999). Handbook of cognition and emotion in T. Dalgleish and M. Power (Eds).
Wiley and Sons. Ltd., Sussex, U.K.
Foolen, A. (1997). The expressive function of language: towards a cognitive semantic approach.
In: S. Niemeier & R. Dirven (Eds.), The language of emotions (15–31). Amsterdam:
Benjamins.
32
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Foolen, A. (2012). The relevance of emotion for language and linguistics. Moving
ourselves,moving others: Motion and emotion in intersubjectivity, consciousness and
language, 349-369.
Inclusive teaching practices: International learners with Specific
Learning Differences (SpLDs) in EAP
Jennifer Cowell
(University of St Andrews & University of Stirling)
Annually 45,000 new entrants to UK Universities report having at least one disability, with
Specific Learning Differences (SpLDs) being the most common (QS, 2019). SpLDs is an
umbrella term proposed in 2005 by the UK Department for Education Working Group and
includes labels such as Dyspraxia, AD(H)D, Dyscalculia, Dyslexia, Visual Stress, and by
association Autistic Spectrum Disorder. A rough rule-of-thumb for UK Universities is that 1 in
10 students require additional support provisions including for SpLDs (Office for Students,
2019), although not all of them access this support. In 2018/19, 20% of the total UK student
population was composed of international students, with some being as high as 48% (House of
Commons Library, 2020). Despite this significant composition, declaration of and explicit
accommodation for international students with SpLDs remains a persistent challenge.
For international students whose L1 is not English, language support is essential,
including EAP modules embedded into the curriculum. This provision can be pivotal to their
overall academic success within their programme of study, yet some learners do not access the
additional learning support to aid them during these learning experiences. This can be due to a
variety of reasons including their cultural norms and expectations, their personal learning
preferences, or due to not knowing that they have one or more SpLD. It is common for learners
to not realise that they have SpLDs and they can at times be hard to detect; for example,
common language errors produced by learners of English, and the common signs of dyslexia
can be identical (Kormos, 2016, p.26). This can make it very difficult for University teaching
staff to discern the source of the problem.
The poster will explore different inclusive teaching techniques which can be
implemented into any student-centric methodology to assist EAP learners with SpLDs, whether
they are declared or not, and thus provide a safety net for all international learners. The poster
will share information and resources related to inclusive teaching which can be adapted for use
in various teaching settings, to meet the needs of learners in a multilingual world.
References:
House of Commons Library, UK Parliament. (2020). Research Briefing: International and EU
students in higher education in the UK FAQs. Retrieved from
https://commonslibrary.parliament.uk/research-briefings/cbp7976/#:~:text=In%202018%2F19%20there%20were,EU%20and%20343%2C000%20
from%20elsewhere.
Kormos, J. (2016). The Second Language Learning Processes of Students with Specific
Learning Difficulties. New York: Routledge.
Office for Students. (2019). Insight brief: Beyond the bare minimum: Are universities and
colleges
doing
enough
for
disabled
students?.
Retrieved
from
33
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
https://www.officeforstudents.org.uk/publications/beyond-the-bare-minimum-areuniversities-and-colleges-doing-enough-for-disabled-students/.
QS Quacquarelli Symonds. (2019). What Support Should Universities Provide to Students with
Specific Learning Difficulties?. Retrieved from https://www.qs.com/what-supportshould-universities-provide-students-specific-learning-difficulties/.
Cultural issues and translation questions in translating manga
Lilla Zsófia Cseh
(Eötvös Loránd University)
This paper offers insight into the translation issues related to a relatively new phenomenon of
popular culture: manga (Japanese comics), read by an increasing number of people worldwide.
There is high demand for them also outside Japan, which naturally means they will need to be
translated, and in great numbers. I will first briefly explain their history, their differences from
Western comics, then describe the peculiar challenges faced by translators when working on
comics, as opposed to literary works or technical texts. Manga, due to particular features of
Japanese language (words read top-down, from right-to-left) are an even harder task. Language
features and cultural differences make translated manga unique pieces of work which need to
be analysed to understand this segment of the often neglected field of comic translation. This
research on manga translation consists of two related parts. First, I will compare translations in
different languages (mainly English, Italian and French), and, if the case demands it, also show
the Japanese original. Using images and texts as examples, I will explain the solutions
translators employed to circumvent the language obstacles. Second, I will address cultural
issues, which do not present themselves only in words or expressions (easier to translate or
explain) unknown in the West, but, in manga, also in images; another challenge for translators,
so I will analyse the ways they faced it. I will attempt to find the best possible translations of
the original and answer the question of how manga could be read fluently and easily in the
West, but formulated without a distortion of its original setting. In conclusion, the paper will
highlight the rarely researched, but intriguing questions of comic translation, focusing on the
especially difficult task of manga. Hopefully, it will motivate researchers to look at comics from
a new point of view, inspiring further research.
References:
Bouissou, J.-M. (2011). Miért vált kulturális világtermékké a manga? Korunk 22 (2), 54–63.
D’Arcangelo, A., & Zanettin, F. (2004). Dylan Dog Goes to the USA: a North American
Translation of an Italian Comic Book Series. Across Languages and Cultures 5(2), 187–
210.
Davies, R. J., & Ikeno, O. (2019). A japán észjárás. Pallas Athéné Könyvkiadó.
Gobbi, P. (2008). Giappone. Storie d’amore e guerra. Arthemisia Books.
De la Iglesia, M. (2015, October 3 – November 2): The task of Manga Translation: Akira in the
West. [Panel presentation] Übersetzungen und Adaptionen von Comics / ‘The Translation
and
Adaptation
of
Comics’,
Hildesheim,
Germany,
https://www.comicsgrid.com/articles/10.16995/cg.59/
Halpern, J. (Ed.) (2011): The Kodansha Kanji Learner’s Dictionary. Kodansha USA Inc.
Koyama-Richard, B. (2007): Mille anni di manga. Rizzoli.
Sato-Rossberg, N., & Wakabayashi, J. (2013): Translation and Translation Studies in the
Japanese Context. Bloomsbury.
34
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Viljanen-Belkasseh, P., & Metsäjätti, M. (2015). Nelle terre dei GAL: Dall’Italia alla Finlandia
attraverso il fumetto. Settentrione, 26, 122–129.
Zanettin, F.(ed.) (2008): Comics in translation. Kinderhook&St Jerome.
Zanettin, F. (1998): Fumetti e traduzione multimediale.
InTRAlinea, 1
http://www.intralinea.org/archive/article/Fumetti_e_traduzione_multimediale
Language contact phenomena in a context of migration
Valentina Del Vecchio
("G. d'Annunzio" University of Chieti-Pescara)
In recent years, bi-/multilingualism has received much scholarly attention from both an
individual and a societal perspective. Research on languages in multilingual contexts has shed
light on various linguistic dynamics, among which are the way in which different language
varieties interact when coming into contact and the effects of such an interaction. Contexts of
migration have proved fertile grounds for exploring bi-/multilingualism from several points of
view (Auer, 1991; Clyne, 2003; Rubino, 2014; Di Salvo, 2014). In these settings, people with
a certain sociolinguistic background are exposed to a new language, i.e. the language of the
host country, which in most cases becomes their second language and part of their linguistic
repertoires. Speakers in these contexts usually exploit their complex repertoires, for example
using the different codes in the same speech, according to the function or identity they want to
perform. This results in some language contact phenomena at the discourse level.
An example of this kind of situation is the case of a community from two small Italian
villages living in Bletchley (Milton Keynes, England), whose average linguistic repertoire is
composed of three main language varieties, i.e. a variety of English, an Italo-Romance dialect,
and a variety of Italian.
Taking into account these characteristics, the research aims at detecting such language
contact phenomena as ‘code-switching’ and ‘code-mixing’ in the speech of migrants from both
the first and second generations and analyse their function. Besides, it also has the objective of
assessing their relation with non-linguistic factors, such as the migration policies of the host
country, the situational context, the age and gender of the speakers, etc. In order to accomplish
these research tasks, data of real interactions will be collected, through both interviews and
recordings of spontaneous speech.
Considering the sociolinguistic features of the community, we expect to detect some of
the classical language contact phenomena, i.e. ‘code-switching’ and ‘code-mixing’, and
specific functions related to them. Furthermore, we expect some of the non-linguistic factors to
have a greater influence than others on the trends of such phenomena.
References:
Auer, P. (1991). Italian in Toronto. A preliminary comparative study on language use and
language maintenance. Multilingua, 10 (4), 403-440.
Clyne, M. (2003). Dynamics of Language Contact. English and Immigrant Languages.
Cambridge University Press.
Di Salvo, M. (2014). Variazione linguistica e identità regionale: I pugliesi di Cambridge e di
Bedford. In R. Bombi & V. Orioles (Eds.), Essere italiani nel mondo globale oggi.
Riscoprire l'appartenenza (pp. 77-107). Forum.
Rubino, A. (2014). Trilingual Talk in Sicilian-Australian Migrant Families. Playing Out
Identities Through Language Alternation. Palgrave Macmillan.
35
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Stops and affricates in Basque
Jakub Dunin-Borkowski
(University of Warsaw)
In Basque, stops are deleted before other stops, e.g., //bat paratu//→[ba paratu] ‘put one’. In the
framework of The Sound Pattern of English (SPE; Chomsky & Halle, 1968), the rule
responsible for such a mapping can be phrased as [-sonorant, -continuant]→ø/_[-continuant].
This analysis becomes problematic once affricates are considered. In similar contexts, affricates
alternate with fricatives when followed by stops, e.g., //ot͡s bat//→[os bat] ‘a cold’. Importantly,
SPE treats affricates as stops specified for the feature [+delayed release]. Therefore, the
proposed SPE rule predicts deletion of the entire affricate (e.g., *[o bat]), rather than a mutation
into a spirant. This drives Hualde (1988, 1991) to conclude that, in fact, affricates must be
treated as segments specified for both values of the feature [continuant] ([+continuant] and
[-continuant] on a single segment).
New light on the representation of affricates is shed by Optimality Theory (OT; Prince
& Smolensky, 1993). In OT, outputs are evaluated based on their violations of constraints.
Consequently, affricates are readily analyzed as phonological stops, without the need of
postulating contradicting specifications of the feature [continuant] on a single segment.
Importantly, following Jakobson et al. (1952), the SPE feature [delayed release] is replaced by
the feature [strident] in the representation of affricates. Such an approach is theoretically
advantageous from the perspective of cross-linguistic data. In English, Turkish, Polish and
many other languages, affricates pattern phonologically with stops (La Charité, 1993, Rubach,
1994, Clements, 1999, Kehrein, 2002, Kim et al., 2015, Berns, 2016) and not with continuants.
This indicates that affricates cannot bear the feature [+continuant].
I propose that the behavior of stops and affricates in Basque can be attributed to
similarity avoidance of two adjacent [-continuant] segments (e.g, Suzuki, 1998), a solution
allowed by OT. An alternative approach is based on syllable well-formedness constraints,
where stops are not allowed in codas (e.g., Artiagoitia, 1993). Such an analysis, however, is not
adequate in the description of Modern Basque since coda stops are permitted in new borrowings
and compounds, e.g., webgune ‘website’.
References:
Artiagoitia, X. (1993). Syllable structure in Modern Basque and in Proto-Basque. In J. I. Hualde
& J. Ortiz de Urbina (Eds.), Generative Studies in Basque Linguistics (pp. 263–287).
Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Berns, J. (2016). The phonological representation of affricates. Language and Linguistics
Compass, 10, 142–156.
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.
Clements, G. N. (1999). Affricates as noncontoured stops. In O. Fujimura, B. D. Joseph & B.
Palek (Eds.), Proceedings of LP ’98: Item Order in Language and Speech (pp. 271–
299). Prague: The Karolinum Press.
Hualde, J. I. (1988). On Basque affricates. Anuario del Seminario de Filología Vasca "Julio de
Urquijo", 22, 379–389.
Hualde, J. I. (1991). Basque Phonology. London: Routledge.
Jakobson, R., Fant, G., & Halle, M. (1952). Preliminaries to Speech Analysis. Cambridge,
Mass.: MIT Press.
36
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Kehrein, W. (2002). Phonological Representation and Phonetic Phasing: Affricates and
Laryngeals. Tübingen: De Gruyter.
Kim, H., Clements, G., & Toda. M. (2015). The feature [strident]. In A. Rialland, R. Ridouane
& H. van der Hulst (Eds.), Features in Phonology and Phonetics: Unpublished Work
from George N. Clements and his Colleagues (pp. 179–194). Berlin: De Gruyter
Mouton.
La Charité, D. (1993). The internal structure of affricates. PhD dissertation, University of
Ottawa.
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative
grammar. Ms., Rutgers University, New Brunswick, N.J., and University of Colorado,
Boulder.
Rubach, J. (1994). Affricates as strident stops in Polish. Linguistic Inquiry, 25, 119–143.
Suzuki, K. (1998). A typological investigation of dissimilation. PhD dissertation, University of
Arizona.
‘Why can’t you just be yourself and not say anything?’ or a
multilevel cognitive approach to coming out
Tomasz Dyrmo
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
Within the framework of multilevel approach to conceptual metaphor (Kövecses 2017), it is
claimed that metaphors can be analysed at various levels of schematicity-specificity continuum,
from image schemas at the one end to mental spaces/scenarios at the other. Coming out, defined
explicitly as ‘movement of LGB sexuality from inside to outside’ (Lovelock 2017: 3), is an
example of a conceptual metaphor expressed in language and as such can be both decomposed
into lower-level structures (e.g. image schemas) and integrated into larger chunks of discourse
(e.g. frames and scenarios). This conceptual decomposition of coming out has not been
attempted yet.
In attempting such a composition and taking a bottom-up approach to coming out as a
process, I suggest in the presentation that coming out conceptual metaphor is based on image
schemas (ITERATION, e.g. Perez and Ruiz de Mendoza 2002 OBJECT, e.g. Szwedek 2011, and
FORCE dynamics, e.g. Talmy 2015), domains of MOVEMENT and TRANSFER (e.g. Reddy
1979, Goldberg 1995), frames that contain various elements: roles and relations (Lakoff 2010),
as well as scenarios that are narratively elaborated and axiologically enriched subtypes of
frames (Musolff 2016, 2017). I show that scenarios, which are the most individualised and least
schematic of all cognitive structures in the analysis, are founded on two generic metaphors:
COMING OUT OF THE BOUNDED SPACE IS REVEALING A SECRET and SECRETS ARE
HEAVY OBJECTS, both of which are chiefly based on the previously discussed, lower-level
cognitive phenomena.
In the present analysis I use a publicly available repository of coming out stories,
published at www.whenicameout.com. The website contains over 2100 narratives, yet I use
only a fraction to illustrate my attempt at presenting coming out as a multilevel conceptual
metaphor, motivated by various interdependent conceptual mechanisms.
37
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument
structure. University of Chicago Press.
Kövecses, Z. (2017). Levels of metaphor. Cognitive Linguistics, 28(2), 321–347.
Lakoff, G. (2010). Why it matters how we frame the environment. Environmental
Communication, 4(1), 70–81.
Lovelock, M. (2017). ‘My coming out story’: Lesbian, gay and bisexual youth identities on
YouTube. International Journal of Cultural Studies, 22(1), 70–85
Musolff, A. (2016). Political metaphor analysis: Discourse and scenarios. Bloomsbury
Academic
Musolff, A. (2017). Truths, lies and figurative scenarios: Metaphors at the heart of
Brexit. Journal of Language and Politics, 16(5), 641–657.
Pérez Hernández, L., & Ruiz de Mendoza, F. J. (2002). Grounding, semantic motivation, and
conceptual interaction in indirect directive speech acts. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(3), 259–
284
Reddy, M. (1979). The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in our language about lang
uage. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought. 284-324. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press
Szwedek, A. (2011). The ultimate source domain. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 9(2), 341–
366.
Talmy, L. (2015). Relating language to other cognitive systems: An overview. Cognitive
Semantics, 1(1), 1–44.
Are complex sentences harder to translate or interpret in some
language pairs than in others?
Selim Earls
(University of Louvain)
Are translation and interpretation of complex sentences between languages from different
families associated with greater production or processing difficulties than transfer between
related languages? If so, are those difficulties proportional to the structural difference of the
language pair, and are there differences in correlations according to the mode of transfer?
This study examines the link between structural difference and the difficulty of complex
sentence transfer between English and five languages from different families (Hungarian,
Turkish, Arabic, Mandarin and Japanese). Specifically, it examines the link between differences
in branching directionality and three indicators of production or processing difficulty: nesting,
reordering and relations between propositions.
Nesting and the associated enlarging of phrasal domains have been shown to be a factor
of processing difficulty (Karlsson 2006; Hawkins 1994). Reordering of items in recall has been
shown to be a factor of production difficulty (Anders & Lillyquist 2013; Thomas, Milner &
Haberlandt 2003). And reproduction of relations between propositions has been identified as
central to the successful transfer of meaning (Larson 1984), so distortion of those relations can
be taken as another indicator of production difficulty for the translator or interpreter.
This study analyzes a sample corpus of 20 complex sentences in two different modes of
language transfer: translation (from the European Convention on Human Rights) and
38
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
simultaneous interpretation (from the European Parliament website, the UN website and a
professional interpreter’s Youtube channel).
Each language version of each sentence is parsed semantically (on a parse tree in the
tradition of transformational grammar, but with each branch showing a semantic proposition).
Those trees are used to illustrate and measure branching directionality and the above three
features of difficulty for each sentence. The resulting values are averaged and correlated for
each language pair in each mode.
The resulting strong correlations (r = ±0.9) provide initial confirmation that translation
and interpretation between languages from different families are associated with specific
production and processing difficulties, and that those difficulties are proportional to the
structural difference of the language pair, with some differences in correlations acording to the
mode of transfer.
References:
Anders, T. and Lilliquist, T. D. (2013). Retrieval time in forward and backward recall.
Psychonomic Science, 22, 205–206.
Hawkins, J. A. (1994). A performance theory of word order and constituency. Cambridge
University Press.
Karlsson, F. (2006). Constraints on multiple center-embedding of clauses. Journal of
Linguistics 43(2), 365–392.
Larson, M. L. (1984). Meaning-based translation: A guide to cross-language equivalence.
University Press of America.
Thomas, J. G., Milner, H. R., & Haberlandt, K. F. (2003). Forward and backward recall:
Different response time patterns, same retrieval order. Psychological Science 14(2): 169–
174.
Language choices and identity construction among Tamils in India
Elizabeth Eldho
(Indian Institute of Technology)
Languages in a multilingual nation work in a different way altogether; for instance, one
language may enjoy supremacy over the other. Such hierarchical relations among languages
often lead to tensions and conflicts in a multilingial setup. The Indian subcontinent is home to
diverse languages and cultures, however, the nation has been witnessing issues of language
identity post independence starting from the formation of separate states based on linguistic
majority. Tamil Nadu, the southern state in the multilingual and multicultural India is in the
forefront of language identity politics against the imposition of single national language
(Pandian, 1994, 2002). In the present scenario, Tamil and English serve as the official and
additional official language of the state respectively. The study draws on qualitative data
elicited through questionnaires and sociolinguistic interviews conducted among randomly
selected native Tamil speakers to examine how they construct linguistic identities based on their
language choices. What are the speakers’ language preferences in different domains in the given
community? What is the attitude that the speakers have towards Tamil, English, and Hindi?
What is the motif behind their language choices and attitudes? What are the social factors that
39
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
influence their language use and choices? By addressing these questions, the paper aims at
understanding how the speakers use language more than just a tool for communication, but as
medium to construct identity (Nadarajah, 2018). Results show that the use of mothertongue,
Tamil is observed to be integral in indexing the Tamil identity, culture, provided the years of
rich literary traditions that Tamil is quipped with among many, whereas English is considered
a major facilitator in job markets and central to upward mobility. The paper also discusses the
state’s opposition to the imposition of Hindi as a means to preserve a distinct Tamil identity
and its implications on the multilingual ethos of the nation as well as among the speakers.
References:
Pandian, M. S. S. (1994). Notes on the Transformation of ‘Dravidian’ Ideology: Tamilnadu, c.
1900-1940. Social Scientist, 84-104. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3517904
Pandian, M. S. S. (2002). One Step outside Modernity: Caste, Identity Politics and Public
Sphere. Economic and Political Weekly, 1735-1741. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4412071
Nadarajah, S. (2018). The Tamil proscriptions: Identities, Legitimacies, and Situated Practices.
Terrorism and Political Violence, 30(2), 278-297.
Transferring stress patterns
Kathrin Feindt
(University of Hamburg)
Emphasis in the form of relative prominence in comparison to other syllables within a word is
produced by modification of the acoustic parameters duration, intensity, and fundamental
frequency (F0). A stressed syllable then is marked by stronger pitch movements and
lengthening of the nucleus and is also louder than surrounding syllables. Through interlinguistic
difference in deploying these parameters stress assignment is a specific issue in L3 acquisition.
Thus, duration is the most important cue in German and English, while changes in pitch are
crucial in Turkish. To investigate which linguistic system leaves traces in bilinguals’ foreign
language English, the oral performance of 29 bilingual Turkish-German students in school year
7 and 9 was examined for word stress, with 37 monolingual German participants in the same
age to act as control group. The data examined here are recordings of spoken language that was
gathered for the quasi-longitudinal research project Mehrsprachigkeitsentwicklung im
Zeitverlauf (MEZ). The recordings are analysed to filter out which of the acoustic correlates is
most dominantly used, whereby examination is restricted to bisyllabic words that meet certain
requirements. The total amount of stressed syllables in the corpus is condensed into a subcorpus containing bisyllabic words that occur in any other position than utterance-final and are
not associated with semantic or pragmatic novelty or paradigmatic/syntagmatic contrasts.
While the word <breakfast> is always stressed on the first syllable, stress falls on the
second syllable in roughly 40% of the cases in the word <coffee> and with that the difference
is even highly statically significant (p-value = 0,000; X2 =3,73; df = 1). The observation that
learners deviate from the English stress pattern in pronouncing the word <coffee> can be
explained by considering the cognates in German and Turkish. In
German, there is (regional) variation. Although stressed first syllable prevails the Northern
German dialect, diachronic analysis showed a tendency towards stressing the second syllable
in modern German. For the Turkish /kʌh've/, stress always falls on the last syllable. Thus,
whereas the acoustic parameters unveiled positive transfer, the influence of the background
languages is negative in this case.
40
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Regarding the acoustic correlates of stress, duration is by far the most prominent feature
(≈40%), followed by loudness (≈11%) and pitch (≈10%). In all other cases (≈38%) none of
these is especially highlighted. Both linguistic groups behave very target-like in signalling
stress through duration. There is no direct negative influence of Turkish because pitch is used
only rarely. Moreover, comparing more advanced to younger learners shows that stress
assignment is so well deployed from early on that there is good reason to assume positive
transfer from German phonological rules. The fact that there is a slight, nevertheless
insignificant, tendency for the learners with knowledge of Turkish to depend more on pitch
movement underpins the claim of a facilitated acquisition process in which typologically similar
structures are transferred to the target language, in this case from the dominant language
German.
Personal pronouns and the identity construction of non-binary users
of Tumblr
Katerina Filipova
(Charles University Prague)
Using indexicality to study gender identity is one of the staples in Sociolinguistics ever since
Bocholt’s and Hall’s highly influential paper presenting a framework for studying identity as a
product of interaction (Bucholtz & Hall, 2005). Since then, attention was paid to transgender
people’s identity creation, while other gender non-conforming identities remain mostly outside
of the main focus.
Although personal pronouns are just one type of indexicals, they are an important part
of gender identity of non-binary people. Many of them seek out and use gender-neutral
pronouns that better reflect their gender. In English, such pronouns can be singular they/ them.
However, not many languages are this flexible, which can bring complications to non-binary
people, who are not native English speakers.
This paper thus examines gender identity creation, personal pronouns and their usage
by non-binary individuals. This topic was part of my larger ethnographic thesis project on nonbinary users of the blogging website Tumblr. By conducting semi-structured interviews, I aimed
to find out:
a) The ways non-binary people construct gender identities using personal pronouns,
b) how interactions in the real world, where they are forced to use more gendered
languages, influence it, and
c) how it changes after they log onto the English-speaking website Tumblr.
While some participants were comfortable using pronouns corresponding to the gender
assigned at birth, most of them preferred using gender-neutral pronouns which allow more
nuanced expression of gender without connections to one of the binary genders. However, often
it was not possible to achieve in most language. Participants were able to use the English neutral
pronouns only during communication on the website, whereas in real life, they were forced to
use pronouns corresponding to their assigned gender. Lastly, some choose binary pronouns
closer to their gender expression.
Gender construction was then vastly dependent on the context of the interaction, mainly
on four factors: 1. are all participants of the conversation aware of person gender and pronouns,
2. do they accept and acknowledge them, 3. place of the conversation, and 4. language used.
41
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Bucholtz, M., & Hall, K. (2005). Identity and interaction: A sociocultural linguistic approach.
Discourse Studies, 7(4-5), 585-614.
Processability approaches to instructed language learning in
multilingual contexts: Developmental stages from a receptive
competence perspective
Helen Forsyth
(Free University of Bozen-Bolzano)
Third Language Acquisition (TLA) is a relatively new field that is growing rapidly. A
consequence of mass migration, globalization, and technological advances, along with the
presence of linguistic minorities in many regions, is that an increasing proportion of the world’s
population is learning English as a Third or Additional Language both in naturalistic and
instructed settings. Existing research indicates a qualitative difference between Second
Language Learning (SLA) and TLA (Cenoz, 2003; Hoffmann, 2001; Jessner & Cenoz, 2007;
Rothman et al., 2015), and certain psycholinguistic and developmental aspects to multilingual
learners merit investigation.
The proposed presentation outlines my ongoing PhD research project into
developmental stages in learner acquisition of English as a Third Language at Italian-language
primary schools within the Italian-German bilingual context of Alto Adige/Südtirol in Italy. It
examines the role that processing-based approaches to acquisition, particularly Processability
Theory (Pienemann, 1998, 2005) and one of its components, the Developmentally Moderated
Transfer Hypothesis (Pienemann, 2015), which propose constraints regarding developmental
readiness and cross-linguistic influence, may play for the emergence of receptive competence
in morphosyntactic structures in Third Language Learners. Previous empirical studies in this
field have focused on production data, and Processability Theory has been overwhelmingly
applied to Second Language Acquisition rather than Third Language Acquisition. The data
discussed and presented were collected cross-sectionally by means of a picture selection task
(ELIAS Grammar Test II, Steinlen et al., 2010) and analysed using implicational scaling
methods, as well as gathered from semi-structured interviews with practitioners teaching
English as a Third Language at local primary schools.
This study attempts to offer insights into the extent that teachability of English in such
contexts may be constrained by processability, possibly resulting in a more complex cognitive
approach to language processing and showing transfer effects that merit pedagogic attention. It
aims to fill a significant gap in the field of Processability Theory research into receptive
competence, examine its applicability to Third Language Acquisition, and contribute towards
a more comprehensive theory of early-stage Third Language Acquisition in general.
References:
Cenoz, J. (2003). The additive effect of bilingualism on third language acquisition: a review.
International Journal of Bilingualism 7(1), 71-87.
Hoffmann, C. (2001). Towards a Description of Trilingual Competence. International Journal
of Bilingualism, 5(1), 1-17.
42
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Jessner, U. & Cenoz, J. (2007). Teaching English as a Third Language. In: J. Cummins & C.
Davison (Eds.) International handbook of English language teaching. Springer
International Handbooks of Education, (Vol. 15, pp. 155-167). Springer, Boston.
Pienemann, M. (1998). Developmental dynamics in L1 and L2 acquisition: Processability
Theory and generative entrenchment. Bilingualism 1(1), 1-20.
Pienemann, M. (Ed.) (2005). Cross-linguistic aspects of processability theory. Amsterdam:
Benjamins.
Pienemann, M. (2015). An outline of Processability Theory and its relationship to other
approaches to SLA. Language Learning 65(1), 23-151.
Rothman, J., Alemán Bañón, J., & González Alonso, J. (2015). Neurolinguistic measures of
typological effects in multilingual transfer: introducing an ERP methodology. Frontiers
in Psychology 6, 1087.
Steinlen, A., Massler, U., Schelletter, C. et al. (2010). Results of the ELIAS Research Studies:
A Summary. In K. Kersten. & A. Rohde et al. (Eds.) Bilingual Preschools (Vol. 2, pp.
35-76). WVT Wissenschaftlicher Verlag, Trier.
Literary text title as a paratextual element and its pragmatic status
Maia Gabadadze
(Akaki Tsereteli State University)
The research paper deals with the linguocultural study of literary text titles. The first attempt to
analyze titles was made in the twentieth century. A solid foundation for this was made by a
famous Russian aesthetician and scholar S. Krzhizhanovskij with his work “On the Poetics of
Titles”. Titology, generally regarded as recent movement in literary criticism appeared in the
beginning of the 21st century. It was formed as a separate field of literary studies.
Different aspects of literary text have always been the object of academic interest of
scholars, still there are many problems that require special attention. We assume that the
linguocultural research will enable us to analyze linguistic phenomena via interdisciplinary and
interparadigmatic methodology.
In linguistic literature and literary criticism the title is considered either as a constituent
part of a text or as an independent text. In modern textual theory, titles are classified as an
element of what Gérard Genette has termed the text’s ‘paratext’. Genette argues in his seminal
Seuils (the title in translation is Paratexts: Thresholds of Interpretation) that texts are ‘rarely
presented in an unadorned state, unreinforced and unaccompanied by a certain number of verbal
or other productions, such as an author’s name, a title, a preface, illustrations’. Paratext
transforms the text into a book so that it can be received and read as such. The title, then, as a
paratextual element, is both inside and outside the text, that is, both part of a text and an
independent text itself.
Linguocultural study of literary text titles enables us to reveal the linguocultural essence
of titles that are considered to be the conceptual nucleus of the work; phenomenon which
expresses author’s individual style and epochal literary thinking.
References:
Firbas J. (1986). On the dynamics of written communication in the light of the theory of Functional
Sentence Perspective. In: Cooper & Greenbaum (Eds.). Studying Writing: Linguistic
Approaches. Beverley Hills: Sage, pp. 40–71.
Genette G. (1983). Narrative Discourse. An Essay in Method. Cornell University Press, Ithaca.
43
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Genette G. (2001). “Paratexts. Thresholds of Interpretation”, Cambridge University Press.
Meyerovich A. (2007). Title and Concept: The Case of Interrelation. Rehovot, Israel.
Searle J. (1958). Proper Names. // Mind, New Series, Vol. 67, No 266 (Apr.), Oxford: Oxford
University Press: 166-173.
Taha I. (2009). Semiotics of Literary Titling: Three Categories of Reference. Applied Semiotics,
University of Haifa.
Basic-level multimodal perception verbs in French, Spanish, and
Hungarian: A contrastive study of sentir and érez
Ádám Galac
(Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem)
The present study focuses on three highly polysemous multimodal perception verbs: French
sentir, Spanish sentir, and Hungarian érez. All three can be considered as the multimodal verbs
par excellence in the corresponding languages (cf. Fernández Jaén 2012: 472) – and not only
can they refer to (external) tactile, olfactory and gustatory sensory perception, but they are
crucial in the linguistic conceptualization of internal sensations (proprioception), emotions, and
certain cognitive states as well. In this way, they do not conform to the five senses folk model
(cf. Winter 2019: 11-15), and underline the inextricable connections between perception,
emotion and cognition. The aim of this research is to follow the footsteps of Enghels and
Jansegers (2013) who have carried out a cross-linguistic analysis of sentir(e) in French,
Spanish, and Italian, and to take a look at their Hungarian counterpart, érez, with a view to
exploring the scope of this highly embodied verb in another social context. This decision can
be justified by the fact that érez shows remarkable semantic similarities with the Romance
verbs, even though they belong to genealogically and typologically completely different
languages, and their etymologies are different as well. The Classical Latin etymon of the
Romance cognates, sentiō, used to refer to general physical and cognitive perception (Jansegers
& Gries 2020: 148), hence its semantic evolution is characterized by both meaning restriction
and profiling, whereas Hungarian érez has probably developed its current polysemy from a
single tactile meaning, related to the verb ér ‘touch; reach, arrive’ (TESz. 1: 785-786; cf.
Sweetser 1990: 37-38). By mapping out and contrasting the semantic and syntactic behaviour
of these verbs and by exploring their diachronic development, I hope to contribute to the
discovery of the interrelations between language and embodiment in the context of modern
Western culture.
References:
Enghels, R. & Jansegers, M. (2013). On the crosslinguistic equivalence of sentir(e) in Romance
languages: A contrastive study in semantics. Linguistics, 51(5), 957-991.
Fernández Jaén, J. (2012). Semántica cognitiva diacrónica de los verbos de percepción física
del español. Tesis doctoral. Alicante, Spain: Universidad de Alicante. Retrieved from
https://rua.ua.es/dspace/bitstream/10045/26481/1/Tesis_Jorge_Fernandez_Jaen.pdf
Jansegers, M. & Gries, S. Th. (2020). Towards a dynamic behavioral profile: A diachronic
study of polysemous sentir in Spanish. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 16(1),
145-187.
44
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Sweetser, E. (1990). From Etymology to Pragmatics. Metaphorical and Cultural Aspects of
Semantic Structure. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
TESz = Benkő, L. (Ed.). (1967–1976). A magyar nyelv történeti-etimológiai szótára 1–3.
Budapest, Hungary: Akadémiai Kiadó.
Winter, B. (2019). Sensory Linguistics. Amsterdam, Netherlands / Philadelphia, PA: John
Benjamins Publishing.
Determining the functions of the discourse marker you know
through its Persian translation equivalents
Zahra Ghane (Heinrich Heine University) and Mehrdad Vasheghani Farahani (Leipzig
University)
Depending on context, the English linguistic unit you know can be used as a unit either in the
grammatical grammar or in thetical grammar, i.e. as a discourse marker (DM). While it has
already been given close attention by scholars, whose research provided much insight into its
nature, the marker has barely been looked into from a cross-linguistic perspective. Such a
perspective is adopted in the present study, which is based on translation material. Unlike its
grammatical counterpart, the clausal DM you know does not lend itself to a straightforward
translation into Persian, representing instead a complex, one-to-many kind of relationship
between the original and the target language.
Based on the assumption that cross-linguistic research making use of translation data
may be highly revealing about semantically opaque expressions, such as DMs, the study aims
to gain a deeper insight into the nature of you know by looking at its Persian equivalents from
the perspective of their potential to bring to the surface and elucidate the functions that the
marker serves in various contexts. The study is qualitative in nature and its objective is to shed
more light on the functional scope of the marker you know by looking at its numerous Persian
equivalents. In order to achieve this goal an attempt is made to provide answers to the following
research questions:
(1) What Persian expressions serve as translation equivalents of the English marker?
(2) What context-sensitive functions of the marker are brought to the surface in Persian
translations and illuminated by them?
To this end, Mizan parallel corpus of English-Persian languages (13,596,676 tokens)
was exploited as the data gathering source. Alongside the parallel corpus, a second Persian
corpus called Bijankhan Corpus (68,560, 954 words) was used as the reference corpus against
which translations were compared. The DMs are chosen based upon what we have in Discourse
Garmmar (Heine et.al, 2005). The image of the DM you know which emerges from the study is
that of a contextually richly nuanced expression, fulfilling numerous discourse structuring and
intensifying functions. In its structural role, you know proves capable of marking a number of
different relations between discourse segments, such as result, repair, explanation or
elaboration. As an intensifying device, the DM can be more explicitly emotive (claiming, for
instance, sympathy or acceptance), emphatic or conative in nature. In more general terms, the
paper shows how a translation-based study may inform pragmatic research.
References:
Kaltenböck, G., Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2011). On thetical grammar. Studies in Language,
35 (4), 848-893.
45
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Heine, B., Kaltenböck, G., Kuteva, T and Long, H. (2013). An outline of discourse grammar.
In Shannon Bischoff and Carmen Jany (eds.), Functional Approaches to Language.
Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 175-233.
Research methods to test working memory in professional
interpreters: A review
Serena Ghiselli
(University of Bologna)
Interpreting is a complex cognitive task, for which an efficient working memory (WM) is of
paramount importance (Gile, 2009; Liu et al., 2004; Moser-Mercer, 2000). The tasks used in
WM standard assessment procedures may not be suitable to detect individual differences for
processes specifically involved in highly complex tasks, such as conference interpreting.
Empirical studies conducted so far have used a variety of methods and obtained mixed results,
which are probably the consequence of the different research designs adopted (Dong & Cai,
2015).
A common problem is the difficulty in finding a number of participants high enough to
reach statistical power when analyzing data. The choice of participants and the parameters used
to define interpreting expertise are other important aspects to take into account. The measures
of interpreting expertise are very diversified in the literature (García, 2014), from years of
experience to hours of practice, often without considering the quality of the interpreted text.
The same applies to the tools used to measure WM. There are various validated tests, from the
N-back task (Morales et al., 2015) to the reading span task (Padilla Benítez, 1995; Christoffels
et al., 2006; Zhang, 2008), the listening span task (Injoque-Ricle et al., 2015; Köpke &
Nespoulous, 2006; Liu et al., 2004; Liu, 2001) or the complex span task (Timarová et al., 2015);
other options are category and rhyme probe task (Köpke & Nespoulous, 2006), symmetry span
task and operation span task (Babcock & Vallesi, 2017).
The aim of the present paper is to compare the advantages and the potential limitations
of the various methods which have been used so far in the studies about WM in professional
interpreters. Data gathered using different study designs will be compared to draw a
comprehensive picture of data patterns. On the basis of the data collected and analyzed, a
proposal for the development of an ecologically sound methodological approach will be
exposed.
References:
Babcock, L., & Vallesi, A. (2017). Are simultaneous interpreters expert bilinguals, unique
bilinguals, or both? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(2), 403–417.
Christoffels, I. K., de Groot, A. M. B., & Kroll, J. F. (2006). Memory and language skills in
simultaneous interpreters: The role of expertise and language proficiency. Journal of
Memory and Language, 54(3), 324–345.
Dong, Y., & Cai, R. (2015). Working memory in interpreting: A commentary on theoretical
models. In E. W. Zhisheng, M. Borges Mota, & A. McNeill (Eds.), Working Memory in
Second Language Acquisition and Processing. Multilingual Matters.
46
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
García, A. M. (2014). The interpreter advantage hypothesis: Preliminary data patterns and
empirically motivated questions. Translation and Interpreting Studies. The Journal of the
American Translation and Interpreting Studies Association, 9(2), 219–238.
Gile, D. (2009). Basic Concepts and Models for Interpreter and Translator Training. John
Benjamins Publishing Company.
Injoque-Ricle, I., Barreyro, J. P., Formoso, J., & Jaichenco, V. I. (2015). Expertise, Working
Memory and Articulatory Suppression Effect: Their Relation with Simultaneous
Interpreting Performance. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 11(2), 56–63.
Köpke, B., & Nespoulous, J.-L. (2006). Working memory performance in expert and novice
interpreters. Interpreting, 8(1), 1–23.
Liu, M. (2001). Expertise in simultaneous interpreting: A working memory analysis [PhD]. The
University of Texas at Austin.
Liu, M., Schallert, D. L., & Carroll, P. J. (2004). Working memory and expertise in
simultaneous interpreting. Interpreting, 6(1), 19–42.
Morales, J., Padilla, F., Gómez-Ariza, C. J., & Bajo, M. T. (2015). Simultaneous interpretation
selectively influences working memory and attentional networks. Acta Psychologica, 155,
82–91.
Moser-Mercer, B. (2000). Simultaneous interpreting: Cognitive potential and limitations.
Interpreting, 5(2), 83–94.
Padilla Benítez, P. (1995). Procesos de memoria y atención en la interpretación de lenguas
[PhD]. Università di Granada.
Timarová, Š., Čeňková, I., Meylaerts, R., Hertog, E., Szmalec, A., & Duyck, W. (2015).
Simultaneous interpreting and working memory capacity. In A. Ferreira & J. W. Schwieter
(Eds.), Psycholinguistic and Cognitive Inquiries into Translation and Interpreting (Vol.
115, pp. 101–126). John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Zhang, W. (2008). A study of the effect of simultaneous interpreting on working memory’s
growth potential. Modern Foreign Languages, 31(4), 423–430.
Interpreting quality and effort in expert and novice interpreters
Anne Catherine Gieshoff
(ZHAW - Zurich University of Applied Sciences)
The question of what expertise in interpreting is has sparked numerous studies. These studies
do not only suggest that expert interpreters perform generally better than novices (Dillinger,
1990), but also that they are more successful in dealing with “problem triggers”, such as
complex sentence structures (Liu, Schallert, & Carroll, 2004), fast speech (Rosendo & Galván,
2019) or high information density (Hild, 2015). Gile’s Effort models suggest that the superiority
of experienced interpreters does not result from lower cognitive effort, but rather from a better
coordination of cognitive resources (Gile, 2009; Liu, 2009). The question whether expert
interpreters indeed find interpreting less effortful than novice interpreters, however, received
less attention. Ongoing data collection in the SNSF-funded CLINT project (Cognitive load in
interpreting and translation) allows us to address this question.
At the YMLP, I will present a first set of data of 7 professional and 7 student interpreters
for the investigation of the effect of expertise on interpreting. The participants, all German
47
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
native speakers, interpreted a speech from English to German. The source speech is an authentic
speech that was delivered at a conference on energy-related matters. It was recorded,
transcribed and re-spoken by a Canadian native speaker in order to obtain clear sound without
ambient noise. After interpretation, participants assessed the cognitive demands or the effort
they perceived to be involved in the task by means of the NASA-TXL (Hart & Staveland, 1988).
Based on previous studies, we expect to find higher interpreting quality in expert than
in novice interpreters but no difference between experts’ and novices’ effort ratings as predicted
by Gile’s Effort models. In order to triangulate interpreting quality ratings and participants’
effort ratings, we developed a new method for the quantitative assessment of sense consistency
and completeness of a target speech over time. Sense consistency and completeness, although
reflecting the multidimensional concept of interpreting quality only partially, are regarded by
interpreters (Tiselius, 2010; Zwischenberger, 2010) and users (Pradas Macías, 2006) as two
major aspects of interpreting quality. The newly designed method has been used on a first set
of data and will be presented alongside participants’ effort ratings.
References:
Dillinger, M. (1990). Comprehension during Interpreting: What Do Interpreters Know that
Bilinguals Don’t? The Interpreters’ Newsletter, 3, 41–58.
Gile, D. (2009). Basic concepts and models for interpreter and translator training (Rev. ed).
Amsterdam ; Philadelphia: John Benjamins Pub. Co.
Hart, S. G., & Staveland, L. E. (1988). Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results
of Empirical and Theoretical Research. In Advances in Psychology (Vol. 52, pp. 139–
183).
Hild, A. (2015). Discourse comprehension in simultaneous interpreting: The role of expertise
and redundancy. In A. Ferreira & J. W. Schwieter (Eds.), Benjamins Translation Library
(Vol. 115, pp. 67–100).
Liu, M. (2009). How do experts interpret? Implications from research in Interpreting Studies
and cognitive science. In G. Hansen, A. Chesterman, & H. Gerzymisch-Arbogast (Eds.),
Benjamins Translation Library (Vol. 80, pp. 159–177).
Liu, M., Schallert, D. L., & Carroll, P. J. (2004). Working memory and expertise in
simultaneous interpreting. Interpreting, 6(1), 19–42.
Pradas Macías, M. (2006). Probing quality criteria in simultaneous interpreting: The role of
silent pauses in fluency. Interpreting, 8(1), 25–43.
Rosendo, L. R., & Galván, M. C. (2019). Coping with speed: An experimental study on expert
and novice interpreter performance in the simultaneous interpreting of scientific
discourse. Babel. Revue Internationale de La Traduction / International Journal of
Translation, 65(1), 1–25.
Tiselius, E. (2010). A Sociological Perspective on Expertise in Conference Interpreting. A case
study on Swedish Conference Interpreters. In O. Azadibougar (Ed.), Translation Effects:
Selected Papers of the CETRA Research Seminar inTranslation Studies 2009 (pp. 1–24).
Leuven: CETRA.
Zwischenberger, C. (2010). Quality criteria in simultaneous interpreting: An international vs.
A national view. The Interpreters’ Newsletter, 15, 127–142.
48
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
The manifestations of language attrition in speech production
Sergei Gnitiev
(University of Pannonia)
One of the characteristics of spontaneous speech are the occurrences of disfluencies.
Hesitations are one type of disfluencies and are indications of the speaker's attentional
preoccupation at the macroplanning phase (Levelt, 1999). The presence of disfluencies in L1
(first language) speech signals that even monolinguals face problems in producing fluent
speech, which is more challenging when an additional language (L2) is present in the
speaker's mind. An increase in the number of disfluencies may indicate that the bilingual is
undergoing language attrition. A number of researchers (Gürel, 2002; Schmid, 2002; Schmid
& Jarvis, 2014; Laufer & Baladzhaeva, 2015) have reported the changes in L1 of those who
move to a new language environment. The present study focuses on the L1 attrition of
Russians living in Hungary with a special attention on speech production and disfluencies.
All participants (N = 18) have spent more than one year in Hungary, and their age ranges
from 22 to 72 years old. The primary objective of the study was to investigate the influence
of extralinguistic variables, such as age, education, frequency of use, length of residence, on
lexical and verbal fluency and observe changes in speech fluency. In order to gather data ont
he aforementioned factors, the Sociolinguistic and Personal Background Questionnaire
(SPBQ) (Keijzer, 2007) was used which focuses on four domains: personal background,
language choice, language contact and language attitude. The outcome variables were
fluency and lexical diversity and 2 instruments were used to collect speech production data.
The picture-book retelling task ( (Mercer Mayer "Frog, where are you?") elicited spontaneous
speech data which later was analysed for disfluencies and lexical diversity. Two versions of
the verbal fluency task was used: semantic verbal fluency with the category "animals" and
the letter fluency task with most frequent letter in the L1 alphabet. Together, these results
provide important insights into how speech production of attriters differs from monolinguals
and what are the main factors contributiong to the extent of attrition. The findings of the
present study coincides with previous studies that the immigrant group did significantly
worse in comparison with the control group.
References:
Gürel, A. (2002). Linguistic characteristics of second-language acquisition and first language
attrition. Turkish overt versus null pronouns [Unpublished doctoral dissertation].
Montreal: McGill University.
Keijzer, M. (2007). Last in first out? An investigation of the regression hypothesis in Dutch
emigrants in Anglophone Canada. [PhD dissertation], Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam.
Laufer, B. & Baladzhaeva, L. (2015). First language attrition without second language
acquisition; An exploratory study, ITL-International Journal of Applied Linguistics 166(2).
229-253.
Schmid, M. S. (2002). First language attrition, use and maintenance. The case of German Jews
in Anglophone countries. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Schmid M. S. (2004). First language attrition: The methodology revised. International Journal
of Bilingualism 8 (3): 2004; p. 239-255
Schmid, M. S., & Jarvis, S. (2014). Lexical access and lexical diversity in first language
attrition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 729– 748
49
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
“I believe in the power of transformation”: Coming-out stories of
non-binary identities in beauty vlogs on YouTube
Luisa Grabiger
(University of Greifswald)
The beauty and fashion community on YouTube is well-established and diverse. Members of
the Community of Practice (Wenger 2018) are constantly negotiating their (aesthetic) values,
attitudes, and beliefs. Several YouTubers have risen to prominence beyond the community and
gradually transformed into microcelebrities and influencers (Abidin 2018, Garcia-Rapp and
Roca-Cuberes 2017). A relatively recent development is an interest in genderless and genderfluid makeup, which gave rise to self-identified non-binary beauty influencers like Jeffrey Star,
Manny Mua, Nikki Tutorials, and James Charles. Scholarly attention to non-binary identity
construction on social media therefore seems all the more pressing (Hasmath and Cook 2013,
Oakley 2016, Abidin 2019). Also, YouTube as a fast-developing platform that enables
individuals to express themselves and simultaneously generate money has increased in
relevance (Burgess and Green 2018, Benson 2017). However, research that focuses particularly
on YouTube’s beauty genre and its promising opportunities, algorithmic restrictions, and
challenges for non-binary people seems to be still in its early stages (Raun 2018, Abidin 2019,
Homant and Sender 2019). In my ongoing PhD project, I aim to explore how non-binary beauty
vloggers construct and enact their gender identities and the relation with YouTube’s affordances
as a platform to sell personal stories and generate profit.
In the first part of my presentation, I want to elaborate on theoretical frameworks of
linguistic approaches to queer identities (Motschenbacher 2010, Livia and Hall 1997), (gender)
identity construction online (Marwick 2005), and on indexicality and stance-taking (Ochs 1997)
as analytical concepts for my PhD thesis. My literature review will then lead me to a case study.
I want to introduce a small specialised corpus of 5 coming-out stories of beauty influencers on
YouTube and the corresponding comment sections. I aim to investigate through which semiotic
resources non-binary beauty vloggers construct their gender identities and how gender,
influencer, and group identities intersect. The analysis draws on a sociolinguistic approach to
identity (Buchholtz 2005) and adopts a multimodal discourse analytic framework.
References:
Abidin, C. (2018). From Internet Celebrities to Influencers. Internet Celebrity: Understanding
Fame Online, 71-98.
Abidin, C. (2019). Yes Homo: Gay Influencers, homonormativity, and queerbaiting on
YouTube. Continuum, 33(5), 614-629.
Bucholtz, M. (2005). Identity and interaction: A sociocultural linguistic approach. Discourse
Studies, 7(4-5), 585-614.
Benson, P. (2017). Discourse of YouTube: Multimodal text in a global context. London and
New York: Routledge.
Bös, B., Kleinke, S., Mollin, S., & Hernández, N. (2019). The discursive construction of
identities on- and offline; personal, group and collective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Publishing Company.
Burgess, J., & Green, J. (2018). Youtube: Online video and participatory culture. Cambridge,
UK: Polity Press.
Bucholtz, M. (2005). Identity and interaction: A sociocultural
linguistic approach. Discourse Studies, 7(4-5), 585-614.
50
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
García-Rapp, F., & Roca-Cuberes, C. (2017). Being an online celebrity: Norms and
expectations of YouTube’s beauty community. First Monday.
Hasmath, R., & Cook, J. (2013). The Discursive Construction and Performance of Gendered
Identity on Social Media. SSRN Electronic Journal.
Homant, E., & Sender, K. (2019). Queer Immaterial Labor in Beauty Videos by LGBTQIdentified YouTubers. International Journal of Communication, 13, 5386-5404.
Livia, A., & Hall, K. (1997). Queerly phrased: Language, gender, and sexuality. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Marwick, A. E. (2005). Selling your self: Online identity in the age of a commodified Internet.
Seattle: University of Washington.
Motschenbacher, H. (2010). Language, gender and sexual identity: Poststructuralist
perspectives. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
NikkieTutorials. (2020, January 28). Responding To My Coming Out. Retrieved December 20,
2020, from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=AgKLxQmONgM
Oakley, A. (2016). Disturbing Hegemonic Discourse: Nonbinary Gender and Sexual
Orientation Labeling on Tumblr. Social Media + Society, 2(3), 205630511666421.
Ochs, Ellinor. (1997). Indexing Gender. In A. Duranti (Author), Rethinking context: Language
as an interactive phenomenon (pp. 335-358). Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press.
Raun, T. (2018). Capitalizing intimacy. Convergence: The International Journal of Research
into New Media Technologies, 24(1), 99-113.
Wenger, E. (2018). Communities of practice: Learning, meaning, and identity. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
The knowledge of non-compositional expressions (idioms) of
Heritage Russian speakers and L2 Russian learners
Ekaterina Gridneva, Maria Grabovskaya, Anastasia Ivanenko, Nina Ladinskaya and Nina
Zdorova
(National Research University Higher School of Economics)
The current study examines the knowledge of non-compositional expressions (idioms) of
Heritage Russian speakers comparing the results with L2 Russian learners. On the one hand,
heritage language is a weaker one which makes Heritage Russian speakers close to L2 learners.
On the other hand, heritage speakers are bilinguals and their language acquisition takes place
in a family which is similar to native speakers. Basically, knowledge of idioms always
differentiates native speakers. As a consequence, idioms’ awareness can be a marker which
shows on which level heritage speakers are: native speakers or L2. We examine idioms’
knowledge by using a self-paced reading test. First, we divided our idioms in accordance with
a cross-linguistic factor: there are Russian - English full equivalents, semi-equivalents and noequivalents. Based on the reading time data, Matlock and Heredia suggested that less advanced
L2 learners first translated the L2 idiom into L1 and then attempted to make sense of it before
they accessed its figurative meaning. At a more advanced level of L2, it was hypothesized that
learners might process idiomatic expressions by directly retrieving their figurative meaning,
consistent with the Direct Access Model proposed by Gibbs for monolingual language users
(1980). Additionally, we take into account idioms’ usage frequency (for this purpose The
Russian National Corpus was used) plus their figurative meaning level.
51
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Boers, F., Demecheleer, M., & Eyckmans, J. (2004). Cross-cultural variation as a variable in
comprehending and remembering figurative idioms. European Journal of English Studies,
8(3), 375-388.
Bortfeld, H. (2003). Comprehending idioms cross-linguistically. Experimental Psychology,
50(3), 217.
Cieślicka, A. B., Heredia, R. R., & Olivares, M. (2014). It’s all in the eyes: How language
dominance, salience, and context affect eye movements during idiomatic language
processing. In Essential topics in applied linguistics and multilingualism (pp. 21-41).
Springer, Cham.
Gibbs, R. W. (1980). Spilling the beans on understanding and memory for idioms in
conversation. Memory & cognition, 8(2), 149-156.
Kopotev, M., Kisselev, O., & Polinsky, M. (2018). Collocations and near-native competence:
Lexical strategies of heritage speakers of Russian. International Journal of Bilingualism,
1367006920921594.
Matlock, T., & Heredia, R. R. (2002). 11 Understanding phrasal verbs in monolinguals and
bilinguals. In Advances in psychology (Vol. 134, pp. 251-274). North-Holland.
Polinsky, M., & Kagan, O. (2007). Heritage languages: In the ‘wild’and in the classroom.
Language and linguistics compass, 1(5), 368-395.
Rakhilina, E., Vyrenkova, A., & Polinsky, M. (2016). Linguistic creativity in heritage speakers.
Glossa, 1(1), 1.
Tabossi, P., Fanari, R., & Wolf, K. (2008). Processing idiomatic expressions: Effects of
semantic compositionality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 34(2), 313.
On the debonding of the Polish intensifying adjectival prefix przeDamian Herda
(Jagiellonian University)
Based on empirical data derived mainly from Internet sources, this paper aims to provide an
account of a relatively recent non-standard development of the Polish intensifying prefix prze,
which originally emerged as a result of the grammaticalization of the Proto-Slavonic
preposition *per ‘through’ (cf. SEJP, pp. 487–488). Attached to gradable adjectives, the prefix
intensifies the quality denoted by the derivational base (Szymanek 2010: 123), as in przemiły
‘extremely nice’ (< miły ‘nice’) or przeokropny ‘extremely terrible’ (< okropny ‘terrible’). In
present-day colloquial Polish, the formative has additionally extended its distribution to nouns
invoking gradable properties (cf. MSSiMP), such as kozak ‘someone brave; lit.: Cossack’ (cf.
przekozak ‘someone very brave’) or zabawa ‘fun’ (cf. przezabawa ‘great fun’). Moreover, the
element has developed free uses as an evaluative adjective (1) and adverb (2).
(1)
Ja czytam zazwyczaj kryminały szczególnie te Agathy Christie są po prostu prze.
‘I usually read detective stories, especially those by Agatha Christie are just fantastic.’
[https://www.fcbarca.com/la-rambla/dyskusja-4283680]
(2)
Wygląda prze ;) Byle do wypłaty i idzie przelew :)
‘Looks great ;) Once I get my salary, I’ll make a bank transfer :)’
52
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
[https://forums.cdprojektred.com/index.php?threads/ruszyla-przedsprzedaz-grywojna-krwi-wiedzminskie-opowiesci.10984859/]
However, whereas the bound element’s further distributional expansion is characteristic
of advancing grammaticalization, the latter change runs counter to a typical grammaticalization
scenario (cf. Hopper & Traugott 2003: 1). It will be argued that rather than constituting an
instance of simple clipping, the rise of an independent item out of the prefix prze- exemplifies
degrammaticalization, more specifically debonding in the sense of Norde (2009). First of all,
while clippings are instantaneous (Brinton & Traugott 2005: 85), degrammaticalization occurs
gradually. As for prze-, the intermediate stage between affixhood and free word status arguably
consisted in the formative’s phonetic strengthening and semantic enrichment. In other words,
the free item prze does not function as a hypernym of all potential adjectival prze-formations.
Instead, the erstwhile prefix has become lexically infused with the positivity of its most frequent
adjectival bases, as suggested by the fact that in the comprehensive Universal Dictionary of
Polish (USJP), a vast majority of entrenched adjectives prefixed by the intensifier prze- exhibit
a positive coloring.
References:
Brinton, L. J. & Traugott, E. C. (2005). Lexicalization and Language Change. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Hopper, P. J. & Traugott, E. C. (2003). Grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
MSSiMP = Miejski słownik slangu i mowy potocznej. Retrieved from https://www.miejski.pl.
Norde, M. (2009). Degrammaticalization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
SEJP = Boryś, W. (2005). Słownik etymologiczny języka polskiego. Kraków: Wydawnictwo
Literackie.
Szymanek, B. (2010). A Panorama of Polish Word-Formation. Lublin: Wydawnictwo KUL.
USJP = Dubisz, S. (ed.). (2008). Uniwersalny słownik języka polskiego: P–Ś. Warszawa:
Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.
Social Network Analysis as a window onto Second Language
Acquisition
Magdalena Jelińska (University of Warsaw), Michał B. Paradowski (University of Warsaw),
Andrzej Jarynowski (Interdisciplinary Research Institute in Wrocław), Karolina Czopek
(University of Warsaw) and Agnieszka Cierpich (Ignatianum Jesuit University in Kraków)
Social networks play an important role in the behaviour and attainment of individuals. This
study investigates how interactions with peer L2 learners catalyse or inhibit second language
acquisition, and constitutes one of the first applications of computational social network
analysis (SNA) to investigating the phenomenon in unregulated conversational interaction.
Unlike some previous notable studies (Dewey et al., 2012; 2013; Zappa‐Hollman & Duff, 2015;
Gautier, 2019), we do not limit the scope of enquiry to individual (ego-)networks, which only
investigate the links between the individual and her/his alters, but we set out to examine the full
L2 learner network in its entirety, where knowledge of the links between (almost) all network
members enables a reconstruction of the connected social graph of the complete learner group.
53
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
In a sample of participants in two editions of a 4-week-long intensive summer course of
the Polish language and culture (n=332), we find that peer learner networks can have both a
positive and a negative impact on L2 acquisition. Among others:
i)
a positive predictor of L2 improvement is reciprocal out-of-class interactions in the
language being acquired,
ii)
outgoing interactions in the L2 are a stronger predictor than incoming interactions,
iii)
there exists a clear negative relationship between performance and interactions with
same-L1 speakers,
iv)
there is a clear negative relationship between L2 performance and weighted indegree centrality in total communication,
v)
fluency in lingua-franca English tends to significantly impede progress in the (nonEnglish) L2.
While the link between social relations and language acquisition has been universally
acknowledged by SLA scholars, social network analysis offers not only a novel methodology,
but a whole new insight into the language learning process, demonstrating how network
structure and the dynamics of interaction are stronger predictors of TL performance than many
individual factors such as attitude or motivation. The findings may deliver practical
recommendations for language teachers as to how, by first identifying students’ and groups’
communication profiles and social interaction patterns, and subsequently seeking ways to
reinforce and promote advantageous educational behaviours while discouraging or inhibiting
deleterious ones, they could aid learners in their quest to acquire the L2.
References:
Dewey, D. P., Belnap, R. K., & Hillstrom, R. (2013). Social network development, language
use, and language acquisition during study abroad: Arabic language learners’
perspectives. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad, 22(1), 84–110.
Dewey, D. P., Bown, J., & Eggett, D. (2012). Japanese language proficiency, social networking,
and language use during study abroad: Learners’ perspectives. Canadian Modern
Language Review, 68(2), 111–137.
Gautier, R. (2019). Understanding socialisation and integration through social network
analysis: American and Chinese students during a stay abroad. In M. Howard (Ed.),
Study abroad, second language acquisition and interculturality (pp. 207–236).
Multilingual Matters.
Zappa-Hollman, S., & Duff, P. A. (2015). Academic English socialization through individual
networks of practice. TESOL Quarterly, 49(2), 333–368.
Language and linguistics teaching and learning during COVID-19induced school closures: Predictors of teachers’ and students’
success
Magdalena Jelińska (University of Warsaw), Michał B. Paradowski (University of Warsaw)
and Andrzej Jarynowski (Interdisciplinary Research Institute in Wrocław)
We present the findings of a global longitudinal study (involving over 6,000 participants from
118 countries) investigating how language teachers and learners as well as linguistics
54
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
instructors and students have been handling the 2020 transition to emergency remote
instruction.
We begin by revealing easily interpretable clusters of naturally correlating variables
(Fig. 1). Crucially, the giant component of the four highly interconnected clusters associated
with i) self-regulation/leadership-organisation potential, ii) engagement/openness, iii) positive
orientation and iv) social skills/contacts (left hand-side of the graph) has a predominantly
positive valence, while the three peripheral clusters related to v) family relationships, vi) future
expectations and vii) remote instruction-related experiences and perspectives on students’
coping (right hand-side) are mainly negative. We also identify clusters of better- and worsecoping teachers and learners, as well as the following meaningful distinguishing features:
preparedness level and support received, effectiveness and engagement in using new
technologies, perception of students’ coping, logistic problems, and general positive orientation
in the case of the educators, and motivation, engagement in the learning process, difficulties
with staying focused, concern regarding the assessment of in-class activity, the teachers’ ability
to meet individual/special needs, initial confidence in the ability to learn remotely, general
attitudes towards distance teaching, and interaction with the teacher and classmates in the
learners’ population.
The findings offer valuable guidelines for the current context of the second wave of
the pandemic, as well as help language teachers and learners as well as stakeholders outside
the narrow context of distance learning.
A feminist stylistic analysis of Anita Nair’s “Ladies Coupé” and
Shashi Deshpande’s “That Long Silence”
Roniya Jose and Shahila Zafar
(Central University of Punjab)
The study employs feminist stylistics approach proposed by Sara Mills to explore the
representation of women characters in Indian novelists, Anita Nair’s Ladies Coupe (2001) and
55
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Shashi Deshpande’s That Long Silence (1989). The analysis is carried out at the levels of word,
sentence/ phrase, and discourse, with the data collected from the selected texts. The theory is
equipped in analyzing gender differentiation, silence and subjugation of women with respect to
the culture and traditions of the dominated society in terms of words, phrases and
sentences. The study is carried out by identifying dominant and recurring features of
characterization, and by foregrounding these recurring patterns in the novels of Indian woman
writers, Anita Nair and Shashi Deshpande. This resulted in finding significant differences
between representations of the women and men in the selected texts in the level of words,
phrases and sentences.
The study focuses on specific nouns used to refer to male and female characters, as well
as the differences in naming the characters which illustrate the gender-based power structures
that exist. The texts are analysed with lexical items indicating gender specific words referred to
men in general, their overall appearance. Additionally, gender-specific lexical items are
analyzed and the transitivity processes are explored. A discourse level analysis identifies the
various types of unfairness projected and roles attributed to women by men as well as by
society. It is apparent that the authors had opened the way to a new point that operates with free
will of female identity in their stories, using new content and style different from the traditional
concepts, standing against the internalized submission of woman by the masculine power. It is
clearly observed from the study that the authors have tried to subvert the established ideologies
and stereotyping on gender roles; with a feminist interest as evident from the protagonists of
the novels. The study discusses the linguistic structures and their effects on gender
representation.
References:
Abrams, M. H., & Harpham, G. G. (2009). Handbook of literary terms (VI ed.). New Delhi,
India: Cengage Learning.
Adams, R. (2019, January 04). Michel Foucault: Discourse. Retrieved April 14, 2020, from
https://criticallegalthinking.com/2017/11/17/michel-foucault-discourse/
Barry, P. (2002). Beginning theory: An introduction to literary and cultural theory. Manchester:
Manchester University Press.
C. (2014). Gender and disharmony in Shashi Deshpande’s that long silence. International
Journal of English and Literature, 5(4), 97-100.
Deshpande, S. (1989). That long silence. New Delhi, India: Penguin Books.
Klarer, M. (2013). An introduction to literary studies (III ed.). London: Routledge.
Manjula, S., & Saburunnisa, A. (2016). “Quest For Identity: A Feministic Approach of Female
Characters in Anita Nair’s Ladies Coupe.” International Journal of English Language,
Literature and Translation Studies, 474-477.
Mills, S (1995). Feminist Stylistics. New York: Routledge.
Nair, A. (2003). Ladies coupe (II ed.). Gurgaon, Haryana, India: Penguin Random House.
Acoustic analysis of Chinese plosives among Hungarian learners of
Mandarin Chinese
Kornélia Juhász
(Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest)
In this study we analyse the production of (Mandarin) Chinese and Hungarian plosives in
Hungarian learners compared to native Chinese speakers.
56
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
It is known that the L1 phonological system influences the acquisition of L2 speech
sounds (Flege, 1987). While in Hungarian, voiced unaspirated [b d g] and voiceless unaspirated
[p t k] are contrasted phonologically (Gósy, 2009), in Chinese, voiceless unaspirated [b̥ d̥ g̥]
and voiceless aspirated [pʰ tʰ kʰ] plosives are in opposition (Chao, 1968; Chen et al, 2007).
Due to the differences outlined above, we expect that Hungarian speakers transfer the
phonetic implementation of their L1 voicing contrast to the production of the contrasing
Chinese plosives. Thus we hypothesise that Hungarian learners of Chinese produce voiceless
unaspirated Chinese plosives more voiced than Chinese speakers; while their voiceless
aspirated Chinese segments are less aspirated than those of native Chineses’.
We analysed the above mentioned 12 plosives of Hungarian and Chinese in isolated
two-syllable (CVC structured) pseudowords. We compared two groups of Hungarian learners
of Chinese (with different levels of language competence) with Chinese natives (5 speaker per
group). Firstly, we analysed voicing-in-closure (VIC) of unaspirated plosives (either voiced or
voiceless) as a percentage value: we measured the duration of the voiced closure (if existed),
which we divided by the total duration of the closure. Secondly, we analysed aspiration: we
measured the duration between the start of the release and the onset of the following vowel.
Data were submitted to statistical analysis (linear mixed effects models).
We found no statistically significant differences between Hungarian and Chinese
speakers’ Chinese sounds neither in VIC, nor in aspiration. However, (not significant)
differences between groups showed the expected tendencies in voicing patterns, namely
Hungarians produced Chinese unaspirated voiceless plosives with higher VIC, and they
produced aspirated plosives with less aspiration than Chinese natives. We also found that
Hungarian speakers differentiated unaspirated voiceless Chinese plosives from Hungarian
voiced plosves, as well as aspirated Chinese plosives from unaspirated voiceless Hungarian
segments. Therefore, we concluded that they were able to produce native-like Chinese plosives
irrespective of their level of competence.
References:
Chao, Y. R. (1968). A Grammar of Spoken Chinese. University of California Press.
Chen, L. M., Chao, K. Y. & Peng, J. F. (2007). VOT productions of word-initial stops in
Mandarin and English: a cross-language study. Proceedings of the 19th Conference on
Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing, 303-317.
Flege, J. E. (1987). Effects of equivalence classification on the production of foreign language
speech sounds. In James, A. & Leather, J. (eds.) Sound patterns in second language
acquisition. Foris, 9-39.
Gósy, M. & Ringen, C. (2009). Everything you always wanted to know about VOT (in
Hungarian). Talk presented at the Ninth International Conference on the Structure of
Hungarian, Budapest, 1. September 2009.
Translation trainees' emotional intelligence and its impact on the
use of stylistic devices in the translation process
Magdalena Jurczenia
(University of Białystok)
Emotional Intelligence gets progressively visible, in particular in the field of translation studies.
This is due to the fact that research in this area has only recently been launched. The concept
of Emotional Intelligence has a significant impact on our daily lives as well as on our decisions
57
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
(see Triphaty 2018). Nevertheless, this type of intelligence does not always apply to our
emotions (see Goleman 1997). This can also be seen in the work of a translator, where
Emotional Intelligence can influence the process of literary translation as well as his/her sense
of job satisfaction (see Hubscher-Davidson 2016). Still, there are cases where Emotional
Intelligence has nothing in common when it comes to the work of translators, in particular with
relation to the quality of translation (see Verzande and Jadidi 2015, Çoban 2019).
The aim of my study was to find a correlation between Emotional Intelligence and the
usage of stylistic devices in the process of literary translation. This study was conducted on a
group of students who study English Philology and Elements of Translation Studies at the
University of Białystok. The study required conducting a special test for Emotional Intelligence
(TEIQue-SF) and translating a fragment of a novel from English into Polish. The participants
of the study had to fill in the questionnaire and then translate into Polish an extract describing
nature from Lucy Maud Montgomery's novel “Anne of Green Gables”. The level of Emotional
Intelligence of the participants had to be checked and for this, a special program that is available
on the TEIQue website was used. Following this, the stylistic devices used by the participants
were analysed. The results of the study allowed to answer the question of whether Emotional
Intelligence in translation students has any impact on their usage of stylistic devices in the
process literary translation.
References:
Tripathy, M. 2018. Emotional Intelligence: An Overview. LAP LAMBERT Academic
Publishing
Goleman, D. 1997.”Inteligencja Emocjonalna”. Media Rodzina of Poznań
Hubscher-Davidson, S. 2016. ”Trait Emotional intelligence and translation. A study of
professional translators” in Target. John Benjamins Publishing Company
Varzande, M, & Jadidi, E. 2015.”The Effect of Translators’ Emotional Intelligence on Their
Translation Quality” in English Language Teaching; Vol. 8, No. 8. Canadian Center of
Science and Education
Fadime, Ç. 2019. ”The Relationship between Professional Translators’s Emotional Intelligence
and theı̇ r Translator Satisfaction” in International Journal of Comparative Literature &
Translation Studies Volume: 7 Issue: 3. Australian International Academic Centre.
The power of indirectness.: Raising L2 pragmatic awareness of
adult Polish EFL learners at A2 level
Emilia Jurewicz
(University of Warsaw)
This presentation investigates the effectiveness of the pragmatic approach in teaching English
as a foreign language in Poland to a relatively challenging group being older adults at low level
of proficiency (A2). The teaching of pragmatics, defined as the study of language from a
functional perspective (Levinson, 1983) and the study of how more gets communicated than is
said (Yule, 1996), aims at enhancing the learners’ ability to find socially appropriate language
for the situations they may encounter.
Researchers emphasize that no considerable gain in pragmatic competence can be
achieved without explicit teaching of pragmatic rules, especially when learning in a foreign
language context (Szczepaniak- Kozak, 2016, Cohen, 2020). Yet, the teaching of pragmatics is
still often overlooked in the foreign language classrooms. Most L2 course books in their
58
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
presentation of speech acts present learners only with a list of their different linguistic
realizations („useful phrases”) but without guiding learners in how to choose a linguistic
strategy in order to express the speech act appropriately. While speech acts themselves are
universal, the forms used in greetings, leave-takings and other speech acts are very much
language and culture bound. In the Slavic culture, invitations, advice and suggestions are often
imposed on the interlocutor by the use of the imperative and multiple repetitions (Wierzbicka,
1991), while in the Anglo-American culture it is common to avoid such imposition in these
speech acts by the use of indirect structures, hints and preliminary questions.
This talk describes the research design and procedures of a study of pragmatic
development conducted in a language school in Warsaw, Poland over the period of 16 weeks
in the school year 2019-2020. Results of the study showed that despite their general higher
proficiency in EFL, Polish learners at B2 level who did not receive pragmatic instruction
demonstrated lower pragmatic competence than learners at A2 level, who received pragmatic
instruction. The presentation concludes by considering the implications of the study for further
research.
References:
Cohen, A. (2020). Considerations in assessing pragmatic appropriateness in spoken language.
Language Teaching, 53(2), 183-202
Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Szczepaniak-Kozak, A. (2016). Developmental trends in requests rendered by EFL speakers in
Poland. Konin Language Studies.
Wierzbicka, A. (1991). Cross-cultural Pragmatics: The Semantics of Human Interaction.
Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Yule, G. (1996). Pragmatics. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Shifting notions of otherness: Identity construction of GermanTurkish descendants in Germany
Yesim Kakalic
(University of Warwick)
The German media landscape is characterised by discourses of ethnic tension, migration,
integration and assimilation in relation to German-Turkish individuals (Mueller, 2006; Ramm,
2009; Schneider, 2001) and has led to a stereotypical and negative public image of this very
group (Mora, 2009; Ramm, 2010). Such discourses typically construct and portray GermanTurkish individuals as the other, ultimately intensifying discrimination (Bonfadelli, 2007) and
feelings of alienation. This paper aims to understand how mainstream discourses of GermanTurkish individuals contribute to this othering and how they thereby influence the selfperceptions and identity constructions of German-Turks, who often find themselves torn
between positioning themselves as Germans, Turks or German-Turks. This paper thus aims to
answer the following Research Question: How do German-Turkish descendants construct their
identities as Germans, Turks and German-Turks in and through the processes of othering?
Drawing on over 13 hours of audio- and video recorded focus group discussions with
German-Turkish adolescents, this paper analyses and discusses narratives of discrimination
which emerged as one of the main themes during the coding process. More specifically, it
explores the discursive and pragmatic processes through which German-Turks construct and
59
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
negotiate their own (and others’) multiple identities by othering either “the Germans” or “the
Turks”, and positioning themselves in relation to these larger groups – sometimes embracing
and sometimes rejecting membership in them.
Findings illustrate that what is considered to be “the other” by participants is constantly
shifting throughout these processes of identity construction and negotiation. This highly
dynamic nature of otherness is closely intertwined with issues of social integration and
discrimination, which participants experience on a daily basis. Through analysing the pragmatic
and discursive processes of this othering, this paper provides important insights of how
mainstream and stereotypical discourses of German-Turks affect the identity construction of
members of this group, and provides empirical evidence of how social integration takes place.
References:
Bonfadelli, H. (2007). Die Darstellung ethnischer Minderheiten in den Massenmedien. In H.
Bonfadelli and H. Moser (Eds.), Medien und Migration. Europa als multikultureller
Raum (pp. 95 116). Wiesbaden: VS Verlag.
Mora, N. (2009). Turkey and Turks in the German media. Journal of Human Sciences, 6(2),
606-625.
Mueller, C. (2006). Integrating Turkish communities: a German dilemma. Population research
and policy review, 25(5-6), 419-441.
Ramm, C. (2009). The ‘sick man’ beyond Europe: the orientalization of Turkey and Turkish
immigrants in European Union accession discourses in Germany. In: Huggan, G. and
Law, I. (eds). Racism Postcolonialism Europe (pp. 102-115). Liverpool: Liverpool
University Press.
Ramm, C. (2010). The Muslim makers: how Germany ‘islamizes’ Turkish immigrants.
Interventions, 12(2), 183-197.
Schneider, J. (2001). Talking German: othering strategies in public and everyday discourses.
Gazette (Leiden, Netherlands), 63(4), 351-363.
Deaf learners and (foreign) language exams
Krystian Kamiński
(Maria Curie-Skłodowska University, Lublin)
In the Polish educational system each of the educational stages is completed with an exam.
Because English and Polish are compulsory subjects, at the end of each educational stage
learners take the obligatory exam in these fields, which also applies to learners with various
disabilities. Therefore, most exam sheets prepared by the Central Examination Board
(Centralna Komisja Egzaminacyjna) take into account the needs of learners with disabilities
and specific learning difficulties. Adapting the exam sheet shall involve changing the type of
tasks and adjusting them to the capabilities of a given group of learners. The main purpose of
such exams is to check to what extent graduates meet the requirements set out in the General
Education Core Curriculum. Thus, the language exams in Polish and English contain tasks that
relate directly to all requirements of the general educational objectives of the General Education
Core Curriculum.
The main aim of the presentation is to demonstrate the results of research conducted
among foreign language teachers working with d/Deaf learners. It was focused on examining
the language acquisition of the d/Deaf in the Polish educational system. The paper ponders upon
characteristics of Polish and English language exams for d/Deaf learners. As the examined
60
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
teachers report, English language exams are adapted to the capabilities of d/Deaf learners by
omitting listening comprehension, enabling sign language interpreter to translate the tasks into
the Polish sign language, or increasing the exam time. However, the obstacle in the case of
Polish language exams is the structure of the test, as it treats the d/Deaf as native speakers of
this language. The interviewed teachers of Polish claim that this subject should be regarded as
a foreign language, and the form of the exam shall be adapted for people who do not use it at a
native level. The paper presents the teacher’s opinion on exams adaptations and suggests
possible aspects to be improved.
References:
Al-Khamisy, D. (2013). Edukacja włączająca edukacją dialogu. W poszukiwaniu modelu
edukacji dla ucznia ze specjalnymi potrzebami edukacyjnymi. Warszawa:
Wydawnictwo Akademii Pedagogiki Specjalnej.
Dłużniewska, A. (2011). Model pracy z uczniem niesłyszącym lub słabo słyszącym. In J.
Wrona (ed.), Podniesienie efektywności kształcenia uczniów ze specjalnymi potrzebami
edukacyjnymi. Materiały szkoleniowe. Część 2. (pp. 90-129). Warszawa: Ministerstwo
Edukacji Narodowej.
Doleżalova, M. (2013). An English Quest: an art of teaching English to the deaf and hard-ofhearing students. In E. Domagała-Zyśk (ed.), English as a Foreign Language for the
Deaf and Hard of Hearing Persons in Europe (pp. 153-162). Lublin: Wydawnictwo
KUL.
Domagała-Zyśk, E. (2013). Written English of Polish deaf and hard of hard of hearing grammar
school students. In E. Domagała-Zyśk (ed.), English as a Foreign Language for the
Deaf and Hard of Hearing Persons in Europe (pp. 163-180). Lublin: Wydawnictwo
KUL.
Domagała-Zyśk, E. (2018). Dostosowanie podstawy programowej w zakresie języka obcego
do potrzeb uczniów z dysfunkcją słuchu. Języki Obce w Szkole 4/2018, 5-10.
Dotter, F. (2013). What language development in deaf and hard of hearing children should look
like? In E. Domagała-Zyśk (ed.), English as a Foreign Language for the Deaf and Hard
of Hearing Persons in Europe (pp. 27-45). Lublin: Wydawnictwo KUL.
Gulati, B. (2013). Deaf students and English – the art of teaching and learning. In E. DomagałaZyśk (ed.), English as a Foreign Language for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Persons
in Europe (pp. 207-215). Lublin: Wydawnictwo KUL.
Informator o egzaminie ósmoklasisty z języka angielskiego od roku szkolnego 2018/2019 dla
uczniów słabosłyszących i niesłyszących. (2017). Warszawa: Centralna Komisja
Egzaminacyjna.
Informator o egzaminie ósmoklasisty z języka polskiego od roku szkolnego 2018/2019 dla
uczniów słabosłyszących i niesłyszących. (2017). Warszawa: Centralna Komisja
Egzaminacyjna.
Janicka, M. (2016). Nauczyciele języków obcych wobec różnorodności w klasie językowej
oraz wyzwań dydaktyki włączającej. Języki Obce w Szkole 4/2016, 39-46.
Pater, M. (2018). Komunikacja, czyli co jest najważniejsze na egzaminie ósmoklasisty. Języki
Obce w Szkole 4/2018, 115-120.
Skibska, J. (2014). Uczeń z wadą słuchu w młodszym wieku szkolnym w szkole
ogólnodostępnej – problemy i ich rozwiązania. In: J. Skibska (ed.), Dziecko z wadą
słuchu oraz Centralnymi Zaburzeniami Przetwarzania Słuchowego (CAPD). Wybrane
problemy (pp. 27-43). Kraków: Wydawnictwo LIBRON.
61
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Smolik, M., Poszytek, P. (2018). Podstawa programowa kształcenia ogólnego w zakresie
języka obcego nowożytnego w wychowaniu przedszkolnym i ośmioletniej szkole
podstawowej – słowo komentarza. Języki Obce w Szkole 1/2017, 70-82.
Szczepankowski, B. (1999). Niesłyszący – głusi – głuchoniemi. Wyrównywanie szans.
Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne i Pedagogiczne.
Szczepkowska, K. (2019). Edukacja włączająca w szkole – szanse i wyzwania. Warszawa:
Ośrodek Rozwoju Edukacji.
Topicalization and focus formation in a Hungarian lovari idiolect
Krisztina Karácsonyi
(University of Pécs)
Hungarian lovari is a Romani dialect, which language uses the differential object marking
(DOM) (Baló 2013, Elšík 2000, 2020, Matras 2011). If the direct object is an animate noun, it
usually shows the unmodified oblique (accusative) form, but inanimate direct objects usually
have a nominal case (table 1).
Animate
Inanimate
Subject
grašt (NOM)
kher (NOM)
Direct Object
grašt-es (ACC)
kher (NOM)
Table 1: Differential Object Marking in Romani
In the winter of 2019 and in the summer of 2020, I conducted a participatory observation
in a Hungarian village in Baranya county (Bicsérd). During that, I have observed that in some
cases, speakers do, but in many cases, do not use suffixes expressing the subject while they use
animate nouns.
(1)
Bit’in- dem
o
buy- Past.1Sg. a.Masc
I sold the dog.
žuk- les/ žukel
dog-ACC/ dog
In my presentation, I will show word order variations related to the topicalization and
identification-exclusionary focus of marked and unmarked direct objects, based on the common
method of generative linguistic work: the introspection of a native speaker. That in the language
of the examined narrow group the animate, originally marked, and the animate but unmarked
constructions are also present, causes some word order restrictions. The languages flexible
(S)VO word order characteristic (Matras 2002 , Matras - Tenser 2016) becomes more rigid,
thus often preventing the possibility of topicalization. Presumably, unmarked animate nouns
have indefinite, non-specific meanings, and that is why they cannot be topicalized.
(2a)
O
žukles
a.Masc dof- ACC
I sold the dog.
bikin- dem.
buy- Past.1Sg
62
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
(2a’)
*O
žukel bikin- dem.
a.Masc dog-Ø buy- Past.1Sg
I sold the dog. [Not aviable meaning.]
I also examined object-meaning constructs with a ferí (‘just’) identification-exclusion
particle. In doing so, I compared the pre- and postverbal position of the marked and unmarked
components. Based on this, it can be said that the components with the marked nouns can be
located pre- and postverbally in the sentence, whereas constructions that also are animate but
are unmarked too, result in an agrammatical sentence in the postverbial place (cf. (3a) and (3a).
') -t (3a' ')). Those word order variations are presumably also related to the specificity/nonspecificity of the object.
References:
Baló András Márton (2013): A lovári főnévi rendszer analógia alapú megközelítése. In.:
Romológia „akkor és most”. PTE BTK NTI, Pécs. 23 – 46.
Elšík, Viktor (2000): Romani nominal paradigms: their structure, diversity, and development.
In: Elšík, Viktor – Matras, Yaron (eds.): Grammatical relations in Romani: The noun
phrase. John Benjamins, Amszterdam. 9 – 30.
Elšík, Viktor (2020): Romani Morphology. In.: Matras, Yaron – Tenser, Anton (eds.): The
Palgrave Handbook of Romani Language and Linguistics, Springer, Cham, Switzerland,
155 – 186.
Matras, Yaron (2002): Romani. A Linguistics Introduction. Cambridge University Press, 72 –
112.
Matras, Yaron (2011): RomIdent Woking Papers. Paper No. 10. Romani. Hozzáférés:
http://romani.humanities.manchester.ac.uk/virtuallibrary/librarydb/web/files/pdfs/365/p
aper10.pdf, 2020. 12. 12.
Matras, Yaron – Tenser, Anton (2016): Complementizers in Romani. In: Boye, Kasper –
Kehayov, Petar (eds.): Complementizer semantics in European languages, Mouton de
Gruyter, Berlin, 341 – 375.
Bilingualism and creativity: The effects of bilingual experiences of
interpreters and translators on creative thinking
Michal Korenar
(University of Reading)
Mounting evidence suggests that bilinguals have a creative advantage compared to
monolinguals in that bilinguals score higher on tests measuring respondents’ ability to generate
creative ideas by exploring many possible solutions (divergent thinking) or tests for which they
need to bring together different ideas to find a solution to a problem (convergent thinking) (for
review see van Dijk et al., 2019). However, not all previous studies found positive effects of
bilingualism on creativity (e.g., Lange et al., 2020). A possible reason for the discrepancies is
a lack of assessment of specific bilingual language use patterns and inhibitory skills that
influence bilinguals’ creativity. In the current study, we aim to investigate the relative
contribution of specific bilingual experiences on divergent and convergent thinking skills by
focusing on interpreters and translators, who need strongly developed linguistic and inhibitory
skills in their jobs. Based on Hommel et al. (2011), we expected the interpreters to outperform
63
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
the translators and a group of non-professional bilinguals in inhibition and convergent thinking,
but to score lower on divergent thinking tasks. We tested three groups of Czech-English
bilinguals: interpreters (n = 29), translators (n = 37) and bilinguals without professional
experience (n = 47). The participants completed a divergent thinking (ATTA) and a convergent
thinking task (RAT), an inhibitory control task (Eriksen flanker task), an intelligence test
(WAIS-III), a language proficiency test (Lextale), and questionnaires measuring participants'
specific use of and exposure to both languages. Contrary to our expectations, the interpreters
outperformed the regular bilinguals not only on the intelligence and language proficiency tasks,
but also on the divergent thinking task. The groups did not differ on inhibition, although
reaction times on the inhibitory control task did explain some variance in RAT, in line with
predictions. We conclude that bilinguals' professional and everyday-life experiences need to be
analysed carefully in studies of creativity.
References:
Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Fischer, R., & Christoffels, I. (2011). Bilingualism and creativity:
Benefits in convergent thinking come with losses in divergent thinking. Frontiers in
psychology, 2, 273.
Lange, K. V., Hopman, E. W., Zemla, J. C., & Austerweil, J. L. (2020). Evidence against a
relation between bilingualism and creativity. Plos one, 15(6), e0234928.
van Dijk, M., Kroesbergen, E. H., Blom, E., & Leseman, P. P. (2019). Bilingualism and
creativity: Towards a situated cognition approach. The Journal of Creative
Behavior, 53(2), 178-188.
Sign-spoken bilingualism and cognitive control in hearing children,
native signers
Justyna Kotowicz (Pedagogical University of Cracow), Bencie Woll (University College
London) and Gary Morgan (City University London)
According, the hypothesis of an cognitive control (CC) bilingual advantage, bilinguals need to
control language processing to a greater extent than monolinguals and this is thought to bring
greater experience in managing high-level demands, resulting in general CC benefits in nonlinguistic tasks (Bialystok, 2017). Sign-spoken bilingualism – in a sign language an spoken
language – provides a unique opportunity to examine the cognitive effect of using two highly
different languages with two distinct perceptual and motor systems (Emmorey, Luk, Pyers, &
Bialystok, 2008).
This study is the first looking specifically on CC in sign-spoken bilingual hearing
children – British Sing Language native signers who are growing up in deaf families in the UK.
The CC level of sign-spoken bilingual children was compared to unimodal bilinguals and
monolinguals. In the present study three groups of children (sign-spoken bilinguals, spoken
bilinguals and monolinguals) did not differ significantly on working memory skills and
cognitive flexibility. Sign-spoken bilinguals outperformed monolinguals on overall accuracy in
the conflict resolution task. However, the effect size was small, suggesting only a potential
effect of sign-spoken bilingualism on conflict resolution capacity. From the perspective of
bilingualism, cognitive control skills in both groups of bilingual children may be connected
with language switching opportunities and language processing demands (Green & Abutalebi,
2013).
64
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience.
Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262.
Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The Source of Enhanced Cognitive
Control in Bilinguals. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1201–1206.
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control
hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530.
A pragmatic analysis of chosen Brexit speeches of British and
German politicians: A comparison on the basis of speeches by
Theresa May, Angela Merkel, Jeremy Corbyn, and Olaf Scholz
Patrycja Kubicha
(Jan Kochanowski University, Kielce)
The goal of the presentation is to portray a pragmatic analysis of the content and the linguistic
elements (e.g. formal/informal language, deictic expressions) in chosen Brexit speeches
delivered by the following politicians: Theresa May from the British Conservative Party,
Angela Merkel from the German Christian Democratic Union (CDU – ChristlichDemokratische Union), Jeremy Corbyn from the British Labour Party, and Olaf Scholz from
the Social Democratic Party of Germany (SPD – Sozialdemokratische Partei Deutschlands).
The parties of the first two politicians are classified as center-right, whereas the parties of the
latter two are classified as center-left. Thus, the aim is to analyze the following: in what way
the linguistic elements and the issues and views concerning Brexit in May’s and Merkel’s
speeches are similar with regard to the center-right orientation of their parties, how these
elements and matters are depicted in the speeches of Corbyn and Scholz in terms of the centerleft orientation of their parties, what similarities there are concerning the speeches from Great
Britain and, respectively, those from Germany, and in what way the type of language and the
content of the speeches are similar in the case of all the speeches.
Thus, the research is to give a comparative insight into a crucial part of the political
discourse of prominent British and German politicians on the significant and up-to-date topic
of Brexit. Language, the culture of a country, and political orientation are all meaningful signs
of identity in today’s multilingual world, and the issue of Brexit plays a role in shaping certain
aspects of the functioning of different countries.
The analysis is conducted by the help of Laurence Anthony’s program AntConc, which
is helpful e.g. in counting the frequency of given words and phrases appearing in a written text,
as well as in analyzing the context of these words and phrases. The presenting of the analysis
is preceded by a brief theoretical outline of certain pragmatic issues which are crucial for the
analysis itself, especially the notions of locution, illocution, and perlocution (Speech Act
Theory), as well as the topic of political discourse.
References:
Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words? Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Corbyn, J. (2018). Leader’s speech, Liverpool 2018. British Political Speech. Retrieved from
http://www.britishpoliticalspeech.org/speech-archive.htm?speech=369
65
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Chilton, P. (2004). Analysing political discourse: Theory and practice. London and New York:
Routledge: Taylor & Francis Group.
Huang, Y. (2010). Pragmatics. In The Pragmatics Encyclopedia. London and New York:
Routledge: Taylor & Francis Group.
May, T. (2018). Leader’s speech, Birmingham 2018. British Political Speech. Retrieved from
http://www.britishpoliticalspeech.org/speech-archive.htm?speech=370
Merkel, A. (2018, September). Rede von Bundeskanzlerin Merkel auf einer Veranstaltung der
Deutsche Börse AG zur Zukunft des Finanzstandortes Deutschland in Europa am 4.
September 2018 in Frankfurt am Main. Die Bundesregierung. Retrieved from
https://www.bundesregierung.de/breg-de/suche/rede-von-bundeskanzlerin-merkel-aufeiner-veranstaltung-der-deutsche-boerse-ag-zur-zukunft-des-finanzstandortesdeutschland-in-europa-am-4-september-2018-in-frankfurt-am-main-1528610
Scholz, O. (2018, June). Rede von Bundesfinanzminister Olaf Scholz anlässlich der
Bankenwirtschaftlichen Tagung des Bundesverbands der Deutschen Volksbanken und
Raiffeisenbanken (BVR). Bundesministerium der Finanzen. Retrieved from
https://www.bundesfinanzministerium.de/Content/DE/Reden/2018/2018-06-20BVR.html
Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Wodak, R. (2010). Political discourse. In The Pragmatics Encyclopedia. London and New
York: Routledge: Taylor & Francis Group.
Yule, G. (1996). Pragmatics. H. G. Widdowson (Ed.), Oxford Introductions to Language Study.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Syllable repair and lexical stratification in Samoan
Grzegorz Lara
(University of Warsaw)
This paper considers the prosodic biases in Samoan in relation to words of native and foreign
origin. Specifically, native Samoan verbs exemplify a broad variety of consonant-zero
alternations (Pratt 1878, Rubach 2016). Those can be attributed to deletions resulting from a
language-wide ban on closed syllables. Crucially, in native vocabulary, Samoan makes no
attempts to save segments in such prosodically illicit positions. Conversely, Samoan loanwords
show greater propensity to retain stray codas through vowel insertion. This asymmetry is
demonstrably conditioned morphologically and thus may be indicative of lexical layering
(Winters 2015).
Such mappings can be accounted for by the use of morphological diacritics, an approach
that dates at least as far back as the onset of generative phonology (Chomsky & Halle 1968).
The interplay between morphology and phonology has since received considerable attention as
a source of valuable generalizations and insights; notably, this interplay became the core of
Lexical Phonology (Kiparsky 1982, Booij & Rubach 1987). More recently, morphologically
and lexically conditioned phonological asymmetries have been the subject of analyses couched
in Optimality Theory (OT, henceforth; Prince & Smolensky 1993, McCarthy & Prince 1995).
Several morphophonological variants of OT have since been proposed to address such biases,
e.g. Cophonology Theory (Orgun 1996, Inkelas, Orgun & Zoll 1997, Anttila 2002, Inkelas &
Zoll 2007), the Core-Periphery model (Itȏ & Mester 1995, 1999) and Constraint Indexing
(Prince & Smolensky 1993, Benua 1997, Alderete 1999, 2001, Pater 2000, 2007, 2009).
66
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
I propose that the asymmetries found in Samoan are best analyzed in terms of
stratification of the lexicon along the lines of the native-foreign distinction (Itȏ & Mester 1995).
Moreover, I argue that Cophonology Theory is best suited to account for the analyzed data.
Although Constraint Indexing can be used to correctly predict the observed phenomena,
it presents considerable theoretical problems in terms of unlimited multiplication of constraints.
Conversely, Cophonology Theory’s adoption of parallel rankings is fully justified in the
discussed case: there is ample typological evidence indicating that, in loanwords, faithfulness
tends to be more readily observed and, consequently, syllable repair effects are more common
therein (Paradis & LaCharité 1997).
References:
Alderete, J. (1999). Morphologically governed accent in Optimality Theory. Doctoral
dissertation, University of Massachussets, Amherst.
Alderete, J. (2001). Dominance effects as transderivational anti-faithfulness. Phonology 18,
201–253.
Anttila, A. (2002). Morphologically conditioned phonological alternations. Natural Language
& Linguistic Theory 20, 1–42.
Benua, L. (1997). Transderivational identity: Phonological relations between words. Doctoral
dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Booij, G. & Rubach, J. (1987). Postcyclic versus postlexical rules in Lexical Phonology.
Linguistic Inquiry 18, 1–44.
Chomsky, N. & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.
Inkelas, S., Orgun, O. & Zoll, C. (1997). The implications of lexical exceptions for the nature
of grammar. In Roca, I. (ed.) Derivations and constraints in phonology, 393–418. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Inkelas, S. & Zoll, C. (2007). Is grammar dependence real? A comparison between
cophonological and indexed constraint approaches to morphologically conditioned
phonology. Linguistics 45, 133–171.
Itȏ, J. & Mester, A. (1995). The core-periphery structure of the lexicon and constraints on
reranking. In Beckman, J., Dickey, L. W. & Urbanczyk, S. (eds.) University of
Massachusetts occasional papers in linguistics 18, 181–209.
Itȏ, J. & Mester, A. (1999). The structure of the phonological lexicon. In Tsujimura, N. (ed.)
The Handbook of Japanese Linguistics, 62–100. Malden & Oxford: Blackwell
Publishers.
Kiparsky, P. (1982). From cyclic phonology to Lexical Phonology. In van der Hulst, H. & Smith
N. (eds.) The structure of phonological representations 1, 131–75. Dordrecht: Foris
Publications.
McCarthy, J. & Prince, A. (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In Beckman, J.,
Dickey, L. W. & Urbanczyk, S. (eds.) University of Massachusetts occasional papers
in linguistics 18, 249–384. Amherst: GLSA.
Orgun, O. (1996). Sign-based morphology and phonology: With special attention to Optimality
Theory. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.
Pater, J. (2000). Nonuniformity in English stress: the role of ranked and lexically specific
constraints. Phonology 17, 237–274.
Pater, J. (2007). The locus of exceptionality: Morpheme-specific phonology as constraint
indexation. In Bateman, L., O'Keefe, M., Reilly, E. & Werle, A. (eds.) Papers in
Optimality Theory 3, 259–296. Amherst: GLSA.
67
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Pater, J. (2009). Morpheme-specific phonology: Constraint indexation and inconsistency
resolution. In Parker, S. (ed.) Phonological Argumentation: Essays on Evidence and
Motivation, 123–154. London: Equinox.
Paradis, C. & LaCharité, D. (1997). Preservation and minimality in loanword adaptation.
Journal of Linguistics 33. 379–430.
Pratt, G. (1878). A grammar and dictionary of the Samoan language, with English and Samoan
vocabulary. London: Trübner & Company.
Prince, A. & Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative
grammar. Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Sciences.
Rubach, J. (2016). Samoan syllable structure. Handout. Course 2704, Introduction to
Optimality Theory, University of Warsaw.
Winters, M. (2015). Lexical layers. In Taylor, J. (ed.) The Oxford Handbook of the Word, 445–
461. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Translation procedures accompanying phrasemes in “The Gray
Rhino”
Laszlo Laszloczki
(Eötvös Loránd University)
Using phrasemes mean the highest level of language proficiency. We have many definitions
but researchers can decide what is really a phraseme (T. Litovkina and Mieder 2004). In this
paper, I consider one- and two-word expressions, phrases, sayings and proverbs to be
phrasemes. My aim is to identify procedures when translators come across a phraseme when
translating an English economic text into Hungarian. Then I see what happens to this phraseme
in a reviewed text.
For the analysis, I chose Michelle Wucker’s The Gray Rhino and its Hungarian
translation. I consider this economic monograph as a fashionable self-help book. Works of this
kind intended for the general public are full of phrasemes. Economic texts may seem
complicated to carry out such a study, but technical language is really close to everyday
language. It affects almost everyone and its terms quickly become colloquial (Ablonczyné
2006). While doing the research, I performed a contrastive text analysis, in which I manually
searched for the phrasemes of the source and target language texts and of the reviewed target
language text. Based on Klaudy’s transfer operations (1994) and Vermes (2004), I identified
four procedures: equivalent, paraphrase/correspondence, omission and other solutions. Based
on these, I am looking for the answer to which procedure the translator preferred the most and
which the least and what the reviewer did with the phrasemes. I summarized my results in a
table, full of examples. I would like to introduce three new concepts in translation studies:
“phraseme survival”, “phraseme appearance in the target language text” and “phraseme
disappearance in the target language text”.
This study has two aims: to create hypotheses about the procedures of phraseme
translation and to examine whether the research method is suitable for the analysis of a larger
corpus and for the verification of the formulated hypotheses. Thus, if the former condition is
met, it will be possible to create a methodology for a conscious translation strategy, which will
be explained in detail in my doctoral dissertation, which can be a guideline for (economic)
translators in the future.
68
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Ablonczyné Mihályka, L. (2006). Gazdaság és nyelv. Lexikográfia Kiadó.
Klaudy, K. (1994). A fordítás elmélete és gyakorlata. Angol, német, francia, orosz
fordítástechnikai példatárral. Scholastica.
T. Litovkina, A., Mieder, W. (2005). A közmondást nem hiába mondják. Tinta.
Vermes, A. (2004). A relevancia-elmélet alkalmazása a kultúraspecifikus kifejezések
fordításának vizsgálatában. Fordítástudomány 6, 5–17.
Comparison of L2 and L3 learners of English in Germany: Why is
their performance so similar?
Eliane Lorenz
(Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim)
It has been postulated that due to differences in cross-linguistic influence (CLI), L2 and L3
language acquisition and production display crucial differences. Yet, some studies fail to
demonstrate such pronounced differences between L2 and L3 learners (Hopp, 2019; Lorenz et
al., 2019; Şahingöz, 2014). These studies investigated unbalanced bilingual heritage speakers
as opposed to relatively balanced bilinguals. Heritage speakers are bilinguals who grow up with
a minority language (heritage language) and a majority language (the official language of the
speech community). Usually, heritage speakers are dominant in the latter, with limited language
skills in the former (Montrul, 2016).
This study, set in the context of bilingual heritage speakers in Germany, analyzes written
and spoken production (picture descriptions) in the foreign language English by school aged
students (n=176; age 12 and 16). Russian-, Turkish-, and Vietnamese-German bilinguals in
Germany (L3 learners of English) are compared with monolingually raised control groups,
namely German, Russian, Turkish, and Vietnamese learners of English in Germany, Russia,
Turkey, and Vietnam (L2 learners of English). The study focuses on the use of tense and aspect
(formal correctness and target-like meaning as separate constructs). The results reveal that there
are differences in the English production between the monolingually raised Russian, Turkish,
and Vietnamese learners of English and the cohorts in Germany. However, there are no
statistically significant differences in English between the L2 and L3 learners who grow up in
Germany. Therefore, it is argued that CLI in L3 English comes exclusively from the majority
language German.
Two models of CLI illuminate these findings, (i) the ‘Typological Primacy Model’
(Rothman, 2011), and the ‘Linguistic Proximity Model’ (Westergaard et al., 2017). More
importantly, however, the results support dominant language transfer as proposed by Hopp
(2019) and Fallah and Jabbari (2018). Only German, the dominant language and the language
of instruction in school, is activated in L3 English acquisition. This is arguably reinforced by
the largely monolingual habitus which characterizes the current educational focus in foreign
language teaching in Germany (Bonnet & Siemund, 2018) and thus neglects any potential
positive effect of the heritage language on L3 English acquisition.
References:
Bonnet, A., & Siemund, P. (Eds.). (2018). Foreign Language Education in Multilingual
Classrooms. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
69
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Fallah, N., & Jabbari, A.A. (2018). L3 acquisition of English attributive adjectives. Dominant
language of communication matters for syntactic cross-linguistic influence. Linguistic
Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(2), 193–216.
Hopp, H. (2019). Cross-linguistic influence in the child third language acquisition of grammar:
Sentence comprehension and production among Turkish-German and German learners
of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(2), 567–583.
Lorenz, E., Bonnie, R., Feindt, K., Rahbari, S., & Siemund, P. (2019). Cross-linguistic
influence in unbalanced bilingual heritage speakers on subsequent language acquisition:
Evidence from pronominal object placement in ditransitive clauses. International
Journal of Bilingualism, 23(6), 1410–1430.
Montrul, S. (2016). The Acquisition of Heritage Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Rothman, J. (2011). L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy. The
typological primacy model. Second Language Research, 27(2), 107–127.
Şahingöz, Y. (2014). Schulische Mehrsprachigkeit bei türkisch-deutsch bilingualen Schülern:
Eine Analyse von transferinduzierten Wortstellungsmustern. (Doctoral Dissertation).
Retrieved from http://ediss.sub.uni-hamburg.de/volltexte/2018/9128/.
Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R, & Rodina, Y. (2017). Crosslinguistic
influence in the acquisition of a third language: The Linguistic Proximity Model.
International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 666–682.
What is “authentic” in the professional identity of foreign language
teachers teaching abroad?
Rea Lujić (University of Zadar) and Martina Podboj (University of Rijeka)
In this paper we aim to explore the complexities of contemporary identity practices in
superdiverse societies today. We opt for a realistic, anti-essentialist approach to studying
identity practices as discursive orientations towards sets of features that are (or can be) seen as
emblematic of particular identities. We draw on the theory of identity in a context of
superdiversity proposed by Blommaert and Varis (2013, 2015) to answer the question how
authenticity is manufactured by blending a variety of semiotic resources, some of which are
sufficient ('enough') to produce a particular targeted authentic identity, and consequently enable
others to identify us as 'real', 'authentic' members of social groups. We do so by looking into
experiences of foreign language teachers teaching abroad (FLTTA), a specific type of glomads
or professionals who live and work abroad, frequently transgressing social, linguistic, and
cultural spaces and borders (Podboj & Lujić, 2020).
We build on our previous research about Croatian and French FLTTAs, where we
investigated how various ideologies and other social aspects of the fragmented spaces they
navigate affect their agency and their positioning as multilingual teachers, speakers and learners
of certain languages. We expand our findings by looking into the dynamic nature of the concept
of enoughness.
Our data consists of two segments: personal narratives about FLTTAs professional
identity experiences elicited via semi-structured interviews, and open-ended questionnaires,
where we specifically focused on the concept of enoughness. The data is analyzed qualitatively
and presented as several mini case-studies, with illustrative examples that are thematically
related to enoughess. The aim of our analysis was to identify features for ‘being admitted into
70
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
and being recognized as’ a FLTTA. More specifically, what makes these individuals being
perceived as ‘having enough’ of FLTTAs’ features, or recognized as ‘real’ or ‘authentic’
members of this social group? Additionally, do they perceive themselves as authentic enough?
References:
Blommaert, J. & Varis, P. (2013). Enough is enough. The heuristics of authenticity in
superdiversity. In J. Duarte & I. Gogolin (Eds.), Linguistic Superdiversity in Urban
Areas: Research approaches (pp. 143‐159). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Blommaert, J., & Varis, P. (2015). Enoughness, accent and light communities: Essays on
contemporary identities. (Tilburg Papers in Culture Studies; No. 139).
Podboj, M. & Lujić, R. (2020). Living in the ‘in-between’: narrative inquiry into investment of
foreign language teachers teaching abroad. Komunikacija i Kultura Online, (11), 189225.
Fed counterfeeding in Podhale Goralian
Tomasz Łuszczek
(University of Warsaw)
Ever since Kiparsky’s (1968, 1973) pioneering work on phonological opacity, pairwise rule
relations have been argued to fall under the transparent-opaque dichotomy. Specifically, an
interaction is transparent when both of its constituent generalisations hold of the surface
representation. Conversely, an interaction is opaque when at least one of its constituent
generalisations does not hold of the surface representation. Consequently, a single pairwise rule
relation cannot be transparent and opaque at the same time. However, more recent studies
suggest that the familiar dichotomous classification is in fact too restrictive and that a third,
mixed type of rule interaction is called for. In particular, Kavitskaya & Staroverov (2010)
present a case of fed counterfeeding in which an earlier process A feeds a later process B, and
B counterfeeds A. Because feeding order is (typically) transparent and counterfeeding order is
(typically) opaque, fed counterfeeding must be recognised as a third type of interaction, one
that consolidates transparency and opacity.
I argue that an instance of fed counterfeeding is found in Podhale Goralian (PG; Rubach
& Łuszczek, 2019), a Polish dialect spoken in southern Poland. PG exhibits a process called
Final Tensing (FT) which changes ɔ→o before word-final voiced consonants, e.g., B[o]g ‘God’
(cf. B[ɔ]g+a (gen.sg.). FT interacts with Nasal Laxing (NL), a phonotactic generalisation that
changes o→ɔ prenasally, e.g., zł[ɔ]m ‘scrap’. In terms of rule order, FT precedes NL.
Consequently, the relation FT→NL is that of feeding, while the relation FT←NL is that of
counterfeeding. Clearly, the net result is fed counterfeeding.
I offer an analysis of PG fed counterfeeding in two different frameworks: (1) classic
rule theory, such as Lexical Phonology (Kiparsky, 1982; Booij & Rubach, 1987), and (2)
Optimality Theory (OT; McCarthy & Prince, 1995; Prince & Smolensky, 2004). It is shown
that each theory handles fed counterfeeding differently. Significantly, although both theoretical
frameworks can deliver a correct analysis of the data, the OT-based analysis is superior. For
one thing, the rule-based account predicts a circular chain of changes, ɔ→o→ɔ, resulting in
what strikes as gratuitous derivationalism. On the other hand, OT successfully recasts this
analysis as a parallel evaluation.
71
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Booij, G. E., & Rubach, J. (1987). Postcyclic versus postlexical rules in Lexical Phonology.
Linguistic Inquiry, 18, 1–44.
Kavitskaya, D., & Staroverov, P. (2010). When an interaction is both opaque and transparent:
the paradox of fed counterfeeding. Phonology, 27, 255–288.
Kiparsky, P. (1968). Linguistic universals and linguistic change. In E. Bach & R. T. Harms
(Eds.), Universal in Linguistic Theory (pp. 170–202). New York: Holt, Reinhart and
Winston.
Kiparsky, P. (1973). Abstractness, opacity and global rules (Part 2 of “Phonological
representations”). In O. Fujimura (Ed.), Three Dimensions of Linguistic Theory (pp. 57–
86). Tokyo: TEC.
Kiparsky, P. (1982). From Cyclic to Lexical Phonology. In H. van der Hulst & N. Smith
(Eds.), The structure of phonological representations (pp. 131–175). Dordrecht: Foris
Publications.
McCarthy, J. J., & Prince, A. (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In J. N. Beckman,
L. W. Dickey & S. Urbanczyk (Eds.), University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers
in Linguistics 18 (pp. 249–384). Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistic Student Association
Publications.
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (2004). Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative
grammar. Oxford: Blackwell. [Revision of 1993 technical report, Rutgers University
Center for Cognitive Sciences. Available from Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA–537.]
Rubach, J., & Łuszczek, T. (2019). The vowel system of Podhale Goralian. Research in
Language, 17, 105–126.
“Provocativa, cruda, bien Boricua”: Language choice in the music
of Buscabulla
Aidan Malanoski
(City University of New York)
Much recent sociolinguistic research on language mixing has argued that, rather than possessing
discrete codes, speakers draw on a unified repertoire of semiotic resources in their speech
(Seargeant & Tagg, 2011; Vogel & García, 2017; Garley, 2019). Consequently, such research
tends to focus on instances of language mixing. In contrast, the present work investigates the
linguistic practices of the formerly New York-based Puerto Rican band Buscabulla and of their
listeners, presenting a case in which speakers whose repertoires contain both English and
Spanish restrict themselves to “pure” Spanish in order to enact certain identities.
The analysis draws on Bucholtz & Hall’s (2004) tactics of intersubjectivity and Agha’s
(2003) enregisterment. The former term refers to the “contextually relevant sociopolitical
relations” (Bucholtz & Hall, 2004, p. 382) used in identity work, while the latter refers to the
process by which linguistic features come to be perceived as a distinct variety of language
linked to particular social personae. Thus, enregisterment describes the development of
essentialized language varieties associated with essentialized identities. By drawing on
linguistic registers, speakers can invoke these identities in their identity work (Bucholtz & Hall,
2004; Ilbury, 2019).
Buscabulla formerly lived in New York City. As such, we might expect them to engage
in codemixing, which is a “badge of in-group membership” for Nuyoricans (Zentella, 2003, p.
72
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
253). Yet Buscubulla sing almost exclusively in Spanish, largely restricting their use of English
to occasional borrowings (e.g., les gusta mucho el flow ‘they really like the flow’). The nearabsolute use of Spanish aligns Buscabulla with an essentialized Puerto Rican identity indexed
by this “pure” Spanish, while simultaneously distancing the band from a Nuyorican identity
characterized by codemixing.
Their listeners’ language use matches Buscabulla’s to a notable degree. Out of 344
YouTube comments on two of Buscabulla’s videos, 186 were in Spanish and 88 in English,
while only 22 showed codemixing. By replicating Buscabulla’s near-exclusive use of Spanish,
fans express their similarity with Buscabulla, enacting a relation of adequation (Bucholtz &
Hall, 2004). The present work demonstrates that, although speakers have access to unified,
multilingual repertoires, they may draw on essentialized registers as tools of identification.
References:
Agha, A. (2003). The social life of cultural value. Language and Communication, 23, 231-273.
Bucholtz, M., & Hall, K. (2004). Language and identity. In A. Duranti (Ed.), A companion to
linguistic anthropology (pp. 369-394). Blackwell.
Buscabulla. (2020, August 13). Buscabulla - Mío (Official Video) [video]. Youtube.
https://youtu.be/58FLsHzpPec
Garley, Matt. (2019). Choutouts: Language contact and US-Latin hip hop on YouTube. Lengua
y Migración / Language and Migration, 11(2), 77-106.
Ilbury, C. (2020). “Sassy Queens”: Stylistic orthographic variation in Twitter and the
enregisterment of AAVE. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 24(2), 245-264.
Seargeant, P., & Tagg, C. (2011). English on the internet and a ‘post-varieties’ approach to
language. World Englishes, 30(4), 496-514.
Vogel, S., & García, O. (2017). Translanguaging. In Oxford research encyclopedia of
education. Oxford University Press.
Zentella, A.C. (2003). Returned migration, language, and identity: Puerto Rican bilinguals in
dos worlds/two mundos. In M.C. Gutmann & F.V. Matos Rodriguez & L. Stephen & P.
Zavella (eds.), Perspectives on Las Américas: A reader in culture, history, and
representation (pp. 245-258). Blackwell.
Negotiating identities in the translanguaging classroom
Gisela Mayr
(Free University of Bolzano)
The present qualitative research investigates, how students complex plurilingual identities
emerge in a translanguaging classroom. In traditional language learning settings learners are
supposed to concentrate on the target language only, thus eliminating as much as possible any
influences from the other languages of their repertoire, likewise different cultural references
systems present in the classroom are not taken account for. A translanguaging pedagogy to the
contrary has the aim to activate the whole linguistic repertoire of each learner, so that the they
can mediate between the different features of it and in doing so support their language learning
process (García & Wei 2014; García et al. 2017). Such an approach not only does justice to the
linguistic and cultural diversity in the classroom by including all languages of the repertoire
and cultures present in the learning process (Cummins 1996; 2009). It also allows students to
unfold their complex plurilingual as well as cultural identities and in the ideal case to
renegotiate their opinions and attitudes by questioning long standing assumptions and believes
(Li Wei 2011).
73
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
The study was carried out in a secondary school in Bolzano Italy. In a class of 22
students (age 16) transalganguaging modules were inserted in regular language classes in the
course of a school year. The languages involved were: German, Italian, English and Spanish
alongside with heritage languages. A group of five students was chosen for closer observation.
Audio and video recordings were made during groupworks, and semi structured interviews
were carried out with each of the students at regular intervals throughout the data collection
period. The study investigates if and in what way translanguaging pedagogy changes the
learners’ perception of themselves withing the school, furthermore it poses the question as to
whether the complexity of plurilingual identities can emerge in a translanguaging learning
setting and, if yes, what consequences this has for the individual learning process.
References:
Cummins, J. (1996). Negotiating identities. Education for empowerment. California: California
Association of Bilingualism.
Cummins, J. (2000). Language power and pedagogy. Bilingual children in the crossfire.
Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
García, O., Johnson, S.; Seltzer, K. (2017). The Translanguaging Classroom: Leveraging
Student Bilingualism for Learning. Philadelphia: Caslon.
Garcìa, O.; Li Wei (2014). Translanguaging: language, bilingualism and education.
Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Li Wei (2011): Moment analysis and translanguaging space: Discursive construction of
identities by multilingual Chinese youth in Britain. Journal of Pragmatics, 43: 1222–
1235.
Types of allography: Conceptualizing structural variation in writing
at the material and linguistic levels
Dimitrios Meletis
(University of Zurich)
Although structural variation in writing is highly frequent both at the material and functional
levels, the associated notion of allography, although sometimes mentioned in the literature, has
not been systematically studied.
In this talk, two major types of allography are proposed: Graphetic allography,
conceptually comparable to allophony, depends on visual similarity and captures how concrete
units are associated with visual abstractions, i.e. how three graphs in <banana> are instances of
the basic shape |a|. Graphematic allography, on the other hand, is conceptually closer to
allomorphy and does not depend on visual similarity but groups together units which share the
same function, i.e. represent the same linguistic unit (phoneme, syllable, morpheme, etc.).
These are complementarily distributed, i.e. there exist no contexts in which they contrast. A
well-known example is the positionally conditioned alternation between |σ| vs. |ς| for the Greek
grapheme <σ>, but also the positional allography of shapes in Arabic and radicals in Chinese.
By means of a number of criteria, a typology of subtypes of graphetic and graphematic
allography is proposed and illustrated with examples from different writing systems. A special
case that is discussed is the complex phenomenon of capitalization in alphabets. Moreover,
examples are given of variation phenomena which are not accounted for by the concept of
allography. Lastly, it is addressed how orthographic variation, as a type of variation dependent
on the externally regulated standardization of the writing system, interacts with the underlying
74
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
internal regularities of the system. A question present throughout is how the proposed typology
could reflect the psychological reality of how readers and writers process written variation.
The theoretical framework for the unified description of variation proposed in this talk
facilitates the comparison of variation phenomena across writing systems, which is crucial for
the advancement of our understanding of writing systems.
Lexicalizing subversion, gendering language: The secret language
of the Koti-Hijra community in Bengal
Enakshi Nandi
(Jawaharlal Nehru University, New Delhi)
Ulti, spoken by the transfeminine community of hijras and kotis in eastern India, is the site and
means of identity construction as well as gender expression for this speech community. It is a
secret language that has emerged historically from the socially segregated hijra households, and
is used exclusively for intra-community bonding and communication in the current scenario.
From lexical items that give voice to taboo subjects like sexual activities (dhurano – to have
sex, khumur kora – to have oral sex, etc.), private body parts (batli - anus, likom - penis, cipti –
vagina, etc.), sexual and gender identities (bhobrashi – intersex person, koti – transfeminine
person, panthi – cishet lover of koti, etc.), and other aspects intrinsic to the hijra social system
(including kinship terms, rituals, professions), to the semantic gender marker “mashi” (literal
meaning: aunt) that modifies nouns, postpositions, pronouns etc., Ulti has emerged for the
express purpose of giving a voice to the marginalized, disadvantaged, and largely unlettered
hijra-koti communities in a socio-cultural landscape that is predominantly structured along
heteropatriarchal and cis-normative lines.
Bangla, the language that is predominantly spoken in the region, has no grammatical
gender, and therefore possesses no gendered pronouns and displays no gender agreement. This
property extends to Ulti as well, which is essentially a list of lexical items (bare Ulti) unique to
the hijra communities of the Indian subcontinent that are embedded in the syntactic framework
of Bangla to create the mixed language Ulti. However, the speakers of Ulti use the feminine
marker “mashi”, whose use is mostly optional, most productively to assert their agency in their
identity construction. In using it, they claim to be feminizing their language as an extension of
their own feminine identity and presentation. While varieties of such secret hijra languages exist
across the subcontinent, this is the first one amongst the varieties researched upon thus far that
demonstrates this feature; the others, which are mixed languages that have grammatical gender,
demonstrate the shift between the masculine and feminine identities of its speakers through
shifts in gender agreement or gendered pronouns and nouns to index the shifts in the selfconstruction of their gender(ed) identities.
References:
Awan, M. S., & Muhammad, S. (2011). Queer but Language: A Sociolinguistic Study of Farsi.
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science, 127-135.
Dutta, A. (2013). Globalizing through the Vernacular: Gender/sexual Transnationalism and
the Making of Sexual Minorities in Eastern India. University of Minnesota.
Hall, K. (1995). Hijra/Hijrin: Language and Gender Identity. California.
75
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Hall, K. (1997). "Go Suck Your Husband's Sugarcane!" Hijras and the Use of Sexual Insult. In
A. L. (eds.), Queerly Phrased: Language, Gender, and Sexuality (pp. 430-460). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Hall, K. (2005). Intertextual Sexuality: Parodies of Class, Identity, and Desire in Liminal Delhi.
Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 15(1), 125-144.
Nagar, I. (2008). Language, Gender and Identity: The Case of Kotis in Lucknow India. Ohio:
Ohio State University.
Myers-Scotton, C., & Jake, J. L. (2000). Testing a Model of Morpheme Classification with
Language Contact Data. London: Kingston Services.
Myers-Scotton, C. (2004). How Codeswitching as an Available Option Empowers Bilinguals.
Essen: LAUD.
Myers-Scotton, C. (2010). Contact Linguistics: Bilingual Encounters and Grammatical
Outcomes. New York: Oxford University Press.
Reddy, G. (2005). With Respect to Sex: Negotiating Hijra Identity in South India. University of
Chicago Press.
Moody bilinguals: The role of mood in bilingual word processing
depends on word valence
Marcin Naranowicz
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
Affect permeates our communicative interactions. Bilinguals have been observed to process the
emotional content of native (L1) and non-native (L2) words differently (see Jończyk, 2016 for
a review). Interestingly, monolingual research has also showed that mood
(i.e., a current emotional internal state; Herz et al., 2020) influences the mind unobtrusively yet
pervasively, with a positive mood facilitating language processing and a negative mood taxing
it (e.g., Chwilla et al., 2011). Yet, there is little research on how bilinguals process emotional
words in positive and negative moods (Kissler & Bromberek-Dyzman, 2021). Here, we show
that positive and negative moods influence L1 and L2 differently, which is dependent upon
word valence. Participants were 58 proficient late unbalanced Polish–English bilinguals. To
induce the target moods, they were asked to watch 14 positively- and 14 negatively-valenced
animated film clips. The linguistic stimuli included 240 single words: 120 English and 120
Polish abstract adjectives, including 40 negative (e.g., lonely), 40 neutral (e.g., ongoing), and
40 positive words (e.g., awesome) for each language. Participants performed an evaluative
decision task (i.e., decide if presented words were positive, negative, or neutral). We measured
their reaction times (RTs) and analysed them using linear mixed-effects models. The results
showed an interaction between mood, language, and word valence: (i) faster RTs to L1
compared to L2 neutral words in a positive mood, without such a between-language difference
in a negative mood; (ii) no between-mood and between-language differences for negative
words; (iii) faster RTs to L2 positive words in a positive compared to negative mood, without
such a between-mood difference in L1. These results indicate that while standard cognitive
phenomena (here, longer RTs in L2 compared to L1; e.g., de Groot et al., 2002) can be observed
in a positive mood, a negative mood may be a hindrance to the processing of both L1 and L2
(e.g., Kissler & Bromberek-Dyzman, 2021). Then, no temporal differences between L1 and L2
positive words in the positive mood condition, with no such an effect in the negative mood
76
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
condition, may result from the facilitatory cumulative effect of a positive mood and positivelyvalenced words.
References:
Chwilla, D. J., Virgillito, D., & Vissers, C. Th. W. M. (2011). The Relationship of Language
and Emotion: N400 Support for an Embodied View of Language Comprehension. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2400–2414.
de Groot, A. M. B., Borgwaldt, S., Bos, M., & van den Eijnden, E. (2002). Lexical Decision
and Word Naming in Bilinguals: Language Effects and Task Effects. Journal of Memory
and Language, 47(1), 91–124.
Herz, N., Baror, S., & Bar, M. (2020). Overarching States of Mind. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 24(3), 184–199.
Kissler, J., & Bromberek-Dyzman, K. (2021). Mood Induction Differently Affects Early Neural
Correlates of Evaluative Word Processing in L1 and L2. Frontiers in Psychology, 11,
588902.
Jończyk, R. (2016). Affect-Language Interactions in Native and Non-Native English Speakers.
Springer International Publishing.
How words matter: A gender-based cognitive-linguistic investigation
of word meaning perception
Nadine Nasef
(Misr International University)
Words, whether spoken or written, are the vehicle of human communication as they are not
only exchanged between people, but they also determine humans’ sense of self and how they
relate to the world. Life is meaningless without words, since words allow us, humans, to track
human thought, human emotion, and therefore human development. More specifically, words
are the inner dialogue that can change how humans perceive all that happens around them.
Because every fragment of the brain works jointly to interpret, perceive, and understand stimuli
to create reality, a person uses certain words to shape their perceptions of the world. What
people consciously perceive is shown to be profoundly shaped by their perceptions and thus
knowledge and expectations. Given the fact that language is the window to the cognitive
function, it provides insights into the thoughts and ideas that go through the mind, reflecting
the configuration of how the human mind works. Therefore, determining how the mind
recognizes what each word means, and thus affects the behavior of humans, relies heavily on
cognitive linguistics, which aims at analyzing how language is intertwined with the mind, i.e.,
cognition and perception. This poster is based on a work-in-progress research study and thus
reflects the foundational theoretical framework which will be applied to the research project.
Given that a theory has to be applied to enable collecting authentic data that could either confirm
or refute it, this study will endeavor to try to empirically identify the cognitive and
metacognitive processes which the human mind goes through to interpret a word and thus
perceive a certain meaning, i.e., to investigate what goes on in a person’s mind while discerning
the semantics/denotation of a certain word. This poster focuses on cognitive semantics, coupled
with various interdisciplinary perspectives that are interrelated with language, including
psychological gender differences, discourse semantics, conceptual analysis, perceptual
language, metacognition, attribution theory, schema theory, and recently, cognitive semantics.
77
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Non-binary gender reference in Polish: Between grammar and
identity in social context
Jagoda Nosal and Agnieszka Kiełkiewicz-Janowiak
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
The language and linguistic practices of the LGBT+ community have long been of interest to
researchers (Leap 1995, Morrish and Sauntson 2007). Recently, the issue of representing nonbinary individuals in language has become the focus of several studies (Zimman et al. 2014,
Konnelly and Cowper 2020), in particular in languages with rich grammatical gender systems
(Shroy 2016, Derecka 2019).
Polish marks grammatical gender (masculine, feminine, neuter) on all nouns and their
modifiers. Gender is also marked on verbs in the past tense and the imperfective aspect of
analytical future tense forms (Nagórko 1996, Łaziński 2006).
The Polish grammatical gender system poses some issues for non-binary and
transgender Polish speakers who do not identify as either male or female. Normative reference
may thus render gender non-conforming people invisible or deny them means of authentic
(self-)representation. Therefore, many have chosen to seek adequate self-expression via
language.
In this ethnolinguistic study we have explored the linguistic practices of Polish nonbinary and transgender communities with regard to their self-reference. We have asked the
following questions about current practices and attitudes:
− How do non-binary people ‘do gender’ linguistically?
− How do they deal with the challenges of the grammatical gender system?
− Is Polish considered adequate for the expression of non-binary gender identities?
It is our aim to show an array of possible novel forms, for example, verbal forms marked
for the neuter gender, or the omission of the gender marker. This was investigated by means of
a survey among non-binary Polish speakers who were members of LGBT+ Facebook groups.
Because non-normative and/or innovative practices may only be effective if recognized as
gender identity claims, we have also conducted a qualitative inquiry into online discussions to
uncover how and why users find some usages problematic.
The data show a picture of ongoing linguistic and social change manifested by
significant variability of usage. The use of novel forms varies depending on users’ identities
and their variable relevance in communicative contexts. When present in public spaces, those
practices improve the visibility of non-binary people and raise social awareness about some of
the challenges faced daily by the LGBT+ community.
References:
Bucholtz, M., & Hall, K. (2004). Theorizing identity in language and sexuality research.
Language in Society, 33(04), 469-515.
Cameron, D. (2005). Language, gender, and sexuality: current issues and new directions.
Applied Linguistics, 26(4), 482–502.
Derecka, M. (2019). Manifestations of transphobia in computer-mediated communication. A
case study of language discrimination in English and Polish Internet-mediated
discourse. Studies in Polish Linguistics, 14(3), 101-123.
Hord, L. C. R. (2016). Bucking the linguistic binary: gender neutral language in English,
Swedish, French, and German. Western Papers in Linguistics / Cahiers linguistiques de
Western 3, 4. http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/wpl_clw/vol3/iss1/4
78
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Konnelly, L., & Cowper, E. (2020). Gender diversity and morphosyntax: an account of singular
they. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 5(1), 40.
Leap, W. (Ed.). (1995). Beyond the lavender lexicon: authenticity, imagination, and
appropriation in lesbian and gay languages. Amsterdam: Gordon and Breach.
Łaziński, M. (2006). O panach i paniach. Polskie rzeczowniki tytularne i ich asymetria
rodzajowo-płciowa. Warszawa: PWN.
Morrish, L., & Sauntson, H. (2007). New perspectives on language and sexual identity.
Palgrave Macmillan.
Nagórko, A. (1996). Zarys gramatyki polskiej. PWN.
Shroy, A. J. (2016). Innovations in gender-neutral French: language practices of nonbinary
French speakers on Twitter. Ms., University of California, Davis.
https://www.academia.edu/33393890
Zimman, L., Davis, J. L., & Raclaw, J. (Eds.). (2014). Queer excursions: Retheorizing binaries
in language, gender, and sexuality. Oxford University Press.
On how anticipatory mechanisms modulate the processing of verbfinal structures in sight translation
Katharina Oster (University of Mainz, Germersheim), Paweł Korpal (Adam Mickiewicz
University, Poznań), Katarzyna Jankowiak (Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań) and Janna
Verthein (University of Mainz, Germersheim)
Interpreting is a highly demanding cognitive activity, as it requires a simultaneous co-activation
of two language systems. One of the factors that might additionally impact the degree of
cognitive effort involved in performing an interpreting task is a structural characteristic of a
source language (Setton, 1999, p.128). In line with this assumption, interpreting researchers
have often discussed German as an example of a problematic source language due to its frequent
verb-final structures (e.g. Riccardi, 1996; Seeber, 2011), the processing of which may
potentially be more cognitively taxing relative to verb-initial structures (Seeber & Kerzel,
2012). Little attention has, nonetheless, been devoted to studying anticipatory mechanisms
inherent in interpreting verb-final structures (but see Konieczny and Döring, 2003 for studies
on monolingual language comprehension).
The present study is therefore aimed to investigate anticipatory mechanisms engaged in
processing German verb-final and regular (i.e., verb-initial) sentences in a sight translation task,
which is treated as a variant of simultaneous interpreting (Pöchhacker, 2004, p.19), where the
interpreter has visual access to a written source text which has to be delivered orally in the
target language. The stimuli used in the experiment will include 120 German sentences with
subordinating conjunctions (verb-final structure, e.g. “Er kaufte ein Ticket, bevor er zum
Bahnhof fuhr.”) and control sentences with conjunctive adverbs (verb-initial structure, e.g. “Er
kaufte ein Ticket, danach fuhr er zum Bahnhof.”). Around 20 translation students will be asked
to read either for translation into English or for repetition (a shadowing condition). We
hypothesize that the sentences will elicit longer reading times in the translation relative to the
shadowing condition, as the translation task would require an increased working memory and
attention mechanisms to facilitate participants’ performance. Furthermore, we expect this effect
to be even more pronounced in the case of verb-final sentences, which additionally require more
extended anticipation processes to facilitate the process of translation, and are thus even more
cognitively demanding. During our talk, we will present and discuss the preliminary results of
79
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
the experiment. The study outcomes may provide insights into the question of processing
complex, verb-final structures in interpreting.
References:
Konieczny, L. & Döring, P. (2003). Anticipation of clause-final heads. Evidence from eyetracking and SRNs, Proceedings of iccs/ascs (pp. 13–17). Sydney, NSW.
Pöchhacker, F. (2004). Introducing Interpreting Studies. London: Routledge.
Riccardi, A. (1996). Language-specific strategies in simultaneous interpreting. In C. Dollerup
& V. Appel (eds.), Teaching translation and interpreting 3: New horizons. Papers from
the third Language International conference, Elsinore, Denmark, 9-11 June 1995 (pp.
213-222). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Seeber, K. (2011). Cognitive load in simultaneous interpreting. Existing theories – new models,
Interpreting, 13(2): 176-204.
Seeber, K. & Kerzel, D. (2012). Cognitive load in simultaneous interpreting: Model meets data,
International Journal of Bilingualism, 16(2): 228-242.
Setton, R. (1999). Simultaneous interpretation: A cognitive-pragmatic analysis. Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
Gender in the streets
Jeni Peake and Anaïs Carrere
(University Bordeaux Montaigne)
Posters, stickers, graffiti, tags, and street art are all forms of “street behaviour, a form of speech
that occurs [...] alongside or on city streets” (Sage, 2016 p.856). Following the #metoo
movement online, people have also taken to the streets to highlight the gender issues that are
still present and challenged today. The streets have always been a canvas for the marginalised
in society, whereby traditional methods of communities have been inaccessible (Bushnell, 1990
p. 813). Expressions of the self, especially self-identity may be conveyed by verbal
communication and interaction (Eckert & McConnell-Ginet, 2007), social interaction (West &
Zimmerman, 1987) and also visual communication (Belkin & Sheptak, 2018), such as street
behaviour (Ball 2019).
The researchers in Gender Studies define gender as:
Gender identity is as ‘the sameness, unity, and persistence of one’s individuality as male,
female, or ambivalent. … Gender identity is the private experience of gender role, and gender
role is the public experience of gender identity’ (Fausto- Sterling, 2000 [1972] p. 4).
In this study, gender identity may also imply the notions of gender and sexual fluidity.
These approaches are analysed as cultural and social constructions of identity. The term gender
is considered by many scholars a polysemic term: its meaning may vary according to the
context in which it is used.
The study in question explores the use of language in the streets through a corpus of
urban inscriptions, a questionnaire of how the graffiti is perceived by individuals, a survey of
graffiti writers and semi-directed interviews with graffiti writers. Various questions will be
addressed in this study: how is gender expressed through written street behaviour? What gender
issues are addressed? How are the messages conveyed by the writers? How are the same
messages received by the public?
80
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
The initial results demonstrate that all genders take to the streets in order to express
themselves and their discontent with the current status quo. The movements and issues being
addressed in the inscriptions are global and therefore are expressed in both French and English.
We would like to present this comparative study focusing on our theoretical framework and our
data.
References:
Ball, D. (2019). The Writing on the..Desk: Desktop Graffiti as Unobtrusive Measure of Campus
Climate Before and After the Virginia Tech Massacre. Deviant Behavior, 41(3), 366–
377.
Bushnell, J. S. (1990). Moscow Graffiti. Language and Subculture.
Belkin, S, Sheptak, R, (2018) Talking Bodies: Athletes & Tattoos as Nonverbal
Communication in The Sport Journal (Vol.21) 1-23
Eckert, P., & McConnell-Ginet, S. (2007). Putting communities of practice in their place.
Gender & Language, 1(1), 27–37.
Fausto-Sterling, A. (2000). Sexing the Body: Gender Politics and the Construction of Sexuality
(New Ed). Basic Books. Journal of Social History, 49(4), 855–880.
Sage, E. (2016). Jaytalking in Fin-de-Siècle Paris: Streets, Graffiti, and the Police.
West, C, Zimmerman, Don H (1987). Doing Gender. In: Gender and Society [online]. 1987.
Vol. 1, n° 2, pp. 125 151. [Accessed 24 October 2019].
The construction of gender identity in preschool: Do the teachers’
practices matter?
Marie Peuzin, Aurélie Nardy, Jean-Pierre Chevrot
(Université Grenoble Alpes)
Girls and boys are socialised differently in accordance with models of femininity and
masculinity (Vouillot, 2002), by many instances, such as families, schools and peer groups
(Darmon, 2010). Language is crucial for individuals’ socialisation process (Ochs, 2000) and
socialisation contributes to the construction of gender identities (Vouillot, 2002). Adults in
home and school contexts speak differently to girls and boys, in terms of quantity and quality
of speech (Lanvers, 2004; Mosconi, 1999). Differences in adults’ child directed speech (CDS)
could be manifestations of gender stereotypes, reflecting distinct expectations of girls and boys,
and contributing to the construction of gender identities.
Studies on CDS at school have mainly been carried out from elementary school to
university. Less studies are conducted in preschool whereas it is an important period in the
children’s development, during which they integrate school’s norms and start to differentiate
themselves – as girls become less interactive than boys in the classroom (De Boissieu, 2009).
In an ongoing study conducted in a French preschool, we analyse teacher-pupil
interactions, in terms of quantity (duration of speech, number of words and utterances) and
nature of CDS (mean length of utterances, lexical diversity, sociolinguistic use of the variable
particle ne in French negation, pragmatic content of utterances) in two teachers, male and
female, in charge of classes composed of children aged 3-4 years. Data were extracted from the
DyLNet project (Nardy et al., 2016, available at https://dylnet.univ-grenoblealpes.fr/?language=en).
On the majority of the observed features, teachers did not show differentiated practices
towards girls and boys, except for the male teacher who produced more injunctions related to
81
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
behaviour to boys and more injunctions related to learning activities to girls. We are currently
exploring these results through an extension of the database and through a detailed analysis of
speech acts in the discourse of teachers.
It would appear that gender-based differences in the utterances produced by parents do
not fully manifest themselves when preschool teachers talk to pupils. Professional posture could
partly neutralize social stereotypes in school context.
References:
De Boissieu, C. (2009). Sexes et genres à l’école maternelle. Recherches & éducations, 2, 2343.
Darmon, M. (2010). La socialisation. Paris: Armand Colin.
Lanvers, U. (2004). Gender in discourse behaviour in parent–child dyads: a literature review.
Child: Care, Health and Development, 30(5), 481 493.
Mosconi, N. (1999). Les recherches sur la socialisation différentielle de sexes à l’école. In Y.
Lemel & B. Roudet, Filles et garçons jusqu’à l’adolescence: socialisations
différentielles. Paris: L’Harmattan.
Nardy, A., Fleury, É., Chevrot, J.-P., Karsai, M., Buson, L., Bianco, M., Rousset, I., Dugua, C.,
Liégeois, L., Barbu, S., Crespelle, C., Busson, A., Léo, Y., Bouchet, H. & Dai, S. (2016).
DyLNet – Language Dynamics, Linguistic Learning, and Sociability at Preschool:
Benefits of Wireless Proximity Sensors in Collecting Big Data. https://dylnet.univgrenoble-alpes.fr/dylnet-project?language=en
Ochs, E. (2000). Socialization. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 9(1‐2), 230‐233.
Vouillot, F. (2002). Construction et affirmation de l’identité sexuée et sexuelle : éléments
d’analyse de la division sexuée de l’orientation. L’orientation scolaire et
professionnelle, 4, 485-494.
Rethinking corpora: Multidimensional annotation of PolishGerman bilingual speech in the LangGener corpus
Irena Prawdzic (Institute of Slavic Studies, Polish Academy of Sciences) and Carolin Centner
(University of Regensburg)
In our speech, we want to show the functions and research possibilities of the Polish-German
speech corpus of bilinguals. It consists of over 78 hours of recordings with 58 speakers in Polish
and German. It is divided into two groups of speakers: The first one was born in the 1930’s and
lives in Poland (Generation Poland) while the second one was born in the 1950’s in the same
region and now lives in Germany (Generation Germany). In Generation Poland L1 is German
and Polish was obtained as L2. Generation Germany’s L1 is Polish and German became L2
after migration to Germany, mostly in the 1980’s.
While there is a growing interest in corpus linguistics, subdisciplines as e.g.
sociolinguistics still very poorly interact with corpus linguistics (Kendall, 2011, p. 362).
Therefore, it is the aim of the LangGener corpus to investigate natural spoken data of GermanPolish bilinguals on several layers of interpretation. The corpus is richly annotated,
sociolinguistically as well as from a contact linguistic point of view.
The sociolinguistic annotation is based on three categories: stage of development,
language domain and conceptualization of bilingualism. It will help to answer the question of
what connects and what differs the linguistic biographies of speakers from particular regions
82
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
and generations. Therefore, it will also be possible to investigate whether and what
sociolinguistic factors influence the use of languages of bilingual people.
Grammatical annotation will allow to study the influence of both languages on each
other. The annotation follows the basic distinction between replication of matter (MAT) and
pattern (PAT), where “only the patterns from one language are replicated, […], while the form
itself is not borrowed” (Sakel, 2008). It is based on the following categories: pattern replication,
other deviations from the norm, self correction, word order, code switching and replication of
matter.
In our presentation, we would like to show a hands-on analysis using the LangGener
corpus. Several research questions will be answered using sociolinguistic as well as contact
linguistic information obtained by corpus queries.
The project LangGener is financed by the NCN and DFG.
References:
Kendall, T. (2011). Corpora from a sociolinguistic perspective. Revista Brasileira de
Linguística Aplicada, Belo Horizonte, v.11, n.2, pp. 361-189.
Sakel, J. (2008). Types of loan. Matter and pattern. In Y. Matras, & J. Sakel, Grammatical
Borrowing in Cross-Linguistic Perspective. Empirical approraches to language typology
(pp. 15-31). Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Identifying speakers of Bambaiya Hindi and social perception
towards the dialect
Natika Puthran
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
Bambaiya Hindi is spoken as a lingua franca in the city of Mumbai in India and is often viewed
by its inhabitants as a low-prestige dialect (Gopalakrishnan, 2006), associated with poverty.
The purpose of this study was twofold: to identify the speakers of Bambaiya Hindi and the
strata of society they belong to, and to investigate the current social attitudes towards the dialect.
The available literature, which is scarce, states that this dialect is connected mainly to the lower
classes (Chatterji, 1980; Apte, 1974). However, (1) recent literature (Mazumdar, 2001), (2) my
own observations as a speaker of this dialect who grew up in Mumbai as well as informal
interviews with locals and (3) results of a pilot study interviewing 6 male speakers of Bambaiya
Hindi, spanning two generations, states otherwise. It becomes evident that the dialect is used
across the entire social class spectrum in varied social interactions (Nijman, 2006) and is not
limited to its use in contexts of interaction with the lower classes. Despite this, Bambaiya Hindi
is still looked down upon if used in official or educational situations and speakers are
discouraged from using it. People disdain the dialect yet speakers continue to use it as it offers
them a sense of comfort and community (Rajgor, 2018). In conclusion, although Bambaiya
Hindi continues to have an unfavourable image in society, speakers understand its limitations
and use it accordingly; making it clear that the usage of this dialect is a combination of diverse
communicative contexts and one’s social situations, not just one or the other.
83
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Apte, Mahadev L. (1974): Pidginization of a lingua franca: A linguistic analysis of Hindi-Urdu
spoken in Bombay. In International Journal of Dravidian Languages Vol. 1, No.3, pp.
21-41.
Chatterji, Buddhadeva (1980): A Contrastive Study of Khariboli and Bazar (Pidgin) Hindi of
Bombay and that of Calcutta. Poona (Pune): University of Poona.
Gopalakrishnan, V. S. (2006): Verbal Assault of 'Bambaiya' Hindi. Worli: DNA.
Mazumdar, Ranjani (2001): Figure of the 'Tapori': Language, Gesture and Cinematic City. In
Economic and Political Weekly Vol. 36, No. 52, pp. 4872–4880.
Nijman, Jan (2006): Mumbai's Mysterious Middle Class. In International Journal of Urban and
Regional Research Vol. 30, No. 4, pp. 758–775.
Rajgor, Hardik (2018): My Bambaiyya Hindi is Better Than Your North-Indian Hindi. Arre.
English and coloniality of language: Rethinking culture, language
and identity
Hamza R'boul
(Public University of Navarre)
Enduring colonial structures and power imbalances have enabled English to assume an
unprecedented authority around the world. For instance, linguistic imperialism is linked to the
expansionism of America and the global spread of the English language. This is manifested in
the scholarship as multiple studies have emphasized the need for more critical English
Language teaching that is not only informed by current theories of interculturality but also
decolonial. This presentation argues that English language teaching methodology should reflect
a critical understanding of the global spread of English and its potential influence on local
cultures, languages and epistemologies. Another important element is English is often used to
mediate intercultural interactions; this is particularly indicative of English can either contribute
to the maintenance of inequities or be used to alleviate the imbalances among different spaces.
While different regions including Africa, Asian, Latin America and the Caribbean are still
struggling to overcome their postcolonial positionality, teaching the English language with the
aim of encouraging students to embrace native speakers’ values, attitudes and ways of knowing
would result in the marginalization of local languages and identities. That is why is necessary
to consider local culture, language and identity as central to English language teaching
methodology especially with the current status of English as a lingua franca. The aim is to offer
some insights that situate the discussion of English language teaching methodology within the
broader issues of coloniality and power structures. For example, importing theories from innercircle countries and use them in less popular cultures is likely to exert a negative impact on
local norms and identities. This presentation will offer a critical discussion of the intersection
of global English and the coloniality of language as promoted by power relations and neoliberal
tendencies. Also, it will present some practical guidelines that reflect the development of critical
theory and praxis in English language teaching methodology.
84
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
A diachronic study of the speech of New Haven and Hartford
counties
Mateusz D.S. Rogalski
(University of Rzeszów)
Although numerous studies have thoroughly investigated much of Eastern and Northern New
England (cf. Nagy & Roberts, 2004; Leddy-Cecere et al., 2011; Stanford et al., 2012), the
absence of large metropolitan cities in Southwestern New England may be the reason for a
relative lack of research conducted in Connecticut (Boberg, 2001, p. 3). The proposed paper,
titled “A Diachronic Study of the Speech of New Haven and Hartford Counties”, aims to fill a
void in current American dialect geography and constitutes a stepping-stone to furthering
linguistic research in Southwestern New England. The area of interest is the two central
Connecticut counties of New Haven and Hartford, which together constitute nearly half of the
state population at over 1.7 million inhabitants and span an industrial corridor from Long Island
Sound to western Massachusetts (U.S. Census Bureau, 2019). This in-depth analysis of two
neighboring New England speech communities, which originated with the New Haven Colony,
founded in 1638 at Quinnipiac, and the Connecticut River Colony, founded in 1636 at Hartford,
focuses on the long-lasting founder effect as well as the rapidly changing linguistic diversity of
the Northeast (Johnson, 1976, pp. 18-19; Wolfram & Schilling, 2016, pp. 29, 102; Stanford,
2019, p. 71). Aspects of traditional New England variables, such as rhoticity, START-fronting,
the TRAP-BATH split, the mergers of FATHER-BOTHER and HORSE-HOARSE, as well as
the three-way MARY-MERRY-MARRY and low-back LOT-THOUGHT mergers, are
analyzed (Stanford et al., 2012, p. 131; Stanford, 2019, p. 38). In addition to other contemporary
works and the author’s own sociolinguistic research on the local dialect, the paper utilizes the
seminal Linguistic Atlas of New England by Kurath et al. (1939-1943) as well as the Atlas of
North American English by Labov et al. (2006) to establish both the historical and modern
status of the aforementioned Connecticut speech communities. The Southwestern New England
dialect region continues to be under-represented in sociolinguistic research into the North
American dialects, however, this paper explores the very core of this founding region of
American English.
References:
Boberg, C. (2001). The Phonological Status of Western New England. American Speech, 76(1),
3-29. Duke University Press.
Johnson, M. L. (1976). Yesterday’s Connecticut (Seemann’s Historic States Series No. 6). E.A.
Seemann Publishing, Inc.
Kurath, H., Hanley, M. L., Bloch, B., Lowman, G. S., Jr., & Hansen, M. L. (1939-1943).
Linguistic Atlas of New England (Vol. 1-3). Brown University.
Labov, W., Ash, S., & Boberg, C. (2006). Atlas of North American English. Mouton de Gruyter.
Stanford, J. N. (2019). New England English: Large-Scale Acoustic Sociophonetics and
Dialectology. Oxford University Press.
Stanford, J. N., Leddy-Cecere, T. A., & Baclawski, K. P., Jr. (2012). Farewell to the Founders:
Major Dialect Changes Along the East-West New England Border. American Speech,
87(2), 126-169. Duke University Press.
Leddy-Cecere, T., Baclawski, K., Jr., Walker, N., & Stanford, J. (2011). New England
Borderlands: A New Investigation of the East-West Boundary. University of
85
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 17.2 Selected Papers from NWAV 39,
Article 15, 125-134. University of Pennsylvania.
Nagy, N. & Roberts, J. (2004). New England Phonology. A Handbook of Varieties of English,
Vol. 1, Phonology, 270-281. Mouton de Gruyter.
United States Census Bureau. (2019). U.S. Census Bureau QuickFacts: Hartford County,
Connecticut; New Haven County, Connecticut; Connecticut (V2019) [Data set]. Census
Bureau
QuickFacts.
https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/fact/table/hartfordcountyconnecticut,newhavencou
ntyconnecticut,CT
Wolfram, W. & Schilling, N. (2016). American English: Dialects and Variation (3rd ed.). John
Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Language variation and change in Romanian-speaking Bayash
communities in Hungary
Mátyás Rosenberg
(Research Institute for Linguistics, Budapest)
There are thousands of Gypsies in Hungary and its neighboring countries living mainly in small
settlements or villages; their mother tongue is Romanian or they spoke the language before they
had shifted to speaking other languages. They are known as “Bayash”, “Vlach”, “tub-makers”
or “spoon-carvers”, referring either to their language or their traditional occupation (cf.
Kovalcsik, 1996, p. 77). According to earlier research, Bayash were divided into three ethnic
subgroups, all self-identifying as Bayash, forming a relatively coherent community, and
speaking clearly connected language variants. However, the latest research suggests that none
of these statements hold true in any case, since the communities using dialectal Romanian
language as their mother tongue are much more heterogeneous and complex (Arató, 2013, p.
49; Bodó, 2009, p. 84).
The aim of there presentation is to introduce and assess preliminary findings from a
unique and ongoing dialectological field-based research, conducted among the Bayash
communities in Hungary and the neighboring countries which began in 2014. The study makes
a contribution to the existing body of literature on language, dialects, and sub-dialects of Bayash
people, allowing further cross-country comparison with data describing various Bayash
communities residing in different parts of each country.
This research uncovers previously unstudied terrain of Bayash dialects, and due to
lacking data, there fieldwork findings truly enrich academic understanding of the subject. The
goal is to further classify the dialects, explicitly revealing the relationship between those, as
well as compare the Bayash dialects to various Romanian dialects. The presentation relies on
data collected during fieldwork; namely, using audio-recordings of elicited speech, direct
conversations, structured around a linguistic questionnaire that has been prepared for the
documentation of the Bayash dialects, and casual conversations. After discussing the findings
regarding the active use of language, these results are juxtaposed with the content of published
sources from different countries (grammatical descriptions, dictionaries, folk songs, folk tales,
etc.). Overall, I go beyond linguistics by shedding light on cultural and social characteristics of
the Bayash communities, explaining how and why these groups assimilate linguistically and
socially.
86
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Arató, M. (2013b.) A beás nyelvjáráskutatás előzetes tapasztalatai. In M. Arató & T. Cserti
Csapó (Eds.), Romológia „akkor és most”– romológusok második szakmai
konferenciája. Konferenciakötet. Gypsy Studies – Cigány Tanulmányok 30 (pp. 47–65).
Pécsi Tudományegyetem, Bölcsészettudományi Kar, Neveléstudományi Intézet,
Romológia és Nevelésszociológia Tanszék – Romológiai Kutatóközpont – Wlislocki
Henrik Szakkollégium.
Bodó, Cs. (2009). Nyelvi megkülönböztetés és etnicitás. Nyelvi ideológiák a magyarországi
cigányok tudományos osztályozásában. Néprajzi Látóhatár 18(3), 66–85.
Guțu Romalo, V. (2005). Aspecte ale evoluției limbii române. Editura Humanitas Educațional.
Kovalcsik, K. (1996). Roma or Bayash Identity? The Music of the “Arďelan” Bayashes in
Hungary. The World of Music 38(1), Music of the Roma, 77–93.
Pană Dindelegan, G. (Ed). (2013). The Grammar of Romanian. Oxford University Press.
Sorescu Marinković, A. (2008). The Bayash in Croatia: Romanian vernaculars in Baranja and
Medjimurje. In Biljana S. & Tijana A. (Eds.), The Romance Balkans (pp. 173−225).
Balkanološki Institut.
Some remarks on the relation of evaluativity and indefiniteness on
the example of Czech compound indefinite pronouns
Martina Rybova
(Universität Regensburg)
According to the recent research of evaluativity (respectively emotionality, appraisal theory or
stance), the ability of evaluation is one of the important human skills (Hunston, 2011;
Veselovská, 2017). It is generally assumed that in language, one is able to evaluate explicitly
(with lexical or grammatical means) and implicitly (with neutral, non-expressive means)
(Hunston, 2011). Among the explicitly evaluative means e.g. the interjections, pejorative
lexicon, some affixes indicating evaluation (-ei in German; Dudenredaktion, o. J.) are usually
mentioned. However, compound indefinite pronouns (CIP; e.g. bůhvíkdo ‚god knows who‘ in
Czech or neizvestno što ‚unknown what‘ in Russian) also play their role in obviously evaluative
texts, but the evaluation component of their meaning is more or less left aside in relevant
literature.
CIPs are also problematic for other reason. Pronouns themselves are usually considered
as a closed word class. Though, the CIPs are results of different grammaticalization processes
and their repertoire seems to be open (Fisun, 2016). For Russian and Polish, the CIPs have been
described by many researchers (Padučeva, 2015; Wierzbicka-Piotrowska, 2007). In other Slavic
languages, however, the CIPs were not analysed in depth yet and in relevant scientific literature,
there are only few traditional CIPs mentioned (e.g. in Petr et al, 1986, for Czech). Also a brief
corpus analysis shows that the spectre is much broader.
The paper deals with preliminary results of research of the relation between evaluativity
and indefiniteness on the example of selected Czech CIPs with a different degree of
grammaticalization (e. g. bůhvíkdo, kdejaký ‚whatever‘ and jak (je) libo ‚as you please‘) and
their counterparts in Russian and Polish.
Firstly, the paper shows the results of corpus analysis in a webcorpus czTenTen. Using
the typical collocates of the researched pronouns, it will be demonstrated that the selected CIPs
tend to occur in evaluative environment. However, such method reveals only the explicit
87
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
evaluation. Secondly, the results of a corpus analysis of translation texts (Intercorp) will be
presented. This method provides an interesting tool to supply the previous method as it
demonstrates, whether the translators also consider some non-explicit evaluative statements
with CIPs and/or their collocates as evaluative or not. The aim of the paper is to discuss the
pros and cons of the used methodology.
References:
Dudenredaktion
(o.
J.).
-ei.
In
Duden
online.
Retrieved
from
https://www.duden.de/rechtschreibung/_ei.
Fisun, R. (2016). Об определенности неопреленных местоимений (Вестник Московского
государственного областного университета). Русская филология, 5, 158–170.
Haspelmath, M. (1997). Indefinite pronouns (Oxford Studies in Typology and Linguistic
Theory). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hunston, S. (2011). Corpus approaches to evaluation: Phraseology and evaluative language.
London: Routledge.
Padučeva, E. V. (2015) Snjataja utverditel´nost´ i neveridiktal´nost´ (na primere russkich
mestoimenij otricatel´noj poljarnosti). Russian Linguistics, 2(39), 129–162.
Petr, J. et al. (1986). Mluvnice češtiny 2. Tvarosloví. Praha: Academia.
Veselovská, K. (2017). Sentiment Analysis in Czech. Praha: Institute of Formal and Applied
Linguistics.
Wierzbicka-Piotrowska, E. (2007) Bóg wie kto, Bóg wie co, czyli o frazach, które mogą pełnić
funkcję zaimków nieokreślonych. Poradnik językowy, 4, 80–91.
Translating otherness – experimental Polish subtitles for the
Lithuanian local dialect of Puńsk in Poland
Tomas Senda
(University of Warsaw)
The presence of non-standard language varieties in audiovisual translation (AVT) is often a
challenge for translators. This is especially true in subtitling, frequently referred to as a
“constrained translation” (Titford, 1982, as cited in Díaz-Cintas, 2004). On the one hand, there
is a tendency to highlight the presence of language variation in subtitles (Díaz-Cintas, Remael,
2007), but on the other hand, the non-standard is often erased (Díaz-Cintas, 2012).
In this study, we report on the results of a study on the reception of experimental Polish
subtitles for the Lithuanian local dialect of Puńsk in Poland, created in the spirit of “positively
abusive” subtitling (Nornes, 1999). The “abusiveness” defies norms and routine practices,
whereas “positiveness” advocates inventive and experimental approaches. Experimental
subtitles aimed to render the linguistic otherness in grammar, vocabulary, and phonetics by
altering the standard language – its morphological, lexical, and visual traits.
134 survey participants – monolingual Polish and bilingual Polish-Lithuanian speakers
– evaluated experimental (ES) and standard subtitles (SS) for excerpts of a Lithuanian news
programme subtitled in Polish. Participants answered questions related to their linguistic
background, then evaluated both types of subtitles in terms of their comprehensibility,
rendering of the dialectal features, faithfulness to speakers’ speech, perception of speakers’
language and education level, preference for the technique used to render the dialect traits. The
answers on the linguistic background of the bilingual Lithuanians living in Poland showed a
great complexity in the perception of their native language. The ES appeared to be more faithful
88
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
to the speakers’ speech and render the traits of the local dialect of Puńsk better than SS.
However, they were perceived as incorrect and less comprehensible. A vital factor for the
speakers’ perception turned out to be the familiarity with the original soundtrack language;
especially in the case of SS, Polish and Polish-Lithuanian respondents evaluated the speakers
differently.
The current linguistic situation worldwide comprises many interchanging languages and
language variations. This heterogeneity also appears in audiovisual products – films or TV news
– and triggers new, unconventional approaches in translation. The issue of non-standard
minority language transfer in AVT seems more topical now than never.
References:
Díaz-Cintas, J. (2004). Subtitling: the long journey to academic acknowledgement.
The Journal of Specialised Translation, 1, 50-68.
Díaz-Cintas, J., Remael A. (2007). Audiovisual translation: Subtitling.
St. Jerome Publishing.
Díaz-Cintas, J. (2012). Subtitling. Theory, practice and research. In C. Milán and F. Bartrina
(Eds.) The Routledge Handbook of Translation Studies (pp. 285-289). Routledge.
Nornes, A. M. (1999). For an Abusive Subtitling. Film Quarterly, 52(3), 17-34.
Methods of evaluating the semantic equivalence of a poetic text
Polina Sikatskaya
(Novosibirsk State University)
There are some texts which have been translated in another language a lot of times. It is
particularly typical for ancient poetry: for instance, some of Horace’s Odes have up to 40–60
russian translations. Different reasons are possible when composing a novel translation of a text
that has been formerly translated many times: some translators are not satisfied with the
precision of existing translations, the other try to create new interpretations. In all cases, the
difference between translations can be very minor or rather consciously discernible. We claim
that the degree of the translational equivalence of such texts can be measured and evaluated
comparably.
Our investigation is dedicated to the comparative evaluation of the translational
equivalence on the base of one of Horace’s Odes, namely «Ad Leuconoën» (I:XI), consisting
of 8 lines, and its 58 translations into Russian.
A detailed study of the problem has revealed the impossibility of postulating a united
equivalence scale; at least for the translations of a Latin poetic text, the metrical, cultural and
semantic equivalence must be counted separately. So, we created an original method of
counting equivalence percentage with each of these scales. Here we are going to describe one
of them, namely the semantic equivalence.
Our method is divided into two blocks - semantic analysis and arithmetic calculation.
First, in the Horace’s poem, we determine 30 key words that are essential for the semantic
authenticity of the text, and then, with lexicographic sources, we distinguish actual semes for
each of the key words in the original text and in each translation. We then compare the semes
and register the differences in the lists of original and translation semes. That allows us to
specify a percentile of equivalence for each of the keywords. After that, using the arithmetic
mean, the coefficient of semantic equivalence is determined for each of the translations.
89
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
High semantic equivalence does not always correlate with its poetic perception (for
example, the most prevalent Russian translation of the expression carpe diem is not the most
semantically equivalent). Moreover, less modern translations tend to have less equivalence.
References:
Alekseeva, I. (2004) Vvedenie v perevodovedenie (Introduction to translation). St. Petersburg:
Faculty of Philology, St. Petersburg State University; Moscow: Publishing Center
"Academia".
Komissarov, V. (1990) Teoriya perevoda: lingvisticheskie aspekty. (Theory of translation:
linguistic aspects). Moscow: Vysshaja shkola.
Latyšev, L. (1981) Kurs perevoda (ekvivalentnost’ i sposoby ejo dostizheniya) (Course of
translation (equivalence and ways of achieving it)). Moscow: Mezhdunarodnye
otnoshenija.
Learning academic words through writing: Can an increase in
cognitive load affect task involvement?
Breno Silva, Katarzyna Kutylowska and Agnieszka Otwinowska
(University of Warsaw)
Laufer and Hulstijn's (2001) Involvement Load Hypothesis (ILH) predicts the lexical learning
potential of tasks. According to the ILH, writing sentences (SW) with keywords and writing
compositions (CW) with keywords should yield similar incidental vocabulary acquisition. Four
studies compared the learning yielded by these two tasks. Among these, Kim (2008) confirmed
the hypothesis while Zou (2017) found higher lexical gains for CW. Yet, Zou's participants
sacrificed text quality to focus on the keywords, possibly due to cognitive overload (Kellogg,
1990). Thus, we hypothesize the amount of lexical learning following CW tasks depends on
writers’ capacity to allocate attentional resources: Writing essays may overwhelm learners’
attentional resources, forcing them to either focus on the essay-writing process (thus decreasing
lexical learning), or on the words, as in Zou’s.
To verify this, we conducted a quasi-experiment with 39 Polish advanced-level English
majors. We selected 20 keywords controlled for part of speech, length, frequency, and
concreteness. Using these, participants produced either sentences (n=22) or 60-minute essays
(n=17). In a pretest-posttest design, we measured lexical learning via an adapted Vocabulary
Knowledge Scale (Wesche & Paribakht, 1996) and a free association test. First, all participants
wrote a control essay (without keywords). Then, SW learners wrote two sets of 10 sentences,
one keyword per sentence; CW participants wrote two essays, each with 10 keywords
(counterbalanced). Cognitive load was measured via textual measures derived from the control
essay and the two CW essays. These measures were holistic scores, number of errors (controlled
for text length), and words per minute. Two raters counted the errors and marked the essays
(using the TOEFL criteria). To ensure learning was incidental, participants completed a
questionnaire on their out-of-class exposure to the keywords and perceptions of the study.
Generalized linear mixed models found that SW yielded more learning than CW, which
contradicts the predictions of the ILH. The measures of cognitive load showed that control
essays were written faster and were more accurate than the CW essays, thus demonstrating that
keyword-use taxed essay-writers’ cognitive resources. We hypothesize that this increase in
cognitive load reduced lexical learning; therefore, CW yielded less learning than SW.
90
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
Kellogg, R. T. (1990). Effectiveness of prewriting strategies as a function of task demands. The
American Journal of Psychology, 103(3), 327-342.
Kim, Y. (2008). The role of task-induced involvement and learner proficiency in L2 vocabulary
acquisition. Language Learning, 58(2), 285-325.
Laufer, B., & Hulstijn, J. H. (2001). Incidental vocabulary acquisition in a second language:
The construct of task-induced involvement. Applied Linguistics, 22(1), 1-26.
Wesche, M., & Paribakht, T. M. (1996). Assessing vocabulary knowledge: Depth vs breadth.
Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 13-40.
Zou, D. (2017). Vocabulary acquisition through cloze exercises, sentence-writing and
composition-writing: Extending the evaluation component of the involvement load
hypothesis. Language Teaching Research, 21(1), 54-75.
Challenges in researching syntactic representations in the bilingual
mind
Anna Skałba
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
Researching grammatical knowledge of bilinguals has to a considerable extent relied on the
acceptability judgement task (Keating & Jegerski, 2015). However, it cannot be used to test the
shared-syntax (Hartsuiker et al., 2004; Jacob et al., 2017) vs. separate-syntax (de Bot, 1992;
Ullman, 2001) hypotheses for the bilingual mind, because the task fails to tap into unconscious
processes occurring on-line, i.e. in real time. The development of psycholinguistics has led to
the growth of on-line methods and paradigms investigating the organisation of grammatical
knowledge in the bilingual mind. Nevertheless, each of them presents a number of difficulties.
Therefore, the presentation aims to outline challenges related to testing the shared-syntax and
separate-syntax hypotheses, and to offer potential solutions.
For instance, a frequently-used method providing a direct insight into syntactic
processing is eye-tracking during reading. Despite it being a powerful tool, the researcher faces
a challenge of selecting only the most appropriate measures in order to avoid inflating the risk
of type I error, as well as of controlling for a number of factors while preparing target stimuli
(Godfroid, 2020).
In turn, the main on-line paradigm used in bilingual research of syntax is cross-linguistic
syntactic priming. However, its scope is limited because its current usage is restricted to
constructions with an alternative structure expressing the same meaning. It may also be
impossible to state with confidence whether the observed effects result from purely structural
elements, or lexical repetition, thematic roles, animacy (McDonough & Trofimovich, 2009).
Recently, Declerck et al. (2019) have also used the Rapid Parallel Visual Presentation
paradigm to test these hypotheses by investigating sentence superiority effect in sequences of
words from two different languages. Nonetheless, this paradigm entails the selection of target
sequences formally equivalent in the languages under investigation, whose length cannot
exceed working memory capacity.
All these factors contribute to the relative difficulty of investigating the representation
of syntax in the bilingual mind. The presentation will not only review the so-far used methods
and paradigms to highlight their advantages and disadvantages, but it will also attempt to
propose solutions to some of the discussed challenges.
91
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
References:
De Bot, K. (1992). A bilingual production model: Levelt’s ‘Speaking’ model adapted. Applied
Linguistics, 13, 1-24.
Declerck, M., Wen, Y., Snell, J., Meade G., & Grainger, J. (2019). Unified syntax in the
bilingual mind. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(1), 149-154.
Godfroid, A. (2020). Eye Tracking in Second Language Acquisition and Bilingualism. A
Research Synthesis and Methodological Guide. New York: Routledge.
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between
languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals.
Psychological Science, 15, 409-414.
Jacob, G., Katsika, K., Family, N., & Allen, S. E. 2017. The role of constituent order and level
of embedding in cross-linguistic structural priming, Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition 20: 269-282.
Keating, G., & Jegerski, J. (2015). Experimental designs in sentence processing research: A
methodological review and user’s guide. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(1),
1-32.
McDonough, K., & Trofimovich, P. (2009). Using priming methods in second language
research. New York: Routledge.
Ullman, M. T. (2001). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language:
The declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 105-122.
Modern approach to language learning methodology: eLearning,
traditional learning and hybrid learning
Anita Skóra
(Jan Kochanowski University, Kielce)
There is no doubt that the development of IT industry has had an enormous impact on nearly
every field of life. It revolutionized the way we communicate and function as a society.
Therefore, it is hardly surprising that it shed a new light on the subject of education. The advent
of new technologies led to the emergence of e-learning and hybrid platforms for teaching. This,
in turn, transpired to be remarkably valuable at the time of the global epidemic situation that
emerged in 2020.
The prime objective of the presentation is to answer the question concerning the
effectiveness and the function of e-learning and hybrid learning in the process of education.
The study reflects on the divergence between the attitudes of the scholars that are completely
against the implementation of technical novelties and those who are very enthusiastic about
them and look at them through rose-tinted glasses. Hence, the research analyzes not solely the
opportunities that digital and blended courses may offer, but also the difficulties that they can
conceivably generate. However, it does not end with the definition of problems, but attempts to
offer possible solutions. The issues that are taken into consideration involve subjects such as
the role of a teacher and a student, the management of a class and, perhaps the most importantly,
tools that are useful while developing students’ linguistic knowledge and skills.
Despite the fact that the research is carried out from an English teacher perspective, its
results and conclusions can also be applied by any other professional in the field of foreign
language teaching in the whole world. What is more, the presentation constitutes an input in the
discussion of rethinking the classical form of education comparing it with more modern
92
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
methods in order to discover which of their elements are the most beneficial for a learner in any
particular scenarios.
References:
Chew, E., Turner, D. A., & Jones, N. (2010). In love and war: blended learning theories for
computer scientists and educationists. Handbook of research on hybrid learning
models, 1-23. Information Science Reference.
Harmer, J. (2001). The practice of English language teaching, 3rd edition (Longman
Handbooks for Language Teachers) (3rd ed.). Pearson Education ESL.
Holmes, B., & Gardner, J. (2006). e-Learning: Concepts and practice. SAGE Publications Ltd.
Horton, W. K. (2001). Evaluating e-learning. Alexandria, Va: American Society for Training
& Development.
Lin, O. (2008). Student Views of Hybrid Learning. Journal of Computing in Teacher
Education, 25(2), 57–66.
Ordóñez, . P. P., Tennyson, R. D., & Lytras, M. D. (2015). Assessing the role of mobile
technologies and distance learning in higher education. IGI Global.
Rossen, E., & Hartley, D. E. (2001). Basics of e-learning: Training technology. Alexandria,
Va: ASTD.
Thorne, K. (2003). Blended learning: How to integrate online & traditional learning. London:
Kogan Page.
Walsh, J. (2011). Information literacy instruction: Selecting an effective model. Oxford:
Chandos Publishing.
The pragmatic functions of hyperbola and litotes based on a
discourse completion tests’ results among Hungarian university
students
Éva Szabó
(University of Szeged)
This research was motivated by the results of a discourse completion test (DCT) which
purposed to analyze impression management strategies and tactics in the language use of
university students. This test was filled by 72 students from University of Szeged and contained
21 different situations. These situations were aimed to trigger various impression management
motivations treated in social psychology and linguistic pragmatics.
Beside the strategies in Leary’s (1995) and Nemesi’s (2011) taxonomies there were
other strategies identified in DCTs to form or maintain impressions such as (1) concealment or
mitigation of deficiencies and mistakes of the individuum (2) or of the partner, (3) lie, (4)
emotional gapping from the partner’s attitude and viewpoint. In addition, the occurrences of
hyperbole and litotes were frequent. The definitions of hyperbole and litotes are diversified in
rhetoric because many approaches consider them as tropes or figures, but their division is based
on semantics. The present paper followed the pragmatic approaches and manages them as
figures because their effects are founded on forms, syntactic and semantic variations. The paper
aimed to reveal the functions of hyperbole and litotes because their role depends on pragmatic
and contextual factors. The main functions of these phenomena set up with heuristic methods
which also take into consideration the contextual and sociopragmatic factors. The results
supplemented Nemesi’s (2010) systematization, according to the data, we could differentiate
the following functions of hyperbole: (1) persuasion, (2) expression of speaker’s attitude, (3)
93
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
self-presentation and (4) flattery. While researchers claim that flattery relates to selfpresentation, it can also satisfy other aims such as influencing the partner to reach a given aim
therefore it should be separated from self-presentation. In case of litotes, there were two new
functions identified, namely (1) concealment the deficiencies of the individuum or the partner,
and (2) emphasizing.
Nevertheless, the use of hyperbole or litotes had different pragmatic functions, they
emerged in various impression management strategies, even if their borders were vague their
use in daily conversations forms the participants’ social perception.
References:
Leary, M. R. (1995). Self-presentation: Impression Management and Interpersonal Behavior.
Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Nemesi, A. L. (2010). Data-gathering methods in research on hyperbole. In Németh T, Enikő–
Bibok, Károly (Eds.) The Role of Data at the Semantics–Pragmatics Interface (pp. 381–
417). New York, Berlin, De Gruyter Mouton.
Nemesi, A. L. (2011). Nyelv, nyelvhasználat, kommunkáció. Hét tanulmány. Budapest: Loisir
Kiadó.
Slovjansko Esperanto: How can the Interslavic language aid the
collective Slavic identity?
Katarzyna Szyszka
(University of Warsaw)
The notion of national and international identity is inextricably connected with language and
its potential to articulate the concepts and phenomena characteristic for its users and their
culture. Yet, just as is often the case, personal identity is oftentimes strongly influenced, or even
defined by one’s closest relationships and upbringing, so is it possible to talk about a collective
identity of a language family. This is the philosophical backdrop against which the concept of
the Interslavic language will be introduced and discussed in this presentation.
As a zonal constructed language, Interslavic’s main potential is crossing the
communication gap between users of all Slavic languages, without the need to learn it. Its
common comprehensibility among Slavs makes it an ideal alternative lingua franca for them.
What is problematic is the language’s little publicity and the fact that it is still under intensive
development.
The author will begin by introducing the circumstances of Interslavic’s creation, the
current state of its development, and the factors that influence its spread, use, and shape in
addition to the awareness of its existence by potential users.
The factors discussed have been divided into three strands. The first includes initiatives
undertaken by the academic community such as the writing of textbooks, the creation of an
online dictionary and the SLOVJANI.info online journal, and the organization of the
Conference on Interslavic Language. The second strand comprises the cultural works created
in Interslavic, such as Antoine de Saint-Exupéry’s Little Prince translated by Jan van
Steenbergen in 2017 or the 2019 film Painted Bird by Václav Marhoul. The third strand
summarizes the activities of the community of users and enthusiasts of Interslavic, mainly
initiatives within a Facebook group devoted to the language.
94
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
In the final section, the author will ponder upon the legitimacy of developing the
Interslavic language and its usefulness, as well as the possibility of its popularization as a Slavic
lingua franca and building a plausible collective interslavic identity around it.
References:
Anderson, B. (1991). Imagined communities: Reflections on the origin and spread of
nationalism (Revised edition). Verso.
Baldwin, J. (1997). If Black English isn’t a Language, Then Tell Me, What Is? The Black
Scholar, 27(1), 5–6.
Barber, K. (2004, December 6). Old Church Slavonic and the ‘Slavic Identity’. Languages and
Nationalism.
CISLa 2020. (n.d.). CISLa 2020. Retrieved 6 November 2020, from
http://conference.interslavic-language.org/2020/
de Saint-Exupéry, A. (2016). Maly Princ (J. van Steenbergen, Trans.).
http://steen.free.fr/interslavic/maly_princ.html
Interslavic—Medžuslovjansky—Меджусловјанскы | Facebook. (n.d.). Retrieved 6 November
2020, from https://www.facebook.com/groups/interslavic
Kocór, M., Yordanova, L., van Steenbergen, J., & Merunka, V. (2017). Zonal Constructed
Language and Education Support of e-Democracy – The Interslavic Experience. In S.
K. Katsikas & V. Zorkadis (Eds.), E-Democracy – Privacy-Preserving, Secure,
Intelligent E-Government Services (Vol. 792, pp. 15–30). Springer International
Publishing.
Marhoul, V. (2019, September 12). Nabarvené ptáče [War drama]. Bioscop.
Melucci, A. (1989). Nomads of the Present. Social Movements and Individual Needs in
Contemporary Society. Temple University Press.
Merunka, V. (2019, 19.07). The Interslavic language: An opportunity for promotion of
intangible heritage. SHARING CULTURES 2019 - 6th International Conference on
Intangible Heritage, Guimarães, Portugal.
Merunka, V., & Molhanec, M. (2016). Neoslavonic Language Zonal Language Constructing:
Challenge, Experience, Opportunity to the 21st Century. Proceedings of the 2015 2nd
International Conference on Education, Language, Art and Intercultural
Communication. 2015 2nd International Conference on Education, Language, Art and
Intercultural Communication (ICELAIC-15), Kaifeng, China.
Merunka, V., & van Steenbergen, J. (2017). Slovjanska kulturna diplomacija—SWOT analiza,
strategija i taktika do budučnosti.
Merunka, V., van Steenbergen, J., Yordanova, L., & Kocór, M. (2019). The Interslavic
language as a tool for supporting e-democracy in Central and Eastern Europe. Int. J.
Electronic Governance, 11(3/4), 29.
Norton, B. (2013). Identity and Language Learning: Extending the Conversation. Multilingual
Matters.
Shohamy, E. G., & Gorter, D. (Eds.). (2009). Linguistic landscape: Expanding the scenery (1st
ed). Routledge.
Silberstein, S. (2003). Teaching Culture: Imagined Communities and National Fantasies in the
O. J. Simpson Case. Journal of Language, Identity & Education, 2(4), 319–330.
SLOVJANI.info—Medžuslovjansky časopis—Interslavic journal. (n.d.). SLOVJANI.
Retrieved 6 November 2020, from http://slovjani.info/
van Steenbergen, J. (2018). Język międzysłowiański jako lingua franca dla Europy Środkowej.
Język. Komunikacja. Informacja, 13, 47–61.
95
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
van Steenbergen, J., & Merunka, V. (2018, 08.09). The Interslavic Language: An Opportunity
for the Tourist Branch. International Symposium on Advancements in Tourism,
Recreation and Sports Sciences. Proceedings Book. International Symposium on
Advancements in Tourism, Recreation and Sports Sciences, Montenegro.
Monitoring during intralingual subtitling
Anke Tardel
(Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz, Germersheim)
Intralingual subtitling for the deaf and hard of hearing (SDH) has gained increased importance
in audio visual translation but the process of creating SDH has yet to be investigated thoroughly
with empirical methods. It is imperative to better understand the involved processes so tools,
practices, and training can be adapted accordingly. Established methods from translation
process research – keylogging and eyetracking – seem promising to be applied to the process
of subtitling (Orrego-Carmona et al., 2018). They provide insights on behavioral aspects such
as where subtitlers look, how do they type, and they help understanding the process on a
cognitive level. Gaze measures (total and average fixation duration, fixation count) are
established to indicate cognitive load (Holmqvist et al., 2011; Hvelplund, 2011, 2017). In cases
where these measures can be linked target text quality (conformance with SDH standards), they
inform us about cognitive control mechanisms such as monitoring (Schaeffer et al., 2019).
Within the EU-funded Compass project, a pilot study with eight subtitlers creating SDH
for three 5-minute excerpts of a German documentary was carried out. Gaze and typing
behaviour was recorded to investigate the subtitlers’ attention distribution during subtitling with
FAB Subtitler as well as factors that indicate cognitive overload. With an annotation of errors
in the created subtitles the product data was linked to the process data. Results indicate that
during the subtitling process, subtitlers’ attention is shifted back and forth between monitoring
the content of the evolving subtitles, and the timing and segmentation thereof. It was found that
subtitle reading times were significantly higher on subtitles with errors (subtitles that did not
conform with the style guide). In addition, there was a significant interaction effect for the
subtitle ID in that incorrect subtitles that were created earlier in the process had longer reading
times than correct subtitles but for subtitles created later in the process this difference is not
significant. This suggests that later in the process participants monitoring capacities are possibly
impacted by effects of fatigue or cognitive overload. In the presentation the methodology and
procedure will be explained, and possible interpretations and implications discussed.
References:
Holmqvist, K., Nystrom, M., Andersson, R., Dewhurst, R., Jarodzka, H., & de Weijer, J. (2011).
Eye tracking: A comprehensive guide to methods and measures. Oxford University
Press.
Hvelplund, K. T. (2011). Allocation of Cognitive Resources in Translation. An Eye-tracking
and Key-logging Study (Issue 10).
Hvelplund, K. T. (2017). Eye Tracking and the Process of Dubbing Translation. In J. Díaz
Cintas & K. Nikoli (Eds.), Fast-Forwarding with Audiovisual Translation (pp. 110–
124). Multilingual Matters.
Orrego-Carmona, D., Dutka, L., & Szarkowska, A. (2018). Using translation process research
to explore the creation of subtitles: An eye-tracking study comparing professional and
trainee subtitlers. JoSTrans: The Journal of Specialised Translation, 30, 150–180.
96
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Schaeffer, M. J., Halverson, S. L., & Hansen-Schirra, S. (2019). ‘Monitoring’ in translation:
The role of visual feedback. Translation, Cognition & Behavior, 2(1), 1–34.
Synaesthesia between metonymy and metaphor: The
characterization of olfactory stimuli in German
Máté Tóth
(University of Debrecen)
Verbal synaesthesia is generally considered to be a metaphoric phenomenon involving the
integration of concepts stemming from distinct perceptual domains (cf. Taylor, 1995, p. 139;
Vogt, 2013). For instance, in synaesthetic attribute-noun constructions (e.g. loud colors, warm
tones, sweet voice) the head belonging to a higher sensory domain is conceptualized as if it had
a property in another, lower sensory domain designated by the attribute. In the expression loud
color, the head color is metaphorically construed as something that has the property of being
loud in the auditory domain.
However, with the upsurge of cognitive metonymy research some authors have
proposed a metonymic motivation for synaesthetic expressions (e.g. Barcelona, 2002;
Sadamitsu, 2003). In line with these proposals, I argue in my presentation that (i) a considerable
portion of synaesthetic expressions can be analyzed as metonymically motivated and (ii) they
are based either directly on the co-occurrence or indirectly on the resemblance of sensory
stimuli. For instance, in the expression the green scent of the lawn the color associated with the
lawn is activated to describe its scent; i.e., it is not the concept of SCENT that is understood
metaphorically, as if it were a physical object with COLOR, but a property of the LAWN (its
COLOR) is used as a metonymic reference point to activate a PROPERTY of its SCENT.
Olfaction offers itself as an ideal terrain to study synaesthetic expressions since in most
languages properties describing olfactory stimuli are rather poorly lexicalized (cf. Holz, 2007;
Chernigovskaya & Arshavsky, 2007): when describing smells, we tend to utilize attributes
stemming from other sensory domains. Accordingly, in order to test my hypotheses I present
the results of a corpus study conducted on German synaesthetic attribute-noun constructions.
My results suggest that the heterogeneity of verbal synesthesia cannot be grasped simply
by proposing that it is a metaphorical phenomenon. Some synaesthetic expressions might be
analyzed as metonymies, others as metaphors, whereas it might be also possible that some
would resist an analysis in either vein and would constitute a third intermediary or related
category.
References:
Barcelona, A. (2002). Clarifying and applying the notions of metaphor and metonymy within
cognitive linguistics: An update. In R., Dirven & R., Pörings (Eds.). Metaphor and
Metonymy in Comparison and Contrast (pp. 209-277). Berlin & New York: Mouton de
Gruyter.
Chernigovskaya, T.V. & Arshavsky, V.V. (2007). Olfactory and visual processing and
verbalization: Cross-cultural and neurosemiotic dimensions. In M., Plümacher & P.,
Holz (Eds.). Speaking of Colors and Odors (pp. 227-238). Amsterdam & Philadelphia:
John Benjamins.
97
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Holz, P. (2007). Cognition, olfaction and linguistic creativity: Linguistic synesthesia as poetic
device in cologne advertising. In M., Plümacher & P., Holz (Eds.). Speaking of Colors
and Odors (pp. 185-202). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Sadamitsu, M. (2003). Synaesthesia re-examined: An alternative treatment of smell related
concepts. Osaka University Papers in English Linguistics 8, 109-125.
Taylor, J.R. (1995). Linguistic Categorization. (2nd ed.) Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Vogt, S. (2013). Die Analyse ‘synästhetischer’ Metapher mittels Frames. metaphorik.de 23, 1948.
Retrieved
from
https://www.metaphorik.de/de/journal/23/die-analysesynaesthetischer-metaphern-mittels-frames.html
Translating lexical bundles in legal and political academic genre:
A parallel corpus-driven inquiry of English and Persian languages
Mehrdad Vasheghani Farahani (Leipzig University) and Zahra Ghane (Heinrich Heine
University)
Introduction
The term lexical bundles was coined by Biber and Conrad in 1990 and are defined by Biber et
al. (1999) as “combinations of three or more words which are identified empirically in a corpus
of natural language and show a statistical tendency to re-occur” (p.989). Because of the
patterned structure of lexical bundles and their frequency, these features are found difficult for
translators as they are language specific, complex and belong to different semantic, syntactic
and semiotic entities in two languages (Biber & Barbieri, 2007).
Research Questions
This study will study the way lexical bundles are distributed and translated in law and political
texts in an English-Persian context and to investigate if there are any differences between source
and target languages in terms of lexical bundles structures, equivalence and meaning?
Data Gathering and Corpus Compilation
This study enjoys a corpus-driven approach. The authentic texts selected for corpus are written
by native speakers of English from 2002 to 2109 and are all translated by Persian translators.
Lexical Bundles Category
Biber, Conrad and Cortes (2004) is used.
Structural types of
Description
Example of Bundles
bundles
1 Verb phrase fragments
Incorporate
fragments
of
phrases, including subject pronouns
98
verb What do you think
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
2 Dependent clause
fragments
Include both verb phrase fragments If you don’t want
and
components
of
dependent
clause.
3 Noun phrase and
prepositional
Consist of noun phrases, often with Nothing to do with
a head noun
fragments
4 Stance Expressions
Indicate epistemic, attitudinal, modal, or
There is nothing wrong
evaluative assessments of certainty
5 Discourse Organizers
Signal relationships between previous
I would like to
and forthcoming discourse
6 Referential
Expressions
Reference physical, abstract, or textual
For the sake of
entities
Procedure
To create a parallel corpus in an English-Persian context texts are uploaded into Sketch engine
software. After that lexical bundles in English are identified by concordance lines and N-gram
detector and are classified based on the theoretical framework. The same process is done in the
Persian translations through the concordance lines.
Expecting Results
This study will show how are lexical bundles translated and transferred into Persian language
in terms of syntax, type of equivalence and structural pattern.
References:
Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S. & Finegan. E. (1999). Longman grammar of
spoken and written English. Harlow, UK: Pearson Education.
Biber, D. and F. Barbieri. (2007). ‘Lexical bundles in university spoken and written registers’,
English for Specific Purposes 26 (3), pp. 263–86.
Biber, D., S. Conrad and V. Cortes. (2004). ‘If you look at. . . : lexical bundles in university
99
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
teaching and textbooks’, Applied Linguistics 25 (3), pp. 371–405.
Russian homographic verbs in directed associative experiment
Alena Vodneva
(Saint Petersburg State University)
This work was dedicated to study the influence of the aspect on the perception and processing
of verbs-homographs in associative experiments. The results of the study showed that the
imperfect form dominate in interpretations of stimuli and in associative reactions, regardless of
the choice of the aspect for stimulus.
Despite the fact that aspect in Slavic is overtly and morphologically marked, there are
ambiguous verbs, for which it is hard to define their aspect and meaning without sufficient
context because they differ only in stress patterns. Вased on previous articles (Rusakova & Sai
2003; Ventsov 2011; Riekhakaynen, 2014) we hypothesized that if a certain homographic verb
can be interpreted as both a perfective and an imperfective form, the imperfective one will
prevail in the answers of native speakers (if the verbs are presented without context).
We selected 55 homographic verbs from a list of dictionaries, and then we got examples
of use from the General Internet-Corpora of Russian Language (Belikov et al., 2013), which
we manually marked up to find out meaning`s frequencies. When selecting words, we took into
account words’ frequency, word length, presence of at least two meanings in corpus` entries,
and vocabulary marks (we have excluded outdated or regional vocabulary). We also created
fillers with the same word length but from other parts of speech (55 nouns and 55 adjectives).
Experiment 1 (53 participants) was an oral directed associative experiment consisted of
a task to read out loud words that appeared on the screen and name the first verb that came to
mind. Experiment 2 (109 participants) was a written directed associative experiment consisted
of a task to read words that appeared on the screen and write the first verb that came to mind.
We found out that the participants choose imperfective interpretations of stimuli and
give associative reactions in the imperfect, despite the choice of stimulus` aspect, more often
than perfective ones when reading isolated homographic verbs.
References:
Rusakova, M. V., Sai, S. S. (2003). Aspectual pair of the Russian verb in the individual lexicon
and speech activity. In N. V. Bogdanova (Ed.), Grammatical and lexical semantics (pp.
116–128). St. Petersburg: Publishing house of St. Petersburg State University.
Ventsov, A. V. (2011). The verb and the structure of the mental lexicon. Problems of socioand psycholinguistics, 15, 86–91.Riekhakaynen, E. I. (2014). Aspectual Pairs of Verbs
and Associative Relations in the Mental Lexicon: Evidence from Russian. Abstracts of
section reports of the XLII International Philological Conference, 34, 265–274.
Belikov, V., Kopylov, N., Piperski, A., Selegey, V., Sharoff, S. (2013). Big and diverse is
beautiful: A large corpus of Russian to study linguistic variation. Proc. 8th Web as Corpus
Workshop (WAC-8), 24-29.
100
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Explicit knowledge of verbal morphosyntax in the multilingual EFL
classroom in Norway
Dianna Walla
(Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim)
Many English teachers in Norway remain unprepared to teach in linguistically diverse
classrooms, despite having positive beliefs about multilingualism and a national curriculum
which promotes multilingualism and the use of students’ L1s in learning English (Krulatz &
Dahl, 2016; Šurkalović, 2014). Better knowledge about how heritage language students acquire
L3 English will help teachers and teacher educators to address this gap in knowledge. To that
end, this study builds on previous generative L2 research (Bardel & Falk, 2007; Jensen,
Slabakova, Westergaard, & Lundquist, 2019) in order to examine explicit knowledge of verb
placement and subject-verb agreement in L2/L3 English in the primary EFL classroom in
Norway.
To gain a picture of the linguistic heterogeneity faced by teachers of English in
mainstream classrooms, participating students in grades 5-7 at a primary school in Norway took
a written test examining their knowledge of verb placement and subject-verb agreement. The
former was tested using structures that would trigger V2 in Norwegian, the main language of
schooling, while the latter was tested using present tense sentences that require 3rd person
singular -s. The task types included on the test targeted explicit knowledge more than implicit
knowledge (Ellis, 2005). Using a language background questionnaire, students were divided
into three groups: Monolingual L1 Norwegian (reported speaking only Norwegian at home,
N=87), Heritage Language (reported speaking languages other than Norwegian at home, but
not English, N=29), and English (English was reported as one of the languages spoken at home,
N=14). Representing the linguistic diversity of the school, participants in the Heritage
Language and English groups reported speaking a variety of different L1s. As a result, this
study differs from previous L2/L3 studies looking at specific language combinations, and
instead the reality of the classroom context faced by teachers is underlined. Scores of the three
groups will be compared to see if any differences arise. suggestions for further research and
implications for teacher education based on the results will also be discussed.
Data collection will be completed by January 2021 with a preliminary analysis ready
for presentation and discussion by the conference date.
References:
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y. (2007). The role of the second language in third language acquisition:
the case of Germanic syntax. Second Language Research, 23(4), 459-484.
Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A
Psychometric Study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 141-172.
Jensen, I. N., Slabakova, R., Westergaard, M., & Lundquist, B. (2019). The Bottleneck
Hypothesis in L2 acquisition: L1 Norwegian learners’ knowledge of syntax and
morphology in L2 English. Second Language Research.
Krulatz, A., & Dahl, A. (2016). Baseline assessment of Norwegian EFL teacher preparedness
to work with multilingual students. Journal of Linguistics and Language Teaching, 7(2),
199-217.
Šurkalović, D. (2014). Forbereder grunnskolelærerutdanningen engelsklærere for undervisning
i engelsk som tredjespråk i Norge? Acta didactica Norge [elektronisk ressurs], 8(2), 1717.
101
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Linguistic human categorization: Language boundaries and matters
of belonging in East Africa
Deborah Wockelmann
(Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz)
Socio-political processes of order have for a long time been fostering the subdivision of people
according to their language practices. Since the 19h century, linguists endeavoured the
establishment of a neat order of languages into clearly separated systems and speaker
communities that lasted not rarely until today but did little justice to a multifaceted linguistic
reality, especially in multilingual societies.
In their work on semiotic processes that interfere with language ideologies, Irvine &
Gal (2000) revealed that former processes of linguistic differentiation had to be renegotiated in
the fields of linguistics. Examples of artificial language mapping can be found across the entire
African continent since linguists, ethnographers and missionaries under colonial rule were
encouraged to provide clarity on the hundreds of various languages and dialects that were
undocumented and seemed messy from the Eurocentric view at that time.
As part of these endeavours, two closely related Bantu languages spoken in East Africa,
Kinyarwanda and Kirundi, have been declared two autonomous language systems under
German/ Belgian authorities (Nassenstein 2019; 2020). While Kinyarwanda was established as
the national language in Rwanda, Kirundi served as the equivalent in neighboring Burundi,
even though both go back to the same linguistic roots and are very similar. Ever since, the two
languages have become markers of national-political and ethnic differentiation as can be
observed in Uganda's capital Kampala, where many Rwandans and Burundians settled since
the 1960s or stop by for trade or transit.
I my talk I address the following questions: How can languages be investigated that
have undergone questionable categorization in the colonial era? How does the linguistic reality
fit or contradict ideological differentiation processes? How do speakers assess language
practices as indicator for belonging? Finally, I attempt to approach the advantages of narratives
as qualitative research method in sociolinguistics following initial works of De Fina (2003) and
De Fina & Georgakopoulou (2011).
References:
De Fina, A. (2003). Identity in Narrative: A Study of Immigrant Discourse. Amsterdam &
Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
De Fina, A., & Georgakopoulou, A. (2011). Analyzing Narrative: Discourse and
Sociolinguistic Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Irvine, J. T., & Gal, S. (2000). Language ideology and linguistic differentiation. In P. V.
Kroskrity, (Ed.), Regimes of language: Ideologies, polities, and identities. Santa Fe:
School of American Research Press, 35-84.
Nassenstein, N. (2019). Kinyarwanda and Kirundi: On colonial divisions, discourses of national
belonging, and language boundaries. Modern Africa: Politics, History and Society 7(1),
11-40.
Nassenstein, N. (2020). Pragmatics in Bakuli: A Linguistic Ethnographer’s Notes from the
Neighborhood. Journal of Postcolonial Linguistics 2(2020), 108–127.
102
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
From monolingual ideology to multilingual advocacy: An
autoethnography of a multilingual scholar in transnational settings
Yaqiong Xu
(Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim)
With increasing numbers of multilingual learners in classrooms around the globe, there is a
burgeoning interest in centralizing the role of language teacher identity (LTI) to enhance
teachers’ preparedness and competence in multilingual settings. This paper narrates and
analyzes the influential episodes in the author’s life trajectory regarding multiple language
learning and teaching experiences shaped by the broader sociocultural contexts in three
societies, namely mainland China, Hong Kong, and Norway. Drawing on Woolard and
Schieffelin’s (1988) concept of language ideologies, an autoethnography is employed (Ellis,
Adams & Bochner, 2011) as an analytical tool to illuminate the writer’s experiences of different
ideologies and negotiations of identities as a multilingual learner, teacher, and scholar in
transnational settings. Taking self-reflection and writing as a process to research, the author
came to an understanding that the ideological transformation from a monolingual orientation to
a multilingual advocacy is largely impacted by the ideologies of the social, cultural and political
structures at large. In addition, the author’s reflections on how her language teaching practices
were shaped by her linguistic ideologies demonstrated the profound influence of language
ideologies on her pedagogical choices, including both a monolingual ideology, which tends to
result in “separate multilingualism” (Creese & Blackledge, 2011), and a multilingual ideology,
which contributes to multilingual pedagogies such as translanguaging (García & Wei, 2013).
By deconstructing the author’s enactment with language ideologies and identity negotiations,
six stages were generated along the writer’s life journey: 1) Upbring in monolingual ideologies,
2) Becoming an EFL teacher, 3) Wresting with the ideology of “non-nativeness”, 4) Stepping
into a multilingual society, 5) Confronting identity crisis, and 6) Rebirth with “in-between
identities.” Addressed to teachers, teacher educators, and scholars in the era of global migration
and multilingualism, this paper echoes the call for an explicit role of LTI in teacher education
and to provide support of employing autoethnography as a powerful method to address issues
like identity and diversity (Canagarajah, 2012; Yazan, 2019).
References:
Canagarajah, A. S. (2012). Teacher development in a global profession: An
autoethnography. TESOL Quarterly, 46(2), 258–279.
Creese, A., & Blackledge, A. (2011). Separate and flexible bilingualism in complementary
schools: Multiple language practices in interrelationship. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(5),
1196–1208.
Ellis, C., Adams, T., & Bochner, A. (2011). Autoethnography: An overview. Historical Social
Research, 36(4), 273–290.
García, O., & Wei, L. (2013). Translanguaging. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK.
Woolard, K., & Schieffelin, B. (1994). Language ideology. Annual Review of Anthropology,
23(1), 55–82.
Yazan, B. (2019). Identities and ideologies in a language teacher candidate’s autoethnography:
Making meaning of storied experience. TESOL Journal, 10(4), 1–21.
103
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
The acquisition of Chinese functional words by Russian speakers:
The case of the Chinese conjunction hé
Xin Yan, Lemei Peng and Shanshan Yan
(Peking University)
From the perspective of features (Chomsky, 1995; Lardiere, 2008, 2009; Yuan, 2014), this
paper conducts an empirical study to examine Russian speakers’ acquisition of the Chinese
conjunction hé. A sentence-making task, a translation task, and an acceptability judgment task
are used as instruments.
Differences exist between the features of the Chinese conjunction hé and the Russian
conjunction и, while the results show that Russian-speaking learners directly map the
extraneous features of the Russian conjunction и to their second language lexical item, which
causes difficulties in their second language acquisition. Specifically, experimental data shows
that the vitality of an extraneous feature decreases until learners’ language proficiency reaches
an intermediate level. Eventually, it is difficult for Russian-speaking L2 learners to remove this
extraneous feature. It suggests that this feature becomes dormant when the second language
input lacks evidence to disconfirm it.
In addition, this paper provides suggestions for teaching the conjunction hé and other
Chinese functional words to Russian-speaking L2 learners.
References:
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.
Lardiere D (2008) Feature assembly in second language acquisition. In: Liceras JM, Zobl H,
and Goodluck H (eds) The role of formal features in second language acquisition.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 106–40.
Lardiere, D. (2009). Some thoughts on the contrastive analysis of features in second language
acquisition. Second Language Research, 25(2), 173-227.
Yuan, B. (2014). 'wh-on-earth' in Chinese speakers' L2 English: Evidence of dormant features.
Second Language Research, 30(4), 515-549.
We kindly invite you to fuck off: Linguistic (im)politeness and
Polish Women’s Strike
Martyna Zachorska and Anna Jelec
(Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań)
The October 2020 ruling of the Julia Przyłębska Constitutional Court proclaiming a near-total
abortion ban provoked a series of street protests attended by hundreds of thousands of
demonstrators. While such reaction might have been predicted given the opinion polls on the
matter, the ruling had the unintended consequence of an outburst of linguistic creativity among
the protesters.
Written on cardboards or shared as memes on social media, the slogans went beyond
the usual expressions of dissent. Expletives entered the public sphere on an unprecedented
scale, with the main message of the movement being wypierdalać (fuck off) and crowds
chanting Jebać PiS (fuck the governing party) accompanied by cheerful tunes. While some
criticised the expletive-laden language as primitive and unbecoming of public debate, many of
104
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
the banners were highly intertextual work that referenced popular and classical culture, media,
philosophy, cuisine, fairy tales and more. Furthermore, the calls for more polite forms of protest
were answered with banners that, in extremely courteous language, invited the government to
bugger off.
This paper analyses the impoliteness strategies (Culpeper 1996) and humour (Dynel
2008, 2017) employed by the protesters in their various forms of expression. We have gathered
a multimodal corpus of over 200 photos from the protests, including banners, cardboard signs
and murals expressing outrage over the ruling. We use the Face Threatening Act theory (Brown
and Levinson 1987) and an approach to multimodal analysis proposed by Forceville (1996) to
look at the text, image and context of each photo and create a Polish database of acts of civic
impoliteness.
References:
Brown, Penelope and Stephen C. Levinson. 1987. Politeness: Some universals in language
usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Culpeper, Jonathan. 1996. “Towards an anatomy of impoliteness”, Journal of Pragmatics 25:
349-367.
Dynel, Marta. 2008. “There Is Method in the Humorous Speaker’s Madness: Humour and
Grice’s Model”, Lodz Papers in Pragmatics 4: 159-185.
Dynel, Marta. 2017. “Introduction: On the linguistics of humour theoretically”, Lingua 197: 14.
Forceville, C. 1996. Pictorial metaphor in advertising. Psychology Press.
Grammatical and cognitive factors shaping the conceptualization of
motion events in language production
Katharina Zaychenko
(University of Kassel)
The way in which speakers of different languages encode motion events gives insight into
linguistic and conceptual representations of motion. Empirical studies have shown that event
descriptions differ dependent on grammatical factors absent or present in the target languages.
Several contributions to the field focus on differences in the domain of grammatical viewpoint
aspect, which is present in languages such as English, Russian, or Spanish as opposed to
German, Dutch, or Swedish (Stutterheim et al. 2012; Mertins 2018; Papafragou et al. 2008;
Athanasopoulos & Bylund 2012). The results illustrate that speakers of aspect languages rather
focus on the process of an event, whereas speakers of non-aspect languages tend to prioritize
the endpoint (Stutterheim et al. 2012; Mertins 2018).
While Bylund, Athanasopoulos & Oostendorp (2013) or Schmiedtová, Stutterheim &
Carroll (2011) understand the differences in focus on either the endpoint or the process of an
event as a result of the grammatical factors given in different languages, attempts have been
made to incorporate this binary grammatical factor into a more complex set of factors
determining motion event conceptualization. This is consistent with a growing number of
studies which assume that the variation of salience of motion event components such as paths
or endpoints might influence motion event encoding, too (Stutterheim et al. 2012, Bepperling
& Härtl 2013; Georgakopoulos & Härtl 2019).
The study at hand focuses on the extent to which grammatical aspect and visual salience
affect motion event verbalization and memorization. Therefore, speakers of English and
105
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
German as well as German learners of English participated in a verbalization task and a
memorization task. The results reveal a main effect for ‘endpoint salience’ in English such that
an increase in verbalized salient endpoints could be observed. Moreover, native speakers of
German show a tendency to verbalize more regular endpoints than English speakers. Data taken
from the memorization task point at a tendency for speakers of both languages to remember the
endpoint better if it was verbalized before. To conclude, the results gathered from this study
indicate an interdependency between grammatical and conceptual factors as an influence on the
conceptualization of motion events.
References:
Athanasopoulos, P., & Bylund, E. (2013). Does grammatical aspect affect motion event
cognition? A cross‐linguistic comparison of English and Swedish speakers. Cognitive
science, 37(2), 286-309.
Bepperling, S., & Härtl, H. (2013). Ereigniskonzeptualisierung im Zweitspracherwerb–
Thinking for Speaking im Vergleich von Muttersprachlern und Lernern. Zeitschrift für
Semiotik, 35(1-2), 159-191.
Bylund, E., Athanasopoulos, P., & Oostendorp, M. (2013). Motion event cognition and
grammatical aspect: Evidence from Afrikaans. Linguistics, 51(5), 929-955.
Georgakopoulos, T., Härtl, H., & Sioupi, A. (2019). Goal realization: An empirically based
comparison between English, German and Greek. Languages in Contrast, 19(2), 280309.
Mertins, B. (2018). Sprache und Kognition: Ereigniskonzeptualisierung im Deutschen und
Tschechischen. Berlin: De Gruyter.
Papafragou, A., Hulbert, J., & Trueswell, J. (2008). Does language guide event perception?
Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 108(1), 155-184.
Schmiedtová, B., Stutterheim, C. v., & Carroll, M. (2011). Language-specific patterns in event
construal of advanced second language speakers. In: A. Pavlenko (ed.), Thinking and
speaking in two languages, 66-107. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Stutterheim, C. v., Andermann, M., Carroll, M., Flecken, M., & Schmiedtová, B. (2012). How
grammaticized concepts shape event conceptualization in language production: Insights
from linguistic analysis, eye tracking data, and memory performance. Linguistics, 50(4),
833-867.
Research design: Profiling the Chinese modal verbs indicating
possibilities and abilities
Zhuo Zhang and Meichun Liu
(City University of Hong Kong)
This study aims to study the semantic differences and collocational variations of six Chinese
Modal Verbs, including néng (能), nénggòu (能够), huì(会), kě(可), kěnéng(可能), kěyǐ(可以).
These six lexical items can express possibilities and abilities, partially overlapping in
meaning and senses, and are substitutable or legal to replace each other in a specific context
grammatically. The meaning potential of a lexicon is elucidated by the full context in which it
occurs (Hanks, 2013), and such language knowledge is based on actual usage (Pooley, 2014).
The occurrence of such modal lexicons in a natural context exerts observable tendencies
distinctive to the others. Therefore, we propose to utilize the modified Behavioral Profile (BP)
106
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
approach via multidimensional scaling and cluster analysis (Liesenfeld et al., 2020). We have
compiled a corpus with over 5 million characters, in which the sentences with the modal words
will be extracted and annotated for our analysis, created by well-known Chinese authors. The
key features incorporate the language cues(usually referred to as ID tags in BP study) such as
syntactic tree height and node depth, negation, adjunct part-of-speech, and frames of closest
verbs, and Named-entity Recognition (NER) features and collocation with subjects, light verbs,
and construction makers. Most of our data will be annotated automatically, enabling us to
compile a larger dataset and incorporate more uses of modal verbs. Finally, to validate the result
and offer a potential application of our study, we will also train a machine learning model to
predict such modal verbs in the form of filling the blanks. Methodologically, this study will
illustrate how to apply computational and linguistic features into BP studies in a cost-effective
annotated method and draw implications on future studies of modal verbs. The verifiable result
will provide a corpus-based explication of the varieties concerning these modal verbs regarding
theory study or language acquisition, and the prediction model proves its potential usage in
improving the readability of generative texts.
References:
Hanks, P. (2013). Contextual Dependency and Lexical Sets. In Lexical Analysis (pp. Lexical
Analysis, 2013-01-25). The MIT Press.
Pooley, T., ProQuest Information Learning Co, & University of Pennsylvania. Music. (2014).
Melody as prosody : Toward a usage-based theory of music.
Liesenfeld, A., Liu, M., & Huang, C-R. (2020). Profiling the Chinese causative construction
with rang (讓), shi (使) and ling (令) using frame semantic features. Corpus Linguistics
and Linguistic Theory.
107
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
LIST OF ACTIVE PARTICIPANTS
NAME
SURNAME
E-MAIL ADDRESS
AFFILIATION
Agata
Ambroziak
aambroziak(at)uw.edu.pl
University of Warsaw
Katrin
Andermatt
andt(at)zhaw.ch
ZHAW Zurich University of Applied Sciences
Karolina
Baranowska
karbar(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Alice
Bertin
alice.bert98(at)gmail.com
University of Milan
Aneta
Bučková
aneta.buckova(at)ur.de
Universität Regensburg
Natacha
Buntinx
natacha.buntinx(at)uclouvain.be
Université Catholique de Louvain
Zuzanna
Cal
zuzanna.cal(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Raees
Calafato
raees.calafato(at)uib.no
University of Bergen
Anaïs
Carrere
anais.carrere(at)orange.fr
Université Bordeaux Montaigne
Xinqiao
Cen
xinqiao.cen(at)nottingham.edu.cn
University of Nottingham, Ningbo, China
Carolin
Centner
carolin.centner(at)ur.de
Universität Regensburg
Letizia
Cerqueglini
lolaluna2017(at)gmail.com
Tel Aviv University
Jean Pierre
Chevrot
jean-pierre.chevrot(at)univ-grenoble-alpes.fr Université Grenoble Alpes
Spandan
Chowdhury
schowdhury.sll(at)jadavpuruniversity.in
Jadavpur University
Jennifer
Cowell
jmc41(at)st-andrews.ac.uk
University of St Andrews & University of Stirling
Lilla Zsófia
Cseh
l.zsofia.cseh(at)gmail.com
Eötvös Loránd University
Valentina
Del Vecchio
valentina.delvecchio(at)unich.it
"G. d'Annunzio" University of Chieti-Pescara
Jakub
Dunin-Borkowski
j.dunin-borkowski(at)uw.edu.pl
University of Warsaw
Tomasz
Dyrmo
tomdyr1(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Selim
Earls
selim.earls(at)ec.europa.eu
University of Louvain
Elizabeth
Eldho
elizabeth.eldho73(at)gmail.com
Indian Institute of Technology
Kathrin
Feindt
kathrin.feindt(at)uni-hamburg.de
University of Hamburg
Katerina
Filipova
katekF(at)seznam.cz
Charles University Prague
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Helen
Forsyth
heforsyth(at)unibz.it
Free University of Bozen-Bolzano
Maia
Gabadadze
maia.gabadadze(at)yahoo.com
Akaki Tsereteli State University
Ádám
Galac
adam.galac(at)gmail.com
Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Zahra
Ghane
zari_gh2004(at)yahoo.com
Heinrich Heine University
Serena
Ghiselli
serena.ghiselli3(at)unibo.it
University of Bologna
Anne Catherine
Gieshoff
annecatherine.gieshoff(at)zhaw.ch
ZHAW - Zurich University of Applied Sciences
Sergei
Gnitiev
sergei.gnitiev(at)outlook.com
University of Pannonia
Luisa
Grabiger
grabigerl(at)uni-greifswald.de
University of Greifswald
Ekaterina
Gridneva
toki91(at)mail.ru
National Research University Higher School of Economics
Damian
Herda
damian.t.herda(at)gmail.com
Jagiellonian University
Anna
Jelec
jelec(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Magdalena
Jelińska
m.jelinska(at)uw.edu.pl
University of Warsaw
Roniya
Jose
jose.roniya(at)gmail.com
Central University of Punjab
Kornélia
Juhász
juhasz.kornelia8(at)gmail.com
Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest
Magdalena
Jurczenia
magda.jurczenia(at)gmail.com
University of Białystok
Emilia
Jurewicz
eb.jurewicz(at)student.uw.edu.pl
University of Warsaw
Yesim
Kakalic
y.kakalic(at)warwick.ac.uk
University of Warwick
Krystian
Kamiński
krys.kami(at)gmail.com
Maria Curie-Skłodowska University
Krisztina
Karácsonyi
krisztinakaracsonyi.92(at)gmail.com
University of Pécs
Michal
Korenar
m.korenar(at)reading.ac.uk
University of Reading
Paweł
Korpal
pkorpal(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Justyna
Kotowicz
justyna.kotowicz(at)up.krakow.pl
Pedagogical University of Cracow
Patrycja
Kubicha
patrycja.kubicha(at)gmail.com
Jan Kochanowski University
Grzegorz
Lara
g.lara(at)uw.edu.pl
University of Warsaw
Laszlo
Laszloczki
laszloczki.laszlo(at)gmail.com
Eötvös Loránd University
109
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Eliane
Lorenz
eliane.lorenz(at)ntnu.no
Norwegian University of Science and Technology
Rea
Lujić
rlujic(at)unizd.hr
University of Zadar
Tomasz
Łuszczek
t.luszczek(at)uw.edu.pl
University of Warsaw
Aidan
Malanoski
ajmalanoski(at)gmail.com
City University of New York
Gisela
Mayr
gmayr(at)unibz.it
Free University of Bolzano
Dimitrios
Meletis
dimitrios.meletis(at)outlook.com
University of Zurich
Enakshi
Nandi
enakshi.nandi(at)gmail.com
Jawaharlal Nehru University
Marcin
Naranowicz
marcin.naranowicz(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Nadine
Nasef
nadinenasef96(at)gmail.com
Misr International University
Jagoda
Nosal
jagnos(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Katharina
Oster
osterk(at)uni-mainz.de
University of Mainz
Jeni
Peake
jeni.peake(at)u-bordeaux.fr
University Bordeaux Montaigne
Marie
Peuzin
marie.peuzin(at)univ-grenoble-alpes.fr
Université Grenoble Alpes
Martina
Podboj
martina.podboj(at)uniri.hr
University of Rijeka
Irena
Prawdzic
irena.prawdzic(at)ispan.waw.pl
Institute of Slavic Studies Polish Academy of Sciences
Natika
Puthran
natikaputhran(at)gmail.com
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Hamza
R'boul
hamzarboul4(at)gmail.com
Public University of Navarre
Mateusz
Rogalski
rogalski.mateusz92(at)gmail.com
University of Rzeszów
Mátyás
Rosenberg
matyas.rosenberg(at)gmail.com
Research Institute for Linguistics
Martina
Rybova
martina.rybova(at)ur.de
Universität Regensburg
Tomas
Senda
tomas.senda(at)student.uw.edu.pl
University of Warsaw
Polina
Sikatskaya
p.sikatskaia(at)g.nsu.ru
Novosibirsk State University
Breno
Silva
b.barreto-sil2(at)uw.edu.pl
University of Warsaw
Anna
Skałba
anna.skalba(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Anita
Skóra
anaskora8(at)gmail.com
Jan Kochanowski University
110
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
Éva
Szabó
szalayeva0823(at)gmail.com
University of Szeged
Katarzyna
Szyszka
kz.szyszka(at)student.uw.edu.pl
University of Warsaw
Anke
Tardel
antardel(at)uni-mainz.de
Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz
Máté
Tóth
toth.mate(at)arts.unideb.hu
University of Debrecen
Mehrdad
Vasheghani Farahani
mehrdadfarahani1365(at)gmail.com
Leipzig University
Janna
Verthein
janverth(at)uni-mainz.de
Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz
Alena
Vodneva
alena_neva(at)live.ru
Saint Petersburg State University
Dianna
Walla
dianna.walla(at)ntnu.no
Norwegian University of Science and Technology
Deborah
Wockelmann
wockelmann(at)uni-mainz.de
Johannes Gutenberg-University Mainz
Yaqiong
Xu
yaqiong.xu(at)ntnu.no
Norwegian University of Science and Technology
Xin
Yan
yanxin4608(at)pku.edu.cn
Peking University
Martyna
Zachorska
marzac(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Katharina
Zaychenko
katharina_zaychenko(at)web.de
University of Kassel
Zhuo
Zhang
jessy.zh(at)my.cityu.edu.hk
City University of Hong Kong
ORGANIZING COMMITTEE
Joanna
Pawelczyk
pasia(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Katarzyna
Jankowiak
katarzyna.jankowiak(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Patryk
Dobkiewicz
patryk.dobkiewicz(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Tomasz
Dyrmo
tomasz.dyrmo(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
PLENARY SPEAKERS
Robert
Hartsuiker
robert.hartsuiker(at)ugent.be
Ghent University
Katarzyna
Jankowiak
katarzyna.jankowiak(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Julien
Perrez
julien.perrez(at)uliege.be
University of Liège
Helen
Sauntson
h.sauntson(at)yorksj.ac.uk
York St John University
111
YLMP 2021 – www.ylmp2021.amu.edu.pl
WORKSHOP LEADERS
Robert
Lew
rlew(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Julien
Perrez
julien.perrez(at)uliege.be
University of Liège
Helen
Sauntson
h.sauntson(at)yorksj.ac.uk
York St John University
Cassandra
Smith-Christmas
cassandra.smith-christmas(at)nuigalway.ie National University of Ireland, Galway
THEMATIC SESSION GUESTS AND CONVENERS
Raphael
Berthele
raphael.berthele(at)unifr.ch
University of Fribourg
Mikołaj
Buczak
mikolaj.buczak(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Tomasz
Dyrmo
tomasz.dyrmo(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Katarzyna
Dziubalska-Kołaczyk
dkasia(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Patrycja
Kakuba
patkak(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Agnieszka
Kiełkiewicz-Janowiak
akiel(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Paweł
Korpal
pkorpal(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Olha
Lehka-Paul
olhleh(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Jagoda
Nosal
jagoda.nosal(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
Katharina
Oster
osterk(at)uni-mainz.de
Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz
Magdalena
Wrembel
magdala(at)amu.edu.pl
Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań
SPECIAL EVENT
Maria
Coady
mcoady(at)coe.ufl.edu
University of Florida
112