Psychological Mindedness and Awareness
of Self and Others
䊲
Mark Beitel
Marymount Manhattan College and Fordham University
䊲
Elena Ferrer
Marymount Manhattan College
䊲
John J. Cecero
Fordham University
The major goal of this study was to explore the relationship among psychological mindedness (PM) and several facets of awareness, including
a general sense of mindfulness (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale;
Brown & Ryan, 2003), as well as more specific awareness of self (selfconsciousness scale; Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975) and others
(Interpersonal Reactivity Index; Davis, 1980). Participants were 103 undergraduate students at an urban liberal arts college. Results indicated that
PM (PM Scale; Conte, Plutchik, Jung, Picard, Karasu, & Lotterman, 1990)
is related to mindfulness (r ⫽ .41, p ⬍ .01), private self-consciousness
(r ⫽ .27, p ⬍ .05), as well as cognitive (r ⫽ .30, p ⬍ .01) and affective
(r ⫽ .35, p ⬍ .01) indices of empathy. Self-consciousness and empathy
explained a significant amount of variance in PM in a simultaneous-entry
multiple regression. These findings support theoretical claims that PM
involves awareness of self and others. © 2004 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
J Clin Psychol 61: 739–750, 2005.
Keywords: awareness; empathy; mindfulness; psychological mindedness’
self-consciousness
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Mark Beitel, who is now at the Department of
Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, Welch Center, 2nd Floor, 495 Congress Avenue, New Haven,
CT 06519; e-mail:
[email protected].
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY, Vol. 61(6), 739–750 (2005)
© 2004 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/jclp.20095
740
Journal of Clinical Psychology, June 2005
Psychological mindedness (PM), in its broadest sense, involves awareness and understanding of psychological processes, such as thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. Some
definitions of PM focus on self-awareness. For example, Applebaum (1973) wrote that
PM is “A person’s ability to see relationships among thoughts, feelings, and actions, with
the goal of learning the meanings and causes of his experiences and behavior” (p. 36).
Some definitions focus on awareness of others: Gough (1957/1975) defined the psychologically minded person as one who is “interested in, and responsive to, the inner needs,
motives, and experiences of others” (p. 11).
Farber (1985) offered a comprehensive definition of PM that involves awareness of
psychological processes in self and others: “. . . the disposition to reflect upon the meaning and motivation of behavior, thoughts, and feelings of oneself and others” (p. 170).
Following this definition, Conte et al. (1990) designed the PM Scale, a 45-item selfreport measure of PM toward self and others. Factor-analytic work on the PM Scale
(Conte, Ratto, & Karasu, 1996; Shill & Lumley, 2002) has provided an empirical definition of PM: It involves “. . . a degree of access to one’s feelings that leads, through
discussion of one’s problems with others, to an ability to acquire insight into the meaning
and motivation of one’s own and others’ thoughts, feelings, and behavior, and to a capacity for change (Conte & Ratto, 1997, p. 21).
This definition includes awareness of psychological processes in self and others. In
other words, the psychologically minded person perceives the thoughts and feelings that
constitute the psychological world: The high-PM person understands that psychological
constructs have substance and that they matter in a fundamental way. Psychologically
minded thinking about oneself involves awareness of one’s own thoughts and feelings.
Therefore, the alexithymic patient—who lacks emotional awareness—would not be able
to think in a psychologically minded way about him- or herself. Others’ thoughts and
feelings may also become the focus of psychologically minded thinking. As such, the
autistic patient—who exhibits mindblindness, or lack of awareness of the psychological
life of others—would not be able to think in a psychologically minded way about others.
Beyond awareness, PM implies an understanding of psychological processes. Once
the high-PM individual has perceived psychological data, i.e., a sequence of thoughts or
feelings, then he or she is in a position to make sense of that data by noting patterns and
relationships among thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. The relationship between PM and
psychological understanding is beyond the scope of this paper and will be taken up in
subsequent work.
The focus of this paper is the relationship between PM and awareness. Very little
empirical research has been conducted upon the awareness aspect of PM, despite its
central importance to the construct. To begin to fill this gap, we examined the relationships among PM and general awareness (mindfulness), the trait of self-awareness (selfconsciousness), and awareness of others (empathy).
PM and Mindfulness
Both PM and mindfulness involve awareness of emotions. Brown and Ryan (2003) defined
mindfulness as “. . . an enhanced attention to and awareness of current experience or
present reality.” Current experience includes emotional states of which a person might be
more or less aware. In fact, mindfulness correlated with a measure of emotional awareness (Trait Meta-Mood Scale, TMMS; Salovey, Mayer, Goldman, & Palfai, 1995) at .49,
p ⬍ .001 in their undergraduate sample (N ⫽ 313). Therefore, PM and mindfulness
should be directly related to one another.
Psychological Mindedness
741
Horowitz (2002) explained that PM and mindfulness were similar constructs, related
by self-observation. Both PM and mindfulness require that an individual observe what he
or she experiences. Brown and Ryan (2003) reported modest correlations between selfconsciousness (as measured by the self-consciousness scale (SCS); Fenigstein, Scheier,
& Buss, 1975) and mindfulness in their sample of 313 undergraduates: private selfconsciousness (r ⫽ .03, p ⫽ ns), self-awareness (r ⫽ .23, p ⬍ .0001), self-reflectiveness
(r ⫽ ⫺.13, p ⬍ .05), and public self-consciousness (r ⫽ ⫺.14, p ⬍ .05).
Empirically, PM and mindfulness relate to Big Five personality traits in similar ways,
suggesting that they might be positively associated with one another. Brown and Ryan
(2003) reported correlations for mindfulness with neuroticism (r ⫽ ⫺.33, p ⬍ .0001)
and openness (r ⫽ .18, p ⬍ .05) on the NEO-FFI (Costa & McCrae, 1992) which were
in line with Beitel and Cecero’s (2003) findings for PM: neuroticism (r ⫽ ⫺.33, p ⬍ .01)
and openness (r ⫽ .40, p ⬍ .01).
No study to date has investigated the relationship between PM and mindfulness. We
expected that the two constructs would be positively related to one another because both
involve self-awareness. This correlation was predicted to be moderate because mindfulness is a more general construct: It is also more present-focused. PM involves more of a
synthesizing, pattern-spotting attentional capacity. The high-PM patient may do well in
therapy because he or she can focus attention on past maladaptive patterns, on the hereand-now of therapy, and on the future. Therefore, mindfulness is seen as a necessary but
not sufficient condition for PM.
PM and Self-Awareness
Self-awareness is a necessary precondition for PM. Fenigstein, Scheier, and Buss (1975)
defined self-awareness as a state in which attention is directed toward the self. They
described self-consciousness as the chronic disposition to direct attention toward the
self. Self-consciousness and PM have been seen as overlapping constructs by some researchers. In fact, Farber (1989) had used the PR-SCS (described above) as a measure
of PM until the PM scale became available. Fenigstein (1997) suggested that the constructs are similar in important ways. For example, both involve “heightened awareness
of ongoing mental processes” (p. 117).
Fenigstein (1997) noted a conceptual difference between self-consciousness and PM:
self-consciousness involves awareness of mental processes, while PM involves awareness plus an interest in explaining the objects of awareness in psychological terms. Horowitz (2002) observed that the kind of self-observation involved in PM and mindfulness “is
different from stressful self-monitoring or anxious self-preoccupation,” referring to traits
measured by the SCS (p. 115). The SCS appears to have a negative valance, tapping
“concerns.” Empirically, it has correlated positively with measures of depression and
anxiety (Fenigstein, 1997). Trapnell and Campbell (1999) demonstrated that the SCS
measures healthy self-reflection as well as pathological self-rumination, using their Reflections and Ruminations Questionnaire (RRQ).
PM and self-consciousness have been demonstrated to be positive empirical associations. Trudeau and Reich (1995) found that the PM Scale and the PR-SCS were directly
related to one another (r ⫽ .45, p ⬍ .01) in a sample of 89 undergraduates. This relationship was predicted given that PR-SCS measures awareness of internal states. The other
SCS scales PU-SCS and SA were not administered in that sample. Even higher correlations might be expected among PM, public self-consciousness, and social anxiety, because
the latter measure attention toward self plus some consideration of how one is perceived
742
Journal of Clinical Psychology, June 2005
by others. This additional consideration is more akin to PM, which deals with one’s
relationship to others.
Empirical findings also suggest differences between PM and self-consciousness as
well. For example, they exhibited different relationships to well being (PWB Scale; Ryff,
1989) in the study by Trudeau and Reich (1995). Both were significantly predictive of
PWB in a simultaneous-entry multiple-regression context. However, PM was positively
related to PWB (  ⫽ .42, t ⫽ 3.66, p ⫽ .01) and PR-SCS related negatively (  ⫽ ⫺.23,
t ⫽ ⫺2.00, p ⫽ .05). This finding suggests that PM and well-being increase together as
PR-SCS decreases.
PM and Empathy
Davis (1983) wrote that: “. . . empathy in the broadest sense refers to the reactions of one
individual to the observed experiences of another” (p. 113). He defined cognitive and
affective domains of empathy: perspective-taking (PT) is the tendency for a person to
take the “psychological point of view of others.” Empathic concern (EC) taps feelings of
sympathy and concern for unfortunate others. Schlesinger (1994, p. 33) defined empathy
as the midpoint on a continuum between alienation and identification with another person. Both definitions require that one must pay attention to others in order to demonstrate
empathy.
Attending to another person empathically provides an experiential sense of that person. Certainly, one learns something about what the other person thinks and feels. Beyond
content, one may observe cognitive and affective processes in action. Thus, empathy
provides access to the data of interest to a psychologically minded person, i.e., the thoughts
and feelings of others. Once another’s psychological processes are observed, then the
high-PM person is in a position to hypothesize about the origins, maintenance strategies,
and functions of those processes, i.e., to think in a psychologically minded way about
them. Without empathically derived information, one’s psychological formulations are
educated guesses at best.
Since PM is a cognitively toned personality variable, with positive relations to ambiguity tolerance (Beitel, Ferrer, & Cecero, 2004), it should correlate positively with cognitive measures of empathy, such as PT. PM also involves access to feelings: The PM
Scale has been related inversely to measures of emotional impairment in several studies
(Bagby, Taylor, & Parker, 1994; Beitel & Cecero, 2003). Therefore, it should be positively related to more affective measures of empathy, such as EC. Hatcher and Hatcher
(1997) argued that empathy is an important constituent of PM. They suggested that PM
incorporates self-awareness and empathy, but is greater than the sum of these parts.
Therefore, a large percentage of PM’s variance should be explained by self-awareness
and empathy. The present study renders the assertion in testable form.
Goals and Hypotheses
The major goal of this study was to explore the relationship among PM and several facets
of awareness, including a general sense of mindfulness as well as more specific awareness of self (self-consciousness) and others (empathy). This effort is also an attempt to
guard against construct proliferation, by ensuring that PM is not redundant with preexisting constructs. An overarching goal of our research program is to extend the construct validity of the PM Scale.
This study provides an opportunity to replicate and extend PM research findings. We
attempted to replicate the positive relationship between PM and PR-SCS reported by
Psychological Mindedness
743
Trudeau and Reich’s (1995), while extending the effort to include PU-SCS and social
anxiety. This study is the first to explore the relationship between PM and mindfulness. It
is also the first to examine PM in relation to cognitive and affective components of
empathy. The relationship between empathy and mindfulness is also presented. These
replications and extensions occur within the context of a single study, which allows for
the simultaneous examination of several important relationships.
Method
Participants
The 103 undergraduate participants were recruited from an urban, liberal arts college in
New York. There were 79 women and 24 men in this sample, ranging in education level
from freshman to senior: 6% were freshman, 7% were sophomores, 28% were juniors,
and 52% were seniors. The mean age of the sample was 27 years (SD ⫽ 8.80). This
sample was ethnically diverse: 44% Caucasian, 19% Hispanic, 20% African-American,
13% Asian, and 4% described themselves as “other.” Volunteers received a keepsake with
college logo in exchange for their participation in the study. Informed consent was obtained
and participants were debriefed verbally and in writing at the conclusion of their participation. The instruments were presented in counterbalanced order.
To determine the number of participants required detecting a medium-size effect in a
multiple regression analysis with seven predictors, an a priori power analysis was computed. The power analysis was conducted with GPOWER (Erdfelder, Faul, & Buchner,
1996). It was determined that 103 participants would be required to detect a medium
effect, with power set to .80 and alpha set to .05. The observed effect size from the
multiple regression analysis presented in Table 3 was .23, which indicates a medium
effect (Cohen, 1992, p. 343).
Measures
Psychological Mindedness Scale (PM Scale). The Psychological Mindedness Scale
(Conte et al., 1990) is a 45-item self-report measure. The items are presented on a fourpoint scale and range from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree.” Examples of test
items include: “I am always curious about the reasons people behave as they do,” “I like
to try new things, even if it involves taking risks,” and “Often I don’t know what I’m
feeling.” Temporal stability was assessed over a two-week period in a sample of 22
normal adults (r ⫽ .92). Internal consistency (␣ ⫽ 0.87) was good in a sample of 256
psychiatric outpatients.
Beitel and Cecero (2003) found that the PM Scale was negatively correlated
(r (185) ⫽ ⫺.33, p ⬍ .01) with the neuroticism subscale of the NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992). The PM Scale related negatively (r (83) ⫽
⫺0.68, p ⬍ .01) to the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Bagby, Taylor, & Parker,
1994). PM was positively associated with extraversion (r (185) ⫽ .37, p ⬍ .01) and
openness to experience (r (185) ⫽ .40, p ⬍ .01) in Beitel and Cecero’s (2003) study of
university students. PM and self-reported psychological well being (PWB Scale; Ryff,
1989) were positively correlated (r (87) ⫽ .31, p ⬍ .01) in a college sample (Trudeau &
Reich, 1995).
Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS). The MAAS (Brown & Ryan, 2003)
is a 15-item, self-report measure of mindfulness. The items are presented on a six-point
744
Journal of Clinical Psychology, June 2005
Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost always) to 6 (almost never). High scores indicate
more mindfulness. To reduce social desirability, respondents are asked to rate the items in
terms of what “really reflects” their experience rather than what they think their experience ought to be. Temporal stability has been assessed with a sample of 60 undergraduates over a four-week period (ICC ⫽ .81, p ⬍ .0001). Coefficient alpha has ranged
from .82 in an undergraduate sample (N ⫽ 327) to .87 in a general adult sample
(N ⫽ 239).
Evidence for the MAAS’s convergent and discriminant validity is presented in Brown
and Ryan (2003). The MAAS has correlated positively with a variety of self-report instruments that tap self-awareness. For example, the Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS; Salovey
et al. 1995 measures attention to feelings, clarity of emotional experience, and repairing
unpleasant mood states. The MAAS correlated with overall emotional awareness on the
TMMS at .46 ( p ⬍ .001), attention (r ⫽ .19, p ⬍ .001), clarity (r ⫽ .49, p ⬍ .0001), and
repair (r ⫽ .37, p ⬍ .0001). The MAAS has been associated positively with a variety of
well-being measures, such as positive affect (r ⫽ .30, p ⬍ .0001) on the Positive and
Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). In contrast, it was
inversely related to negative affect (r ⫽ ⫺.39, p ⬍ .0001)
Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). The IRI is a 28-item, self-report measure of
empathy. Items appear on a five-point scale ranging from 0 (does not describe me well to
4 (describes me very well). Sample items include: “When I’m upset at someone, I usually
try to ‘put myself in his shoes’ for a while,” and “I am often quite touched by things that
I see happen.” The IRI has four seven-item subscales that tap cognitive and affective
dimensions of empathy: fantasy (FR), perspective-taking (PT), empathic concern (EC),
and personal distress (PD). Reported alpha coefficients have ranged from .71 to .77
(Davis, 1983). Test-retest reliabilities have reportedly ranged from .62 to .71 (Davis,
1983).
The IRI was compared to two pre-existing, self-report measures of empathy, one
more cognitive (Hogan Empathy Scale, 1969) and the other more affective (Mehrabian &
Epstein, 1972). PT was more highly correlated with the cognitive scale (rFemales ⫽ .37,
p ⬍ .05; rMales ⫽ .42, p ⬍ .05) in a sample of 460 undergraduates. As predicted, EC
was more strongly related to the affective measure (rFemales ⫽ .56, p ⬍ .05; rMales ⫽ .63,
p ⬍ .05).
Davis (1983) reported that the IRI related to several psychological variables in
theoretically predicted ways. In a sample of 400 undergraduate students, the highest
IRI subscale correlation was PD with social anxiety on the self-consciousness scale
(SA-SCS; Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975; rFemales ⫽ .39, p ⬍ .01; rMales ⫽ .43,
p ⬍ .01). Public self-consciousness, on the SCS, correlated with FS at .19 for women
and .25 for men. Intelligence, as measured by the WAIS (N ⫽ 114), was uncorrelated (as
predicted) with every IRI subscale with one exception: WAIS vocabulary was related to
FS for women (r ⫽ .28, p ⬍ .05).
Self-Consciousness Scale (SCS; Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975). The SCS is a
23-item instrument intended to measure the trait of self-awareness. The items are presented on a five-point scale ranging from 0 (extremely uncharacteristic) to 4 (extremely
characteristic). Sample items include: “I’m always trying to figure myself out,” “I’m
concerned about the way I present myself,” and “I get embarrassed very easily.” The SCS
has three subscales that measure private self-consciousness (PR-SCS), public selfconsciousness (PU-SCS), and social anxiety (SA-SCS). Coefficient alphas have been
reported as follows PR-SCS (␣ ⫽ .74), PU-SCS (␣ ⫽ .81), and SA-SCS (␣ ⫽ .80).
745
Psychological Mindedness
Test-retest reliabilities, in a sample of 84 undergraduates, were as follows: PR-SCS (.79),
PU-SCS (.84), and SA-SCS (.73).
The SCS has been an extremely popular instrument, as the subject of hundreds of
studies in psychology over the past two decades. Individuals who score high in PR-SCS
provide more detailed and more extensive self-descriptors (Franzoi, 1983). High PR-SCS
also report heightened affective experience relative to low PR-SCS people (Scheier, Carver,
& Gibson, 1979). Trudeau and Reich (1995) reported a significant positive correlation
between the PM scale and PR-SCS (r ⫽ .45, p ⬍ .01) in a sample of 89 undergraduates.
Those individuals who reported high PU-SCS scores tended to be more aware of their
personal appearance (Nasby, 1989) and tended to believe that they were objects of social
attention at higher rates than low PU-SCS individuals (Fenigstein & Vanable, 1992).
Results
Scale descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and
alphas were in line with findings from prior studies, and were acceptable for research
purposes. Table 2 displays correlations among PM and the other study variables. PM
and mindfulness (MAAS) were significantly, positively correlated as predicted (r ⫽ .41,
p ⬍ .01). Both cognitive (PT) and affective (EC) measures of empathy were positively
related to PM (rPT ⫽ .30 and rEC ⫽ .35, p ⬍ .01). PM was inversely related to personal
distress (PD, r ⫽ ⫺.36, p ⬍ .01) on the Interpersonal Reactivity Index.
PM was also related to measures of self-consciousness. As predicted, private selfconsciousness (PR-SCS) and PM were positively associated (r ⫽ .27, p ⬍ .05). Contrary to prediction, public self-consciousness (PU-SCS) was unrelated to PM (r ⫽ ⫺.04,
p ⫽ .67). The PU-SCS item to PM correlations were examined to learn more about this
unexpected finding. PM was significantly, inversely correlated with a cognitive-appraisal
item (r ⫽ ⫺.25, p ⬍ .05): Item 19 “I’m concerned about what other people think of me.”
PM was associated with two items representing awareness of one’s physical self as an
object of attention: The item focusing on self-conscious awareness was negatively associated (but not significantly) with PM (r ⫽ ⫺.12, p ⫽ .28): Item 11 “I’m self-conscious
about the way I look.” The item focusing on self-awareness was positively (but not
significantly associated with PM (r ⫽ .12, p ⫽ .28): Item 21 “I’m usually aware of my
Table 1
Scale Descriptive Statistics
Descriptives
Variable
Psychological Mindedness (PM)
Mindfulness (MAAS)
Perspective-Taking (IRI)
Empathic Concern (IRI)
Personal Distress (IRI)
Private Self-Consciousness (SCS)
Public Self-Consciousness (SCS)
Social Anxiety (SCS)
M
SD
␣
134.74
60.37
18.23
20.98
10.30
25.67
18.11
12.27
14.22
13.99
4.81
4.51
4.96
6.28
6.12
5.66
.86
.88
.70
.71
.67
.70
.81
.75
Note. PM ⫽ Psychological Mindedness Scale; MAAS ⫽ Mindful Attention Awareness Scale; IRI ⫽ Interpersonal Reactivity
Index; SCS ⫽ Self-Consciousness Scale.
746
Journal of Clinical Psychology, June 2005
Table 2
Correlations Among PM and Awareness Variables
Variable
PM
MAAS
PT
EC
PD
PR
PU
SA
PM
MAAS
PT-IRI
EC-IRI
PD-IRI
PR-SCS
PU-SCS
SA-SCS
—
.41**
.30**
.35**
⫺.36**
.27*
⫺.04
⫺.40**
—
.41**
.28*
⫺.49**
⫺.13
⫺.23*
⫺.31**
—
.42**
⫺.32**
.05
.20*
⫺.24*
—
⫺.01
.26*
.12
⫺.06
—
.15
.34**
.52**
—
.57**
.01
—
.35
—
Note. PM ⫽ Psychological Mindedness Scale; MAAS ⫽ Mindful Attention Awareness Scale; Interpersonal Reactivity Index
(IRI) Subscales: PT ⫽ Perspective Taking, EC ⫽ Empathic Concern, PD ⫽ Personal Distress; Self-Consciousness Scale (SCS)
Subscales: PR-SCS ⫽ Private Self-Consciousness, PU-SCS ⫽ Public Self-Consciousness, SA-SCS ⫽ Social Anxiety.
*p ⬍ .05. **p ⬍ .01.
appearance.” PM and social anxiety (SA-SCS) were inversely related to one another in
this study (r ⫽ ⫺.40, p ⬍ .01)
To test Hatcher and Hatchers’ (1997) assertion that PM is constituted by selfawareness and empathy, a simultaneous-entry multiple-regression equation was constructed by regressing PM upon PR-SCS and the IRI subscales of PT and EC (see Table 3).
Note that PU-SCS was left out of the equation, as it was uncorrelated with PM. The
predictors explained 19% of the variance in PM.
The relationships among PM and the distress/anxiety variables (SA-SCS and PD-IRI)
that are not core constituents of their respective constructs (self-consciousness and empathy), were analyzed in a separate, exploratory analysis (see Table 4). SA-SCS and PD-IRI
were not significantly related to one another (r ⫽ ⫺.11, p ⬎ .05). However, both Pearson
(see Table 2) and part (see Table 4) correlations revealed strong, opposing relationships
to PM for each variable.
The variables in this study appeared to be normally distributed. Visual inspection
of pairwise scatterplots suggested that the variables were also linearly related to one
another. Internal consistency coefficients were all above .70, with the exception of PD-IRI
(␣ ⫽ .67). Participant age was unrelated to all variables except PR-SCS, with which it
demonstrated an inverse relationship (r ⫽ ⫺.22, p ⬍ .05). Possible mean differences by
sex within each variable were assessed with independent-sample t tests (two-tailed). The
Bonferroni correction was employed to maintain a family-wise type I error rate of .05,
Table 3
Psychological Mindedness Regressed Upon Self-Consciousness and Empathy Variables
Variable

SE
t
p
rpart
PT-IRI
EC-IRI
PR-SCS
.215
.224
.211
.327
.354
.236
2.01
2.05
2.06
.048
.044
.042
.200
.203
.205
Note. Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) Subscales: PT ⫽ Perspective Taking, EC ⫽ Empathic Concern. PR-SCS ⫽ Private
Self-Consciousness. Standardized beta weights are shown. Overall equation, R 2 ⫽ .19, p ⬍ .001.
747
Psychological Mindedness
Table 4
Psychological Mindedness Regressed Upon Distress/Anxiety Variables
Variable

SE
t
p
rpart
PD-IRI
SA-SCS
⫺.189
⫺.324
.314
.281
⫺1.65
⫺2.82
.102
.006
⫺.159
⫺.272
Note. PD ⫽ Personal Distress; SA⫽ Social Anxiety. Standardized beta weights are shown. Overall equation, R 2 ⫽ .21, p ⬍ .001.
resulting in a more stringent p value of .007. Sex differences were not detected within any
of the variables (all p ⬎ .01).
Discussion
PM is “. . . the disposition to reflect upon the meaning and motivation of behavior, thoughts,
and feelings of oneself and others” (Farber, 1985, p. 170). In order to reflect on psychological data, one must become aware that these data exist in the first place. Therefore, the
capacity for psychological awareness is central to PM. As such, the major goal of this
study was to identify the facets of awareness most relevant to PM. Horowitz (2002)
theorized that PM and mindfulness were similar constructs. The Pearson correlation between
the PM scale and the MAAS support this hypothesis. Though related, they are two independent constructs. PM includes a general attentiveness plus an interest in psychological
conceptualization. Mindfulness, then, may be a necessary precondition for psychological
thinking, as Freud (1953) suggested with his concept of even-hovering attention.
Hatcher and Hatcher (1997) suggested that self-observation and empathy were two
integral elements of PM and findings from this study support this hypothesis. These
findings demonstrate empirically that PM is associated with awareness of cognitive and
affective processes, which are—in theory—of great interest to the highly psychologically minded individual. These findings suggest that people who report high PM also
tend to report higher awareness of self and others.
As predicted, PM and private self-consciousness were positively correlated, though
not as highly as reported in Trudeau and Reich (1995). One surprising finding was that
PM and public self-consciousness were unrelated. It was predicted that they would be
related for two reasons: both take the self as the object of attention and both consider the
self in relation to others. PM and social anxiety were negatively correlated in this study.
Perhaps one reason why high PM patients do well in group therapy (McCallum & Piper,
1997) is that they are low in social anxiety.
Highly psychologically-minded individuals are able to attend to cognitive (Beitel,
Ferrer, & Cecero, 2004) and affective (Beitel & Cecero, 2003) dimensions of their own
experience. This study measured how well they report attending to others in these ways.
PM was positively related to both cognitive and affective empathy. This finding supports
the Hall (1992) model, which suggests that PM requires affective and cognitive involvement.
PM was inversely related to personal distress, suggesting that high PM people are
able to remain calm and attentive in emergency situations. This finding is in line with
Beitel and Cecero’s (2003) report that PM is inversely related to neuroticism on the
Five-Factor Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Taken together, these findings indicate
that individuals who report high PM also report low distress in emergency situations and
in general.
748
Journal of Clinical Psychology, June 2005
Given the inverse relationship between PM and PD, it could be that PM serves as a
protective factor in stressful moments. There are several conceivable reasons for this,
including the possibility that PM is an effective coping mechanism, one that is capable of
buffering personal distress. In general, having an enriched psychological vantage point
provides more information, and perhaps more useful information, to deal with a stressful
situation. For example, the psychologically minded person might be able to appreciate
the psychological factors associated with a crisis and then craft an appropriate psychological response. PM allows one to observe patterns and dynamics, to compare the situation to past experience, and to predict outcomes—all of which might be brought to bear
during a crisis. PM also implies a reflective, analytic attitude that might keep a person
from acting impulsively or mindlessly in an emergency.
As suggested by previous work on PM and attachment (Alvarez, Farber, & Schonbar,
1998; Beitel & Cecero, 2003), PM may develop in securely attached, well-adjusted individuals. Therefore, PM might be accompanied by a wide range of mature defenses, which
could be invoked during times of personal distress. The high-PM individual’s natural
interest and curiosity in psychological life might override the anxiety felt by low-PM
people in similar situations. These possibilities might shed light on the question of why
high-PM patients tolerate the distressing elements of psychotherapy better than low PM
patients do. It also provokes thought about why psychotherapists are able to do work that
many people consider personally distressing.
There are several study limitations that should be addressed. First, these selfreported data provide a view of how participants see themselves, but do not provide
information about how others view them. The second limitation is that the internalconsistency estimate (␣ ⫽ .67) for the PD-IRI scale was slightly lower that Nunnally’s
(1978) recommended cutoff of .70. This might have affected the way in which this subscale related to other variables. Lastly, the sample was composed of university students;
so, the extent to which the findings generalize to other populations remains to be tested.
Multiple methods of assessment, such as peer ratings or semi-structured interviews,
might be used in future studies to examine PM from different vantage points. The relationship between PM and awareness should be studied in various samples such as psychotherapy patients, therapists, and experienced meditators to test the generalizability of
these findings. Once the awareness-related components of PM are understood, then future
research should focus how understanding of self and others relates to PM.
References
Alvarez, J.R., Farber, B.A., & Schonbar, R.A. (1998). The relationship of psychologicalmindedness to adult perceptions of early parental rejection. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
54, 1079–1084.
Applebaum, S.A. (1973). Psychological-mindedness: Word, concept, and essence. International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 54, 35– 46.
Bagby, M.R., Taylor, G.J., & Parker, J.D. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: II.
Convergent discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38,
33– 40.
Beitel, M., & Cecero, J.J. (2003). Predicting psychological mindedness from personality style and
attachment security. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 59, 163–172.
Beitel, M., Ferrer, E., & Cecero, J.C. (2004). Psychological mindedness and cognitive style. Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 60, 567–582.
Brown, K.W., & Ryan, R.M. (2003). The benefits of being present: The role of mindfulness in
psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848.
Psychological Mindedness
749
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
Conte, H.R., Plutchik, R., Jung, B.B., Picard, S., Karasu, T.B., & Lotterman, A. (1990). Psychological mindedness as a predictor of psychotherapy outcome: A preliminary report. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31, 426– 431.
Conte, H.R., Ratto, R., & Karasu, T.B. (1996). The Psychological Mindedness Scale: Factor structure and relationship to outcome of psychotherapy. Journal of Psychotherapy Research and
Practice, 5, 250–259.
Conte, H.R., & Ratto, R. (1997). Self-report measures of psychological mindedness. In M. McCallum & W.E. Piper (Eds.), Psychological mindedness: A contemporary understanding (pp. 1–
26). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO
Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional Manual. Odessa, LF: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Davis, M.H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS
Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.
Davis, M.H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.
Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 1–11.
Farber, B.A. (1985). The genesis, development, and implications of psychological mindedness in
psychotherapists. Psychotherapy, 22, 170–177.
Farber, B.A. (1989). Psychological mindedness: Can there be too much of a good thing? Psychotherapy, 26, 210–217.
Fenigstein, A. (1997). Self-consciousness and its relation to psychological mindedness. In M. McCallum & W.E. Piper (Eds.), Psychological mindedness: A contemporary understanding
(pp. 105–131). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M.F., & Buss, A.H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522–527.
Fenigstein, A., & Vanable, P.A. (1992). Paranoia and self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 62, 129–138.
Franzoi, S.L. (1983). Self-concept differences as a function of private self-consciousness and social
anxiety. Journal of Research in Personality, 17, 275–287.
Freud, S. (1953). Recommendations for physicians on the psychoanalytic method of treatment. In
J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of
Sigmund Freud (Vol. 12, pp. 109–120). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published
in 1912).
Gough, H. (1957/1975). California Psychological Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Hall, J.A. (1992). Psychological-mindedness: A conceptual model. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 46, 131–140.
Hatcher, R.L., & Hatcher, S.L. (1997). Assessing the psychological mindedness of children and
adolescents. In M. McCallum & W.E. Piper (Eds.), Psychological mindedness: A contemporary understanding (pp. 105–131). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hogan, R. (1969). Development of an empathy scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 307–316.
Horowitz, M.J. (2002). Self- and relational observation. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 12,
115–127.
McCallum, M., & Piper, W.E. (1997). The psychological mindedness assessment procedure. In
M. McCallum & W.E. Piper (Eds.), Psychological mindedness: A contemporary understanding (pp. 27–58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
750
Journal of Clinical Psychology, June 2005
Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 40,
525–543.
Nasby, W. (1989). Private and public self-consciousness and articulation of the self-schema. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 117–123.
Nunnally, J.C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill Inc.
Ryff, C.D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological
well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081.
Salovey, P., Mayer, J.D., Goldman, S., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. (1995). Emotional attention, clarity,
and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In J.W.
Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 125–154). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Scheier, M.F., Carver, C.S., & Gibson, R.X. (1979). Self-directed attention, awareness of bodily
states, and suggestibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1576–1588.
Schlesinger, H. (1994). How the analyst listens: The pre-stages of interpretation. International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 75, 31–37.
Shill, M.A., & Lumley, M.A. (2002). The Psychological Mindedness Scale: Factor structure, convergent validity and gender in a non-psychiatric sample. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research and Practice, 75, 131–150.
Trapnell, P.D., & Campbell, J.D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the Five-Factor Model of
personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 284–304.
Trudeau, K.J., & Reich, R. (1995). Correlates of psychological mindedness. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 699–704.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of
positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.