The Phonetics and
Phonology
of Retroflexes
Published by
LOT
Trans 10
3512 JK Utrecht
The Netherlands
phone: +31 30 253 6006
fax: +31 30 253 6000
e-mail:
[email protected]
http://wwwlot.let.uu.nl/
Cover illustration by Silke Hamann
ISBN 90-76864-39-X
NUR 632
Copyright © 2003 Silke Hamann. All rights reserved.
The Phonetics and Phonology of
Retroflexes
Fonetiek en fonologie van retroflexen
(met een samenvatting in het Nederlands)
Proefschrift
ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor
aan de Universiteit Utrecht
op gezag van de Rector Magnificus,
Prof. Dr. W.H. Gispen,
ingevolge het besluit van het College voor Promoties
in het openbaar te verdedigen
op vrijdag 6 juni 2003
des middags te 4.15 uur
door
Silke Renate Hamann
geboren op 25 februari 1971 te Lampertheim, Duitsland
Promotoren:
Prof. dr. T. A. Hall (Leipzig University)
Prof. dr. Wim Zonneveld (Utrecht University)
Contents
1 Introduction
1.1 Markedness of retroflexes
1.2 Phonetic cues and phonological features
1.3 Outline of the dissertation
1
3
6
8
Part I: Phonetics of Retroflexes
2 Articulatory variation and common properties of retroflexes
2.1
2.2
2.3
2.4
2.5
2.6
Phonetic terminology
Parameters of articulatory variation
2.2.1 Speaker dependency
2.2.2 Vowel context
2.2.3 Speech rate
2.2.4 Manner dependency
2.2.4.1 Plosives
2.2.4.2 Nasals
2.2.4.3 Fricatives
2.2.4.4 Affricates
2.2.4.5 Laterals
2.2.4.6 Rhotics
2.2.4.7 Retroflex vowels
2.2.5 Language family
2.2.6 Iventory size
Common articulatory properties of retroflexion
2.3.1 Apicality
2.3.2 Posteriority
2.3.3 Sublingual cavity
2.3.4 Retraction
2.3.5 Summary of characteristic properties
Retroflex fricatives in Slavic languages
2.4.1 Polish
2.4.2 Russian
2.4.3 Slavic postalveolar fricatives as non-prototypical retroflexes
Secondary palatalization of retroflexes
2.5.1 Palatalization as change in primary articulation
2.5.2 Counterexamples: Toda and Kashmiri
Summary and outlook
11
12
14
15
16
17
19
19
20
21
23
24
25
26
27
28
32
33
33
34
34
39
40
40
41
42
44
44
47
50
ii
Contents
3 Acoustic cues and perceptual properties of retroflexes
3.1
3.2
3.3
3.4
3.5
3.6
3.7
3.8
Acoustic cues used for the description of retroflexes
Acoustic realizations of the articulatory properties of retroflexes
3.2.1 Apicality
3.2.2 Posteriority
3.2.3 Sub-lingual cavity
3.2.4 Retraction
3.2.5 Summary of acoustic consequences
Cues of retroflex consonants
3.3.1 Formant frequencies
3.3.2 Spectral shape
3.3.2.1 Mandarin apical post-alveolar fricatives
3.3.2.2 Polish post-alveolar fricatives
3.3.2.3 Toda apical post-alveolar sibilants
3.3.3 Further acoustic cues
Vowel dependency of cues
Symmetrical spread of retroflex cues
Retroflexion as a weak cue and its possible enhancement
Acoustic incompatibility: Retroflexion and palatalization
Summary
53
54
55
55
56
57
58
58
59
59
63
65
65
66
68
69
72
76
77
78
Part II: Phonology of Retroflexes
4 Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
4.1
4.2
4.3
4.4
4.5
Retroflexion in rhotic context
4.1.1 Examples
4.1.2 Phonetic grounding
Patterning with back vowels
4.2.1 Examples
4.2.2 Phonetic grounding
Non-occurrence of retroflexes in front vowel context
4.3.1 De-retroflexion in front vowel context
4.3.2 Change of front vowels in retroflex context
4.3.2.1 Retraction of front vowels
4.3.2.2 Lowering of front vowels
4.3.2.3 Diphthongization of front vowels
4.3.2.4 Rounding of front vowels
4.3.2.5 Retroflexion and secondary palatalization
4.3.3 Phonetic grounding
4.3.4 Exception: The ruki-rule in Sanskrit
Retroflexion of vowels
4.4.1 Examples
4.4.2 Phonetic grounding
Phonotactics of retroflex segments
81
83
83
87
90
90
92
94
95
96
97
99
100
101
102
102
107
111
112
113
114
Contents
4.6
4.7
4.8
4.5.1 Examples
4.5.2 Phonetic grounding
Assimilation of retroflexion
4.6.1 Local assimilation
4.6.2 Non-local assimilation
4.6.3 Phonetic grounding
Excursion: Retroflex fricatives in Toda
Conclusion
5 Phonological representations of retroflexes
5.1
5.2
5.3
5.4
5.5
The functions of features
Featural representations
5.2.1 Jakobson, Fant, & Halle (1952)
5.2.2 Chomsky & Halle (1968)
5.2.3 Feature Geometry
5.2.3.1 Basic notions
5.2.3.2 Proposals for retroflexes patterning with vowels
5.2.3.3 Why does FG fail?
Alternative approaches
5.3.1 Flemming: Dispersion Theory of Contrast
5.3.2 Hamilton: Gestural and Perceptual Theory of Markedness
5.3.3 Steriade: Licensing by Cue
5.3.4 Boersma: Functional Phonology
Featural representation of retroflexes: Present approach
5.4.1 Perceptual representations
5.4.2 Articulatory representations
Conclusion
6 Analysis of retroflex processes
6.1
6.2
6.3
6.4
Constraints on perceptual representations
Constraints on articulatory representations
Analysis of phonological processes of retroflexes
6.3.1 Retroflexion via rhotics
6.3.2 Retroflexion via back vowels
6.3.3 Non-occurrence of retroflexes with front vowels
6.3.3.1 De-retroflexion
6.3.3.2 Vowel retraction and lowering
6.3.3.3 Vowel diphthongization
6.3.3.4 Vowel rounding
6.3.3.5 Secondary palatalization
6.3.4 Retroflexion of vowels
6.3.5 Phonotactics of retroflex segments
6.3.6 Assimilation of retroflexion
Conclusion
iii
115
117
118
119
122
123
125
127
131
132
134
134
136
137
137
139
141
144
144
148
150
153
157
159
161
164
167
167
170
173
174
177
179
179
180
187
187
189
191
191
193
197
iv
Contents
7 Conclusion
7.1
7.2
7.3
7.4
Phonetic characteristics of retroflexes
Phonological representations of retroflex processes
Alleged universality of the retroflex class
Future research
199
199
202
204
207
Samenvatting in het Nederlands
209
Curriculum Vitae
211
References
213
Index
223
Acknowledgements
As I myself am not a big fan of extensive acknowledgements, I want to spare the
reader long lists of fellow students, house animals, or hotshot phonologists and
phoneticians to whom I talked for less than a minute. But there are some people I
really want to thank here since they contributed to the development of this book.
First of all, these are my two supervisors, Wim Zonneveld and Tracy Alan
Hall. Both of them left me great freedom in my work, and thus gave me the
opportunity to find my own way. I want to specifically thank Wim for plainly
believing in me and always encouraging my ideas, and Tracy for teaching me how
to conduct thorough, precise research and how to ask the right questions.
Furthermore, I want to thank the members of my reading committee Colin
Ewen, Vincent van Heuven, Janet Grijzenhout, René Kager, and Daan Wissing for
their helpful comments. In addition, this thesis profitted strongly from the careful
reading and thorough comments by Paul Boersma (I have to apologize to him for not
having discussed my ideas with him until the last minute).
I also want to thank Sabine Zerbian and Hristo Velkov for helping me with the
literature and Jörg Dreyer for support in technical matters during my stays at the
ZAS Berlin.
A thank you goes out to Thorsten, who brought me to linguistics, though
unintentionally and via detours. All other friends and colleagues (from Utrecht
University, the ZAS Berlin, and the rest of the world) who are dear to me will be
thanked in person for their social (and other) support!
Lastly and most importantly I want to thank Paul for the happiness he has
brought to my life outside of linguistics.
Abbreviations and symbols
Abbreviations
ABL
AG
AGR
AUG
C
DAT
DEF
FG
FP
NOM
OT
PASS
POSS
PRES
PS
RE
REM
SG
SUP
V
ablative case
agent
agreement
augmented
consonant
dative case
definite
Feature Geometry
Functional Phonology
nominative case
Optimality Theory
passive
possessive
present tense
person
realis
remote
singular
superlative
vowel
Symbols
//
[]
⎆
⎅
.
(ω )ω
*
!
underlying representation
phonetic representation
primary stress
secondary stress
syllable boundary
prosodic word boundaries
violation of a constraint
fatal violation of a constraint
optimal candidate
1
Introduction
At the outset of this dissertation one might pose the question why retroflex
consonants should still be of interest for phonetics and for phonological theory since
ample work on this segmental class already exists. Bhat (1973) conducted a quite
extensive study on retroflexion that treated the geographical spread of this class,
some phonological processes its members can undergo, and the phonetic motivation
for these processes. Furthermore, several phonological representations of retroflexes
have been proposed in the framework of Feature Geometry, as in work by Sagey
(1986), Pulleyblank (1989), Gnanadesikan (1993), and Clements (2001). Most
recently, Steriade (1995, 2001) has discussed the perceptual cues of retroflexes and
has argued that the distribution of these cues can account for the phonotactic
restrictions on retroflexes and their assimilatory behaviour. Purely phonetically
oriented studies such as Dixit (1990) and Simonsen, Moen & Cowen (2000) have
shown the large articulatory variation that can be found for retroflexes and hint at
the insufficiency of existing definitions.
What does the present dissertation contribute to the topic of retroflexes that is
new and has not been said before? There are four main points, summarized under
(1).
(1) (a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
a new phonetic definition
description of cross-linguistically common phonological processes
phonetic grounding for these processes
phonological analysis of these processes (in OT)
First of all, a new phonetic definition of ‘retroflex’ is proposed based on four
common articulatory properties, see (1a). This definition is necessary in the light of
very recent phonetic work such as Simonsen et al. where the class of Norwegian
retroflexes is claimed to be non-retroflex since it does not fall within the traditional
definition of a tongue tip bent backwards. This claim ignores the fact that the
segments under question share a number of articulatory characteristics with
retroflexes in other languages and behave phonologically like retroflex segments.
The most notable of the four defining properties proposed here is the retraction of
the tongue body, which I claim to be present in all retroflex articulations in all
languages, opposing Bhat’s (1973) claim to the contrary. An implication of my
claim is that retroflex segments cannot be secondarily palatalized, since the two
gestures of tongue back retraction and palatalization are not producible at the same
time. The four properties yield a less restricted definition of ‘retroflexion’ than
2
Chapter 1
traditional ones and allow the inclusion of segments that were previously not
classified as retroflex, e.g. the Russian post-alveolar fricative.1
Secondly, this dissertation describes in detail phonological processes of
retroflexes that occur cross-linguistically, see (1b), giving examples from several
language families. The processes were found by comparing grammars from all
possible language families that employ retroflex segments. This aspect of my
dissertation can be viewed as an extension of Bhat’s (1973) examples of such
processes. Furthermore, the phonological processes identified provide a tool to
determine whether segments with a questionable retroflex status, e.g. [Ѐ] in Russian,
share not only the phonetic properties with retroflex segments in other languages,
but also behave phonologically like other retroflexes.
Thirdly, a phonetic grounding of the phonological processes described under
(1b) is provided, see (1c). Several cross-linguistic regularities involving retroflexes
are argued to be attributable to their common articulatory properties as defined for
(1a), and to their resulting restrictions on co-articulation. In contrast to Bhat (1973),
the grounding proposed here is not restricted to articulation; a number of processes
are shown to be caused by perceptual similarity between input and output segments.
Lastly, a phonological analysis of the phonological processes is given, see
(1d). This is done in the framework of Optimality Theory (henceforth: OT, Prince &
Smolensky 1993), which offers the possibility to express conflicting tendencies such
as articulatory simplicity and perceptual salience in terms of constraint interaction.
In particular, a slightly modified version of Boersma’s (1998, 2003b) Functional
Phonology is employed. This framework does not only include perceptual
information accounting for phonological processes, but also assumes underlying
perceptual representations, from which articulatory specifications are derived. This
departs strongly from phonological accounts employing traditional articulatory
feature representations, as e.g. in the theory of Feature Geometry (Clements 1985,
Sagey 1986), which are often used in traditional OT accounts. Furthermore, the
Functional Phonology framework departs from recent, more phonetically oriented
accounts such as Flemming (1995) and Steriade (1995, 2002), which allow
perceptual information only additionally to articulatory representations. Boersma’s
Functional Phonology framework as applied in this work is supplemented by an
articulatory model with four articulators, the tip, blade, middle, and back of the
tongue. These tongue parts are assumed to be partly dependent on each other, in
contrast to phonological models of the articulatory apparatus such as Feature
Geometry, or phonetic models such as Articulatory Phonology (Browman &
Goldstein 1989, 1992), in which a complete independence of the articulators is
assumed. This dependence can explain the non-occurrence of specific gesture
combinations, which can again account for some of the behaviour of retroflexes.
In addition to these four main points, which will be treated in separate
chapters, there are two recurrent topics in this dissertation that are not dealt with
separately. These are the markedness of the class of retroflexes including the alleged
1
Keating (1991) suggests that the Russian post-alveolar fricative could be considered retroflex but does
not state on which grounds, i.e. a definition of retroflex including this sound is missing in her article.
Introduction
3
weakness of their cues, and the relation between phonetic cues and phonological
features. These latter points are elaborated in the following subsections 1.1 and 1.2.
Section 1.3 gives an overview of the structure of this dissertation.
1.1 Markedness of retroflexes
In this section, the cross-linguistic markedness of retroflexes is described, and two
possible reasons for this markedness are investigated: their articulatory complexity
and the weakness of their cues. It will be shown that the complexity of their gesture
can be made responsible for their relatively rare occurrence cross-linguistically, but
that there is no evidence for a universal perceptual markedness of retroflexes.
Retroflexes occur relatively infrequently cross-linguistically, for instance only
11% of the languages of the world have a retroflex stop (Ladefoged & Bhaskararao
1983: 292). Furthermore, typically only large segment inventories have a retroflex
class, i.e. at least another coronal segment (apical or laminal) is present, as for
instance in Sanskrit, Hindi, Norwegian, Swedish, and numerous Australian
languages. Maddieson’s (1984) database of 317 languages mentions only one
exception to this general tendency, namely the Dravidian language Kota, which has
a retroflex as its only coronal fricative. An overview of coronal inventories of the
languages of the world with respect to retroflex classes and inventory size is given in
table 1.1, based on Maddieson (1984).2
Table 1.1 Inventories in Maddieson’s (1984) database sorted according to manner classes
(stops, nasals, and fricatives), place of articulation (either with or without a
retroflex segmental class), and the number of coronal places used by each
language in these classes (from none up to four).
manner
stops
nasals
fricatives
coronal inventories
without retroflex
with retroflex
without retroflex
with retroflex
without retroflex
with retroflex
0
1
10
29
1
place
230
0
176
0
127
1
2
place
52
21
108
7
127
5
3
place
3
5
3
7
15
10
4
place
0
5
0
6
0
2
According to Greenberg (1978) a marked structure occurs cross-linguistically less
often than its unmarked counterpart and the marked structure implies the presence of
2
The data from Maddieson’s (1984) investigation give a rough overview, however are not precise if
one looks at the details: Norwegian, for instance, is counted as a language with retroflex plosive and
nasal, but non-retroflex fricative.
4
Chapter 1
the unmarked counterpart within a language. The distribution of retroflexes given in
table 1.1 can be interpreted as an indication of the markedness of this class.3
The markedness of a segmental class is often explained phonetically by its
articulatory complexity (see Chomsky & Halle 1968: 300, among others): a
segmental class is marked because it is more difficult to articulate compared to a
class that is easier to articulate. Applying this articulatory grounding of markedness
to the class of retroflexes, it can be stated that retroflexes are more marked than
apical alveolars (or dentals),4 since retroflexes involve a raising and displacement of
the tongue tip towards the post-alveolar region, whereas an apical alveolar involves
only a tongue tip raising. The articulatory complexity can hence account for the
restricted occurrence of the retroflex class.
Within the OT approach applied in this book, markedness is formalized by
imposing that constraints militating against the articulation of more complex
segments are higher ranked than those militating against the articulation of less
complex segments. For retroflexes the following constraint hierarchy can be
assumed, see (2) (along the line of Prince & Smolensky’s 1993: 181 domination
hierarchy and Boersma’s 1998: 152 *GESTURE constraints).
(2) *RETROFLEX >> *ALVEOLAR
In traditional OT work, hierarchies as in (2) are assumed to be part of Universal
Grammar and present in the phonology of every language. They interact with
language-specific rankings of faithfulness constraints, which determine the actual
segmental inventory of a language: if a language has faithfulness constraints that are
lower ranked than the respective markedness constraints in the hierarchy in (2), then
neither a retroflex nor an alveolar segmental class emerges. If, on the other hand, the
faithfulness constraint for a segmental class is higher ranked than its respective
markedness constraint, then this class is present in a language.
In line with traditional OT theory, I state that the hierarchy in (2) is universal
and can account for cross-linguistic variation. In contrast to common belief, I argue
that its universality is based in its phonetic groundedness, and not in its existence in
a separate module called Universal Grammar.
What can also be found in the OT literature are rankings of markedness
constraints that are not phonetically grounded. Coronals, for instance, are often
argued to be less marked than labials or velars (see e.g. Paradis & Prunet 1991)
because they occur cross-linguistically more often and occur in sites of
neutralization (e.g. assimilation processes).5 A constraint hierarchy formalizing this
observation is presented in (3)(e.g. Prince & Smolensky 1993: 181).
(3) {*LABIAL; *DORSAL} >> *CORONAL
3
4
5
A further indication for the markedness of retroflexes is their late acquisition, compared to other
segmental classes. Acquisitional studies addressing retroflexes are few, but Lobacz (1996) reports that
in Polish the retroflex fricative is acquired much later than the dental fricative.
Here and throughout the dissertation the shorthand ‘apical alveolar’ stands for both apical alveolar and
apical dental articulations.
These observations have led several phonologists (Archangeli 1988, Davis 1991, and others) to
propose an underspecification of the coronal class.
Introduction
5
However, the markedness hierarchy in (3) is not grounded in articulation since it is
impossible to know whether articulations with one articulator, e.g. the lips, are more
difficult to produce than articulations with another articulator, e.g. the tongue tip or
blade. For this reason, the frequently assumed universality of hierarchies like the one
in (3) is doubted here, and they are considered irrelevant for the markedness
considerations concerning retroflexes.
Phonologists like Flemming (1995, 1999), Hamilton (1996), and Padgett
(2001, 2003) argue that it is necessary to include perceptual information in
phonological accounts of segmental classes, and therefore assume a perceptual
markedness of segments. The relative notion of perceptual markedness can be
defined in two ways: either as a universal relation between a perceptually more and
a perceptually less salient segment class (similar to the articulatory markedness
hierarchy in (2)) as is done by Hamilton, or as an inventory-specific comparison of
the perceptual distance between segments, where perceptually more distant segment
inventories are less marked, as by Flemming and Padgett.
The first notion of perceptual markedness can often be found in the phonetic
literature, where the term ‘weak feature’ is used to denote a feature that is
perceptually less salient than others and is detected by the auditory system only after
the so-called robust features (Stevens & Keyser 1989). In this sense, weak features
are perceptually marked, a generalization that is attested by Ohala’s (1993: 89)
remark that the distinction between robust vs. weak features correlates nicely with
the way segment inventories in languages are constructed: “those with a small
number of phonemes use the robust features almost exclusively; those with many
phonemes use the same robust features but also exploit weaker, slower features”.
According to this definition, retroflexion should be considered perceptually weak or
marked since it occurs rarely and even then only in large inventories. However, no
perceptual evidence could be found that perceptual cues of retroflexion are harder to
detect than those of any other place of articulation (as illustrated in chapter 3 below).
Therefore, perceptual markedness of retroflexes according to this universal notion
will not be assumed in this dissertation. If additional information restricts or adds to
the perceptibility of a segmental class, then this can be captured by faithfulness
constraints. The perceptibility of retroflexes, for instance, can be ranked according
to the cue availability in specific contexts, as illustrated in (4), where R stands for
features specifying retroflexes (evidence for this constraint ordering, especially for
the universally unusual ranking V_C >> C_V, will be given in section 3.5).
(4) *DELETE (R / V_V) >> *DELETE (R / V_C) >> *DELETE (R / C_V) >>
*DELETE (R / C_C)
The hierarchy in (4) incorporates the fact that retroflexes are least perceptible in
interconsonantal context, and most perceptible in intervocalic context (see Steriade
1995 and 2002 for a similar approach). Such a hierarchy is applied in this
dissertation to account for the phonotactic behaviour and the assimilatory behaviour
of retroflexes.
6
Chapter 1
The second type of perceptual markedness is restricted to the segments within
a language inventory. This is illustrated by Padgett (2003: 5) with the case of
palatalized and velarized consonants in Slavic languages. Both series are present for
instance in Russian, but Russian does not employ the plain consonants without a
secondary articulation. The presence of the plain series is predicted, however, by
articulatory markedness: only if the plain articulation is present can secondary
articulations (which are articulatorily more complex and thus marked) emerge as
well. According to Padgett, the two secondary articulations in Russian are licensed
since they are perceptually more distant than the plain articulation compared to one
secondary articulated consonant. Phonologists working with this inventory-specific
notion of perceptual markedness, such as Flemming (1995) and Padgett (2001,
2003), restrict their analyses to comparing segmental inventories with respect to one
perceptual cue only. However, segments might be perceptually distant with respect
to one cue, but perceptually very close with respect to another. For this reason and
due to lack of sufficient data on the perceptual cues of inventories containing
retroflexes, this notion of perceptual markedness is not pursued here.
A third notion of markedness is introduced by Bhat (1973: 55), who claims
that retroflex segments are marked because they are introduced into a language
through areal spread, through the assimilatory influence of neighbouring sounds
such as back vowels, /r/ or, at a later stage, by other retroflex consonants. Bhat’s
observation only confirms the implication made earlier that retroflexes mostly occur
in large segmental inventories, when other coronals are employed already. The areal
spread of retroflexes provides further evidence for the observation that retroflexes
do not have weak perceptual features, since languages are expected to borrow
segments with strong perceptual features (compare for instance the areal spread of
the click sounds in Bantu languages) rather than segments with weak features.
In sum, there is evidence that an articulatory basis for the cross-linguistic
markedness of retroflexes exists. Furthermore, it was shown that retroflexes are not
in general perceptually more marked than other segmental classes (for instance by
having weak cues), but that their perceptibility is context-dependent. This contextdependent notion of markedness will be employed in the present dissertation to
account for the phonological behaviour of retroflexes such as phonotactic
restrictions and assimilation processes.
1.2 Phonetic cues and phonological features
One of the main questions in the present thesis is whether it is necessary and
desirable to have one universal set of features for phonological representations (for
retroflexes and for other classes), or whether language-specific features are more
useful as they take into account other sounds in the respective languages and hence
put emphasis on the distinctive function of features within a language (cf. Jakobson,
Fant & Halle 1952). The assumption of a universal set of features in phonology
stems from the aim to have only one, highly abstract set that can account for all
languages. What is neglected by such abstraction is the fact that phonological rules
Introduction
7
often target phonetically very detailed cues in specific languages, as will be
illustrated in chapters 4 and 6. This dilemma between abstraction and concrete
detail, and the problem of drawing the borderline between phonological and
phonetic knowledge is illustrated in the following citation by Ladefoged & Wu
(1984: 277) on consonant classes:
[…] as phonologists, we will continue to describe the patterns among the contrasting
sounds. As phoneticians, we will continue to describe the actual sounds that occur.
Phonologists must behave as if there were distinct places of articulation, grouping
sounds together in ways that are appropriate for the particular language being
described. Meanwhile phoneticians will have to go on doing their best to specify the
sounds of each language in general anatomical and acoustic terms. They will not be
able to allocate consonants to a small number of cells on a chart […]. Languages divide
up the continuum of possible places of articulation in different ways […]. There are, of
course, favored regions that occur in many languages. But the phones that are grouped
together phonologically in one language will not be the same as those that are grouped
together in another. Nobody imagines that the vowel and tone spaces are divided into
specific sets of categories. Why should we imagine that there are discrete places of
articulation?
This quotation suggests that phonological representations should be abstracted away
from the possible articulatory variation within a language. They should, however,
not try to generalize across all languages to find one classification for all places of
articulation. Such generalizations might be useful for very general cross-linguistic
observations, but cannot be used as a tool for the phonological description of
specific languages. I will assume a similar view of phonetic-phonological sound
patterning here, in line with the general idea of functional approaches to phonology
such as Boersma (1998) and Pierrehumbert (1999).
The resulting model of phonological knowledge as assumed in this thesis is
represented in figure 1.1.
Phonetics, “universal”
Phonological descriptive device
for language comparisons
Phonology of a specific language
language-specific
arbitrary processes
language-specific phonetic
language-specific abstraction from
Figure 1.1 Model of phonological knowledge (the box outlined in bold in the middle) and its
sources as assumed in the present dissertation.
8
Chapter 1
In this figure, the left, light grey box represents the phonetics, which is based on the
articulatory and acoustic apparatus of the human species and in this sense is
universal. All of this phonetic knowledge is available to the language learner. From
this, the learner chooses the cues that are relevant for the language-specific
phonological processes (i.e., the smaller box within the grey box). To this phonetic
knowledge relevant for phonology, the learner adds language-specific, arbitrary
phonological knowledge, depicted as the striped box in the middle. Such knowledge
includes processes that have to be specifically stored in the lexicon. Lastly, the
phonology of a specific language also includes abstract phonological representations
and processes, represented here as the small dark grey box on the right. This part of
the phonology is an abstraction from the phonetic knowledge. The abstract,
phonological knowledge partly overlaps with what linguists traditionally refer to as
the innate Universal Grammar, the big grey box on the right. I propose that this socalled innate or universal module is only a descriptive device for language
comparisons and yields an economic description of all languages. It is, however, not
necessary to assume such an innate component since phonological knowledge can
be acquired without it. In the present thesis it is assumed that cross-linguistic
similarities of phonological segments and processes do not originate from a
Universal Grammar but from their groundedness in the articulatory and perceptual
apparatus and the categorizing mechanisms of the human, in line with functional
approaches to phonology such as Nathan (1994), Pierrehumbert (1994), and
Boersma (1998).
In general, the model in figure 1.1 represents the phonological knowledge of a
speaker, its dependence on phonetics, and its interrelation with what is traditionally
termed UG. It is, however, not meant to give a full-fledged model of phonology and
phonetics or other grammatical modules. In addition to this phonological
component, the speaker is assumed to have, amongst others, a component including
phonetic knowledge that is not relevant for phonological processes but necessary for
the realization of segments, a lexical component, and so on.
1.3 Outline of the dissertation
The dissertation is divided into two sections, a phonetic one and a phonological one.
The first section, which subsumes chapters 2 and 3, is concerned with the phonetic
definition of retroflexion. In chapter 2, several articulatory parameters responsible
for the large variation of this class are described, and four prototypical properties for
retroflexion are proposed. Chapter 3 provides acoustic definitions of retroflexion
and two cues that hold for all retroflex segments. In terms of the model given in
figure 1.1, chapters 2 and 3 set the possible space for retroflex articulations and
perceptual cues within the general phonetics space, from which languages with a
retroflex class pick their actual realizations. This retroflex space is depicted
schematically in figure 1.2 within the model as introduced in figure 1.1.
Introduction
9
Figure 1.2 Schema of the part of the model that is discussed in chapters 2 and 3. The small
grey box indicates the possible retroflex articulations (within the phonetics
space), from which languages choose their exact realizations. The boxes with
dashed outlines illustrate two language-specific choices from the phonetic space
(including all non-retroflexes).
In figure 1.2 and the following figures, the part of the model that is dealt with in the
respective chapters is highlighted either by grey hatching or by bold lines.
The second section, chapters 4 - 6, deals with the phonology of retroflex
segments. The fourth chapter is concerned with phonological processes involving
retroflex segments that can be found in many languages with this segment class.
Furthermore, chapter 4 illustrates the phonetic grounding of these processes, but also
to what extent the processes are idiosyncratic and not phonetically motivated. In my
model, this can be depicted as in figure 1.3.
Figure 1.3 Topic of chapter 4: Phonological processes involving retroflexes and their
phonetic grounding. Here depicted by the phonology of two possible languages
(excluding the abstract part in the right box) and their overlap with respect to
retroflexion.
In chapter 5, I discuss previous phonological representations of retroflexes that
primarily focused on the search for universal representations and thus mainly
ignored language-specific, phonetic details. In this respect, chapter 5 deals with the
right box in our model, the supposedly “universal” knowledge. This chapter also
includes newer proposals for representations that refer to phonetic (especially
perceptual) details.
10
Chapter 1
Figure 1.4 Phonological representations as topic of chapter 5. These include both
approaches that refer to universal feature sets, and others that use only languagespecific features.
In chapter 6, I propose phonological representations of retroflex-specific processes,
based on the phonetic details chosen by particular languages. These representations
partially describe and account for the variation between languages with respect to
specific processes. This variation, and the cross-linguistical commonness of a
specific variant versus the rare occurrence of another variant, is accounted for by the
universality of the phonetic apparatus. Chapter 6 thus deals with the whole
phonological knowledge of several languages as depicted with the central boxes in
figure 1.5.
Figure 1.5 Topic of chapter 6 are language-specific and universal phonological features that
can account for the phonological processes of retroflexes (topic of chapter 4).
Again, two sample phonologies of specific languages are depicted.
Chapter 7 concludes and points out advantages and shortcomings of the present
dissertation. Furthermore, it gives an overview of topics that could not be dealt with
here and are open to future research.
2
Articulatory variation and
common properties of retroflexes
Retroflexion is traditionally described as an articulation involving the bending
backwards of the tongue tip, see for instance the definition by Trask (1996: 308). An
illustration of such a retroflex is given in figure 2.1, based on a sagittal x-ray tracing
of a Tamil retroflex stop from Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996: 27).
Figure 2.1
Tracing of a sagittal x-ray of a retroflex stop in Tamil, based on Ladefoged &
Maddieson (1996: 27). This articulation is a retroflex in the traditional sense
with the tongue tip bent backwards and having contact on the post-alveolar
area.
The Latin-based term ‘retroflex’ (Dixit 1990), bending backwards, refers to a tongue
gesture as depicted in figure 2.1.
In the present chapter it will be shown that the class of retroflexes displays
large articulatory variation, and that the actual gesture of bending the tongue tip
backwards is not a defining property of this class because it is not true for all sounds
traditionally described as retroflex. This chapter investigates the articulatory
variation of retroflexes and the factors responsible for variation, and proposes a
different articulatory definition of retroflexion, namely by the four properties of
apicality, posteriority, sublingual cavity, and retraction. Bending backwards of the
tongue tip is not included in these four properties. It will be shown that most of the
segments traditionally described as retroflex satisfy all four properties. One
noticable exception to this is the retroflex fricative in Mandarin, that lacks the
property of posteriority. Furthermore, the four properties introduced here will be
shown to imply that secondary palatalization of retroflexion causes a change from
retroflex to non-retroflex, as the palatalized segment satisfies only two retroflex
properties.
This chapter proceeds as follows. In section 2.1, the phonetic terminology to
be used is defined. Section 2.2 is concerned with both the phonetic and phonological
factors responsible for the large articulatory variation in retroflexion. This includes
descriptions of the manners of articulation that retroflexion can occur with.
Abstracting away from the described variation, section 2.3 describes in detail the
12
Chapter 2
four properties that characterize retroflexion; apicality, posteriority, sublingual
cavity, and retraction. In section 2.4, these properties are applied to post-alveolar
fricatives of Slavic languages which are treated in the traditional literature as laminal
but which are argued here to be retroflex. The subsection illustrates that these Slavic
segments can be considered as non-prototypical retroflex fricatives since they satisfy
three of the four properties postulated in 2.3. Section 2.4 concludes that the property
of posteriority is the only one that can be missing for such non-prototypical retroflex
fricatives. Section 2.5 is concerned with the secondary palatalization of retroflexes.
There it is argued that secondary palatalization causes a change from retroflex to
palato-alveolar. The last section 2.6 summarizes.
2.1 Phonetic terminology
In this dissertation, several articulatory terms are used. To avoid confusion, short
definitions of these terms are provided here. First of all, a distinction between active
and passive articulator is made. The active articulator is either the lower lips or some
part of the tongue. This active articulator (sometimes just referred to as ‘articulator’
in the phonetic literature) moves towards the immobile, passive articulator, which is
the area on the upper side of the vocal tract from the upper lips to the pharynx. The
passive articulator is also referred to as ‘place of articulation’ in most phonetic
studies.
For the present study, two specific regions of the active and the passive
articulator are of importance. In the case of the active articulator this is the front part
of the tongue, which is usually divided into tongue tip and tongue blade. This area is
often referred to as ‘coronal’ in the phonetic and phonological literature. Sounds
produced with the tongue tip are called apical, those with the tongue blade laminal.
There are two differing views on where the borderline between tip and blade is
located. According to the traditional view, as represented e.g. by Catford (1988,
1994) and Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996), the tip is the part of the tongue that is
roughly vertical at rest, and the tongue blade starts just a few millimetres after this
vertical part, see figure 2.2 below. A different view is represented by e.g. Ohde &
Sharf (1992), who count both tip and blade as defined above under the term ‘tongue
tip’ and use ‘blade’ for the front area of what is traditionally referred to as the
tongue dorsum. The present study follows the traditional definition of tongue tip and
blade.
Figure 2.2
Representation of the sublingual cavity and of the parts of the tongue (based on
Catford 1988).
Articulatory variation and common properties
13
The underside of the tongue, i.e. the area below the tongue tip, can be referred to as
subapical (e.g. Catford 1977 and Laver 1994), or sublaminal (e.g. Ladefoged &
Maddieson 1996 and Dixit 1990). In the present study, the term ‘subapical’ will be
used. The space between the tongue tip and the lower teeth that comes into existence
when the tongue tip or blade is raised towards the post-alveolar region or further
back is called the ‘sublingual cavity’.
Behind the laminal area, which extends 1 to 1.5 cm behind the apex, the
tongue dorsum starts. The tongue dorsum is sometimes referred to as tongue body
and can be further distinguished into pre-dorsum and post-dorsum, or anterodorsum
and posterodorsum (e.g. by Catford 1988), the latter often called the tongue back.
According to Catford (1988: 79) this distinction of the dorsum is not necessary,
since the anterodorsum practically always articulates against the roof of the mouth in
the palatal area, and the posterodorsum articulates in the velar area. In the present
study I will refer to these two parts of the dorsum as tongue middle and tongue back,
respectively.
For the passive articulator, the focus of the present study will be on the area on
or behind the alveolar ridge. The alveolar ridge is the protuberance immediately
behind the upper teeth, after which a concave slope of the palate starts (cf. for
instance to the definition by Catford 1977). The alveolar ridge can be seen in figure
2.3 in the alveolar area:
Figure 2.3
Representation of the alveolar, post-alveolar, alveolo-palatal, palatal, and velar
regions (based on figures by Heike 1982: 26 and Laver 1994).
Articulations on the alveolar ridge are called ‘alveolar’; those right behind the ridge
are ‘post-alveolar’. Behind the short stretch of the post-alveolar region, the palatal
area is located. The remainder of the palate is the ‘velar’ area.
Besides these distinctions of active and passive articulators, some terms for
specific sound classes have to be defined explicitly since they will be used
throughout the dissertation. First of all, I will use the term ‘apical alveolar’ for all
front apical articulations, including apical dentals. The anterior apical is realized
language-dependently, but the realization of an apical alveolar is cross-linguistically
more common (Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996: 23), therefore this term is used here.
Apical alveolars are transcribed with the IPA symbols [t, d, n, l, s, z, r] without
further diacritics. Laminal dentals or alveolars are transcribed with a dental diacritic,
14
Chapter 2
for instance [t], since they are dental in most languages. Laminal post-alveolars are
transcribed as [t] etc.
Laminal fricatives articulated in the post-alveolar region, transcribed with the
IPA symbols [ , ], will be termed ‘palato-alveolar’ (following e.g. Trask 1996:
255). Together with the retroflexes they make up the class of ‘post-alveolar’
fricatives. Laminal fricatives articulated with a narrow constriction at the postalveolar and the palatal area, i.e. [ , ], are referred to as ‘alveolo-palatal’ fricatives
(e.g., by Trask 1996).
Regarding the tongue shape, the terms ‘domed’ and ‘flat’ will be used in the
present dissertation. ‘Domed’ describes a raised tongue middle as can be found in
articulations of the palato-alveolar [ , ], or in any kind of secondarily palatalized
coronals. ‘Flat’ can be used in two different meanings, one indicating the nonraising of the tongue middle towards the palate, i. e. flatness in the sagittal
dimension: in the present study the term will be used in this meaning. The second
meaning is applied e.g. by Laver (1994: 252) who uses ‘flat’ to refer to a fricative
that lacks a groove in the tongue middle, i.e. flatness refers here to the crosssectional dimension of the tongue.
2.2 Parameters of articulatory variation
In contrast to other articulatory classes (such as e.g. dentals) that have a fixed place
of articulation and active articulator and therefore allow little variation, retroflexes
have a wide range of articulatory possibilities, which will be examined in this
section. The indeterminacy of the retroflex class is reflected by the fact that the IPA
symbol chart lists ‘retroflex’ along with terms referring to places of articulation such
as ‘dental’, ‘alveolar’ etc., although I argue below that ‘retroflex’ describes an
articulatory shape or gesture, rather than a place of articulation. Ladefoged (1975)
nevertheless uses ‘retroflex’ in his phonetic feature systems as a value of the feature
‘articulatory place’. A similar use of this term can be found in Maddieson (1984).
Catford (1977), Ohala (1983), and Laver (1994), on the other hand, apply the term
‘retroflex’ exclusively to the articulatory gesture.
Besides the cover term ‘retroflex’, one can find several more accurate
descriptions of retroflex segments in the phonetic literature; terms used for the active
articulator are e.g. ‘apical’, ‘subapical’ or ‘sublaminal’; terms for the passive
articulator are ‘alveolar’, ‘post-alveolar’, ‘prepalatal’ or ‘palatal’. Nevertheless,
‘retroflex’ behaves parallel to true places of articulation because it has the same
manner of articulation classes, i.e. plosives, fricatives, nasals, and so on.
The difficulty in describing the articulation of retroflexes is due to several
parameters of variation, some of which are purely phonetic, such as speaker
dependency, dependency on the manner of articulation, or the segmental context of
the retroflex. Others are phonological, such as the inventory size of a language or
common characteristics of the language family.
In this section, the main parameters of variation are described, starting with the
phonetically motivated intra-speaker variation in 2.2.1. Three further phonetic
Articulatory variation and common properties
15
sources for variation are subsequently introduced, namely the vowel context of the
segments (2.2.2), speech rate (2.2.3), and manner of articulation (2.2.4). In sections
2.2.5 and 2.2.6, two phonological parameters, namely language family and inventory
size, are described respectively. Mutual influence of the factors on each other
occurs, as will be illustrated when appropriate.
2.2.1 Speaker dependency
The shape of the tongue and the exact placement of the tongue tip depend very much
on the speaker’s vocal tract. There is large variation between speakers in their vocal
tract anatomy and in their flexibility of the articulators. Concerning the place of
articulation for retroflexes, Catford (1970: 310) points out that “some people can
curl up their tongue retroflexively so that the tongue-point touches the uvula: others
can barely reach the centre of the hard palate”. An apical uvular (i.e. a retroflex in
the uvular region) is therefore theoretically possible for some speakers, but no
language seems to employ this sound.
The active articulator for retroflexes is also subject to speaker variability; it
can vary from the tongue tip or blade to the extreme underside of the blade.
Phonetic investigations of the realization of retroflex segments within one
language confirm intra-speaker variation. Ladefoged & Bhaskararao (1983) made xray tracings of Hindi voiceless retroflex stops for five native speakers. Whereas four
of the speakers have an alveolar place of articulation (varying between apical and
subapical articulator), the fifth shows a subapical post-alveolar articulation. An
electropalatographic (henceforth: EPG) and electromagnetographic study of
Norwegian retroflex stops by Simonsen et al. (2000) for two native speakers showed
similar variation between the speakers; one articulates the retroflex with a bent
tongue tip and a post-alveolar contact, the other without bending and with an
alveolar contact. That variation depending on the speaker is consistent across
different manners can be seen in Dart & Nihalani’s (1999) experiment for
Malayalam. Their palatographic and linguographic data of nine speakers show that
eight speakers produce post-alveolar retroflex stops, and one an alveolar one. The
same speaker also shows an alveolar place of the retroflex nasal, whereas all others
have a post-alveolar nasal. The consistency of the single speaker’s articulation
across different manners was also observable for the active articulator.
The degree of variation open to the speaker can be limited by the inventory
size of the language, as will be shown in section 2.2.5. But even for Australian
languages that employ a four-way place distinction in the coronal area (such as for
instance Eastern Arrernte, Miriwung, and Wembawemba), and where one therefore
would expect nearly no variation, Hamilton (1996: 37) observes that the articulation
of retroflexes is less uniform than that of the alveolars, and furthermore that the area
of contact for retroflex segments is much broader.
Variation due to a speaker’s gender should also be mentioned in the context of
speaker-specific differences in articulation. The vocal folds are usually longer for
men than for women, and vocal fold vibration is therefore lower for men (Beck
1997). Furthermore, the vocal tract is generally larger for men, which results in
16
Chapter 2
lower formant values. However, this is a general phenomenon not restricted to
retroflex segments and has therefore not been the topic of any articulatory study on
retroflexion.
2.2.2 Vowel context
Vowel context seems to be one of the major causes for variability in retroflexion.
Several studies such as Sv arny & Zvelebil (1955), Ladefoged & Bhaskararao
(1983), Dixit (1990), Dixit & Flege (1991), and Krull, Lindblom, Shia & Fruchter
(1995) illustrate the change of the active articulator and the place of articulation of
retroflex segments according to the adjacent vowel. Dixit (1990) and Dixit & Flege
(1991) for instance made EPG measurements of the retroflex stops [] in Hindi
preceded and followed by the vowels [a u i]. The results of both studies show that
the place of retroflex constriction changes according to the vowels. In /i/ context, i.e.
preceded and followed by an /i/, the retroflexes were articulated in the dental area, in
/a/ context in the alveolar region, and in /u/ context in the post-alveolar area. Similar
results were obtained by Simonsen et al.’s (2000) EPG experiments with two native
speakers of Norwegian, where the retroflex in the /a/ vowel context was articulated
far more back than in the /i/ context. Variance in retroflexion according to vowel
context is obviously a coarticulatory effect and can be explained physiologically.
For the front vowel /i/ the tongue blade is fronted and the tongue middle raised, and
the tongue tip is usually tucked under the lower front teeth. This tongue shape is
inherently less compatible with that of retroflexion than e.g. a neutral tongue
position, and therefore the combination of both front vowel and retroflex gesture
results in the reduction of either the degree of retroflexion or the frontness of the
vowel. For both processes see the phonological rules of de-retroflexion in high front
vowel context, high vowel lowering, and front vowel retraction, all described in
section 4.3. The articulatory gesture for the low vowel /a/ has neither a fronted
tongue blade nor a raised tongue middle, thus it allows a posterior retroflex
articulation. The high back vowel /u/ is articulated with a raised and retracted tongue
back, a position very similar to that of the retroflex. The tip is not constrained in this
type of articulation and can move easily to or from the retroflex gesture.
The position of the vowel with respect to the retroflex has a considerable
influence on the variation of retroflexes. The quality of the preceding vowel has a
different effect on retroflexion than that of the following one. In Simonsen et al.’s
(2000) study on Norwegian retroflex stops, the contact on the palate was further
back in post-vocalic position than in prevocalic position, both for /i/ and /a/ context.
Krull et al. (1995) have similar results in their study of Swedish, Hindi, and Tamil
retroflexes. The difference between post-vocalic and pre-vocalic position is that
during the preceding vowel the tongue tip advances into the post-alveolar region.
The retroflex consonant itself involves a movement of the tongue tip from this postalveolar displacement to its natural resting position or to the position necessary for
the following vowel. This is sometimes referred to as ‘flapping out’, see e.g.
Ladefoged (1964) on West African rhotic retroflexes or Hamilton (1996: 37) on
Australian retroflex segments. The movement from the retroflex gesture towards
Articulatory variation and common properties
17
neutral tongue position or tongue position of the following segment during the
production of the consonant is very pronounced for retroflex stops and nasals
(illustrated e.g. in Butcher’s 1992 palatography of retroflex stops in Australian
languages). Retroflex fricatives differ slightly, as they usually hold the posterior
tongue position during almost all of the frication and only show a very late flapping
out.1 But in judging these differences one has to take into account that fricatives
usually show less bending backwards of the tongue tip than stops, see section 2.2.4.3
below. The articulatory influence of the vowel preceding a retroflex segment is thus
far greater than that of the following vowel, and more vowel-dependent variation
can be observed in post-vocalic retroflexes than in pre-vocalic ones. This
dependency on position of the vowel is again unique to retroflexion. Dentals do not
show any variation across post- and pre-vocalic position, as illustrated by Krull et
al.’s (1995) study on Swedish and Hindi, and Simonsen et al.’s (2000) study on
Norwegian.
2.2.3 Speech rate
Speech rate is another factor influencing the articulation of retroflex segments. Bhat
(1974b: 236) states in his cross-linguistic study that the bending backwards of the
tongue tip decreases with the speed of the utterance. This is in accordance with
Ladefoged & Maddieson’s (1996: 222) finding that some speakers of Malayalam
have a contrast between alveolar and retroflex trill in careful speech only. Dixit &
Flege (1991) tested in an EPG experiment the influence of speech rate on retroflex
articulations of the Hindi stop []. Their results show that in fast speech the
constriction for the retroflex is more posterior. No information was provided about
the reduction of the tongue tip gesture. The retraction of the place of articulation
seems to go counter expectations, because one would suppose a reduction of the
retroflex gesture to result in a position closer to the neutral position of the tongue tip.
Dixit & Flege (1991) explain their findings in the following way:
[T]he place of tongue-palate contact receded in fast speech probably because the
general shape of the tongue was more like its bunched shape in the surrounding vowel.
Therefore the tongue tip or blade did not reach the same articulatory place in fast
speech as it did in normal or loud speech. (Dixit & Flege, 1991: 223)
Dixit & Flege hence propose that the bunched tongue body shape of the vowel
influences the tongue shape of the retroflex in fast speech, which results in a more
posterior place of articulation for the retroflex stop. Alternatively, the retraction of
the tongue body might be interpreted as a compensatory gesture: the fast speech rate
does not allow a raising and retraction of the tongue tip, thus the speaker retracts the
whole tongue body (which can be done during the vowel artiuclation already) and
then only has to raise the tongue tip.
1
The difference between retroflex fricatives and stops with respect to the occurrence of the ‘flapping
out’ is not the subject of any articulatory study to my knowledge. It can also be inferred from the
transitions in the acoustic signals, see chapter 3.
18
Chapter 2
Lindblad & Lundqvist (1997) looked at Swedish coronal fricatives in isolated
vs. phrasal production (which I assume to be equivalent to slow vs. fast speech).
Their results show a more fronted articulation of the retroflex fricative in phrasal,
fast speech; i.e. a change from a post-alveolar/palatal place of articulation in isolated
speech to an alveolar place in fast speech. Both studies only looked at a change in
the place of articulation, not at the tongue tip. Whether the tongue tip gesture is
reduced in fast speech has to be further tested. Furthermore, it cannot be established
whether the differing results for Hindi and Swedish are language-specific (Hindi has
retroflex retraction in fast speech whereas Swedish has fronting) or manner-specific
(fricatives are fronted whereas stops are retracted). Further studies have to be
conducted to clarify this point.
2.2.4 Manner dependency
The third and most important phonetic parameter for variation of retroflex sounds is
the manner of articulation. As we will see in the following, there are some
systematic differences between retroflex manners. This difference is not restricted to
retroflexes but is a general articulatory necessity. Lindblad & Lundqvist (1999: 417)
observe in their study on front coronal sounds that “the hypothesis that sounds that
share roughly the same articulatory gesture nevertheless tend to differ in gestural
details, caused by general production conditions for each specific articulatory
manner, is certainly valid also for all other sounds, not least posterior coronals such
as retroflex sounds.” To exemplify this in detail, the following sections will examine
the retroflex articulations of plosives, fricatives, affricates, laterals, and rhotics
respectively. Manners articulated with ingressive airstream, such as implosives and
clicks, and with glottalic egressive airstream (ejectives) are not discussed.
Before describing the manners of articulation for retroflexes, it has to be
pointed out that retroflexes also occur as geminates, as in Bengali, Marathi, and
Tamil. As geminates can occur with all of the manners described below, they are
treated briefly here. Balasubramanian (1982) investigated the articulatory difference
between singleton and geminate nasal and lateral retroflex in Tamil and his study
shows that retroflex geminates, which occur intervocalically only, are articulated
with a firmer contact than the non-geminated counterparts. The nasal retroflex
geminates are also articulated further back than the singleton nasals, namely in the
palatal region. Thus, the realization as singleton or geminate can account already for
some articulatory variation of retroflexes.
2.2.4.1 Plosives
In the phonetic and phonological literature one often finds the distinction between
two kinds of retroflex stops. Keating (1991: 34f.) and Hall (1997: 46) for example
describe an apical and a subapical post-alveolar retroflex segment, where the second
involves a more retroflex gesture than the first.2 Figure 2.4 exemplifies these two
2
Keating (1991) and Hall (1997) do not refer to stops specifically, but their articulatory illustrations for
both types of retroflex articulations are taken from stops only.
Articulatory variation and common properties
19
stop types, with a retroflex stop from Hindi on the left, and one from Tamil on the
right.
Figure 2.4
Hindi retroflex stop [] (left) and a Tamil retroflex stop [] (right), both based
on sagittal x-ray tracings from Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996: 27).
The Hindi retroflex stop is articulated with the tongue tip against the post-alveolar
region, whereas the Tamil retroflex stop involves a placement of the underside of the
tongue tip against the palatal region. Whereas Hall (1997) assumes that there are
only these two retroflex articulations, Ladefoged & Bhaskararao (1983) point out
that the two types like the ones illustrated here can be assumed to be extremes on a
continuum of possible retroflex plosives.3 Taking into account all the other
parameters of variation described up to now and the changes in articulation they
incur, the view of retroflex articulation as a continuum seems appropriate. But in
order to avoid mentioning all possible articulations of retroflex stops, the following
will be mainly concerned with the description of the two extreme types.
In order to distinguish phonetically between these two extreme articulations of
retroflex stops, Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996: 15) introduce two different symbols.
The type found in Hindi, involving just the tongue tip (see figure 2.4 left), is
transcribed with a subscript dot beneath the symbol for the alveolar sound [t]
·
whereas the articulation with the underside of the tongue (see figure 2.4 right) as in
Tamil and other Dravidian languages is transcribed with the traditional IPA symbol
for retroflex stop []. Svarny & Zvelebil (1955) also distinguish the two types, and
introduce a different term for each. In their terminology, ‘retroflex’ stops involve the
use of the underside of the tongue, whereas ‘cacuminal’ stops use the tongue tip
only.4 Ball & Rahilly (1999: 56) propose the use of the IPA diacritics for ‘advanced’
and ‘retracted’ to differentiate between the two retroflex places of articulation.
Sections 2.2.5 and 2.2.6 below look at the correlation between the two retroflex
types in figure 2.4 with the language families they occur in, and their co-occurrence
with other coronal segments within one language, respectively.
Usually, the voiced and voiceless retroflex segments in one language are
articulated at the same place. However, Ladefoged (1964) observed that some West
3
4
Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996: 26) assume that the retroflex voiced stop in Ewe (a West-African
language) is even less retroflexed than the Hindi one, being articulated actually at the alveolar ridge,
and therefore should be one endpoint of the continuum. Their palatographic data, however, show that
the tongue moved from the post-alveolar to the alveolar area during articulation, which makes its
place of articulation very similar if not identical to that of the Hindi stop.
Dixit (1990: 190) points out that ‘cacuminal’ is a term referring to the place of articulation rather than
to the tongue gesture, though Svarny & Zvelebil use both ‘retroflex’ and ‘cacuminal’ for describing
the active articulator.
20
Chapter 2
African languages (Ghanaian languages such as Ga, Effutu, Late and Anum) have a
difference in place of articulation between the voiceless and the voiced coronals.
Whereas the voiceless stop is laminal denti-alveolar, the voiced one is apical
alveolar or post-alveolar, i.e. retroflex. No possible reasons for this difference are
given. The only further evidence for a difference between the voiced and voiceless
series could be found in Dixit’s (1990) measurements of Hindi, which showed that
the voiced retroflex (and dental) have narrower constrictions than the voiceless
counterparts. But no change in place of articulation resulted from that.
Retroflex stops have in common that they involve a flapping out of the tongue
tip in their articulation. This gestural release takes place at the release of the stop.5 In
Ladefoged’s (1964) investigations of Ewe stops, the tongue tip movement during the
release is observable from a large area of contact at the roof of the mouth, though
the active articulator was actually quite small. Further unifying properties for both
retroflex stop types and also of the following manners will be discussed in 2.3.
2.2.4.2 Nasals
Coronal nasals are often produced at the same place of articulation and with the
same articulator as the corresponding stops in the respective languages. Maddieson
(1986) observes that the presence of a nasal usually implies the presence of a plosive
or obstruent at the same place of articulation. This is attested in Dart’s study (1991)
on the articulatory similarity between coronal stops and coronal nasals in French and
in English.6
For retroflexes, Laver (1994: 217) shows with sagittal cross-sections that the
retroflex stops and nasals in Tamil have identical active and passive articulators and
the same gesture of articulation.
Further evidence for a similar treatment of retroflex nasals and stops can be
drawn from the fact that languages which employ a retroflex nasal also have a
retroflex stop. The only counterexample in Maddieson’s database is the Finno-Ugric
language Ostyak, which has a retroflex affricate and a retroflex lateral as the only
other retroflex segments besides the retroflex nasal.
Butcher (1992: 25) claims that in Australian languages stops appear to be more
likely to have sublaminal articulation than nasals, thus implies a difference in active
articulator between retroflex nasals and stops. However, Butcher does not give any
phonetic evidence for his claim.
Only a few articulatory studies, such as Balasubramanian (1982) and Dart &
Nihalani (1999), are concerned with nasal retroflexes. Dart & Nihalani (1999)
investigated the coronal nasals and stops of Malayalam. For all nine speakers of
their study the nasal retroflex was articulated at the same place and with the same
articulator as the stop, namely subapical for six speakers, apical for three, with a
post-alveolar place for all except one, who had an alveolar place. Balasubramanian
5
6
This observation holds only for released retroflexes. Unreleased retroflex segments such as e.g. [] do
not show any flapping out. Unreleased [] occur for example before other coronal consonants, before
retroflexes with a different manner of articulation, or phrase-finally in some languages.
See e.g. Boersma (1998: 162f.) on how specific articulatory gestures are used for more than one
manner class and how this can account for symmetries within segment inventories.
Articulatory variation and common properties
21
(1982b) looked at retroflex nasal singletons and geminates in Tamil of one speaker,
who produced a subapical post-alveolar for the singleton and a subapical palatal for
the geminate. As these studies were concerned with retroflex nasals in Dravidian
languages, they give descriptions of subapical post-alveolar articulations only. But
assuming that the nasal is articulated in the same way as the corresponding stop in
the respective languages, there should be a more apical retroflex nasal e.g. in IndoAryan languages. As we do not have any further data to refute the claim, we thus
assume that nasals can vary articulatorily in the same way as retroflex stops do.
2.2.4.3 Fricatives
The articulatory requirements for fricatives are different from those for stops and
nasals. The front coronal fricative [s], for example, seems to be cross-linguistically
more laminal than the corresponding stop [t] (see for instance Dart 1991 on French
and English, Lindblad & Lundqvist 1999 on Swedish, and Wängler 1958 on
German). This is due to the fact that strident coronal fricatives require a grooving of
the tongue blade and an air-jet that strikes the front incisors (Lindblad & Lundqvist
1999), which can be easier achieved with a longer place of constriction. Thus one
would expect retroflex fricatives to differ inherently from the articulatory
configuration found in retroflex stops or nasals, as the contact area of retroflex stops
is rather small. Furthermore, one can speculate that retroflex fricatives require the
retroflexion gesture to be held throughout the whole segment in order to maintain
the air turbulence, whereas stops allow an early flapping out (recall the description
in 2.2.4.1). Keating (1991: 35) confirms these expectations of a different articulatory
position for fricatives by pointing out that retroflex fricatives of India do not seem to
involve the same kind of curling of the tongue as the plosives in these languages, but
are articulated at the same place as the plosives.
Let us look at some realizations of retroflex fricatives in order to test the
assumptions made up to now. The left hand side of figure 2.5 on the next page is a
sagittal x-ray tracing of the Tamil retroflex fricative (Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996:
156). In this picture, the tongue tip is not distinguishable from the tongue body;
therefore no raising or bending backwards of it is discernible. Compared to the
retroflex stop in the same language (refer back to figure 2.4), the fricative differs
very much in its articulation and shows a longer, narrower channel of articulation.7
Thus, the retroflex fricative type found in Tamil is in accordance with the
assumptions made above.
According to the detailed phonetic descriptions of Sridhar (1990), a similar
difference between the retroflex plosive and the retroflex fricative can be found in
Kannada, another Dravidian language. Toda, though from the same language family
(Dravidian) as Tamil and Kannada, and having the same type of retroflex stop,
shows a different articulation of the retroflex fricative, depicted in figure 2.5 on the
right (based on Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996: 160).
7
The x-ray tracing of the Tamil fricative in figure 2.5 shows no discernible tongue tip, which seems to
be retracted into the tongue.
22
Figure 2.5
Chapter 2
Tamil retroflex fricative [] (left) and a Toda retroflex fricative [] (right).
The Toda retroflex fricative involves a raising of the tongue tip towards the palatal
region, and its position of the tongue blade against the post-alveolar region
resembles the articulation of the retroflex stop in Indo-Aryan languages. The
articulation of the Toda fricative, which is further backwards than that of the Tamil
fricative, still does not involve the extreme curling backwards of the tongue tip
found in Dravidian retroflex stops. Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996: 156) introduce
again two different symbols for the two retroflex fricative articulations. Whereas the
Tamil-type of fricative is transcribed with an alveolar symbol with a subscript dot
[s] and is called a ‘flat retroflex’ (defined as not being domed like palato-alveolars),
·
the fricative found in Toda is transcribed with the traditional IPA symbol for
retroflex fricatives, namely [].
A retroflex fricative with an extreme curling backwards of the tongue tip,
comparable to the Tamil stop in figure 4.1 on the right, could not be found in any
articulatory study. Laver (1994: 252) mentions a subapical palato-alveolar fricative
as a possible retroflex articulation but does not refer to any language that employs a
segment like this. From this we can conclude that retroflex fricatives do not involve
the same backwards bending of the tongue tip as retroflex stops, which probably has
to do with the different articulatory requirements on fricatives elaborated above.
Besides the generally accepted two types of retroflex articulation exemplified
up to now, another type of retroflex fricative exists. This type does not necessarily
involve the tongue tip in its articulation and thus shows no kind of bending
backwards of the tip at all. Its place of constriction is the post-alveolar region (recall
the definition in section 2.1), but it differs from the traditional laminal post-alveolar
[ ] and from the retroflex fricative in figure 2.5 left in the shape of its tongue body,
which is flatter. Fricatives like these can be found in Mandarin Chinese, see figure
2.6 (based on Ladefoged & Wu 1984: 269).
Figure 2.6
X-ray tracing of the Mandarin voiceless retroflex fricative (based on Ladefoged
& Wu 1984: 269).
Articulatory variation and common properties
23
In the traditional phonetic literature, the Mandarin sounds of voiced and voiceless
post-alveolars are referred to as ‘retroflex’ (e.g. Chao 1948, 1968). Phonological
descriptions agree with this terminology, cf. Chao (1986), Pulleyblank (1989), and
Lin (1989). Laver (1994: 252), however, describes ‘Standard Chinese’ as having a
laminal ‘flat’ post-alveolar fricative, ‘flat’ as this sound does not show any grooving
of the tongue blade otherwise typical for coronal (including retroflex) sibilants, and
he introduces the symbol [ +] for this segment. Ladefoged & Wu (1984: 277) point
out that the Mandarin retroflex is very different from the same segment class in
Tamil. The study by Lee (1999) with palatograms and linguograms shows that these
sounds are apical post-alveolars for the two speakers tested. These findings lead Lee
to refrain from referring to this segmental class as ‘retroflex’, since it shows no
bending backwards of the tongue tip. The present study follows traditional phonetic
literature and Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996) in classifying these sounds as
retroflex.
Some Slavic languages, such as Polish and Russian, are said to have similar
fricatives, though in the traditional Slavic literature they are never described as
‘retroflex’ (e.g. Wierzchowska 1980 and Rubach 1984). Articulatory arguments for
why these sounds are classified as retroflex in the present study are discussed in
detail in 2.4, phonological reasons are given in chapter 4.
2.2.4.4 Affricates
In the following discussion of retroflex affricates, four types of affricates are
distinguished: first, a segment that is traditionally described as affricate, i.e. a stop
that is released into a homorganic fricative, second, a stop that is released into a
rhotic, thirdly, a laterally released stop, and lastly, a nasally released retroflex stop.
The retroflex affricate []8 can be found e.g. in Burushaski (Edelman 1983),
Gujarati (Pandit 1954), Karok (Bright 1957), and Mandarin (Ladefoged & Wu
1984), which all also have a retroflex fricative.9 As can be seen in many articulatory
descriptions of specific languages (e.g. Ladefoged 1994 on Toda and Ohala 1994 on
Hindi), affricates differ in place of articulation from the corresponding stops, in as
far as they are usually more retracted, and are close to the series of corresponding
fricatives.10 The retroflex affricates in Polish and other languages are therefore
assumed to be identical in place and degree of articulation to the retroflex fricatives
described in 2.2.4.3 and to show similar variation.
The second type of affricate to be treated here are the rhotically released stops,
which can be represented with the symbols [] or []. They occur in Athapaskan
8
9
10
This segmental class is also transcribed as [t]. The exact place of articulation of the plosive part is not
further investigated in any study of my knowledge, therefore no decision on the correct transcription
can be made here. In the present dissertation, the stop is assumed to be homorganic with the fricative
part.
From the seven languages with a retroflex affricate listed in Maddieson (1986), Ostyak and Jaqaru do
not have a retroflex fricative in their inventory. Furthermore, the Micronesian language Ponapean has
an affricate and a flap as its only retroflex segments (Rehg 1973). From this follows that a retroflex
affricate does not imply the presence of a retroflex fricative in the same language.
Kehrein (2002: 7) observes that strident affricates (as opposed to laterally or nasally released
affricates) form phonologically a class with the corresponding fricatives.
24
Chapter 2
languages such as Minto-Nenana (Tuttle 1998) and Upper Kuskokwim (Krauss
1973: 906), and in the Australian language Anguthimri (Crowley 1981: 152).
Rhotically-released stops are usually not included in phonetic descriptions of
possible affricate articulations. In traditional Athapaskan literature (e.g. Kraus
1979), these segments are however referred to as affricates because they show the
same three-way phonation contrast of plain, aspirated and glottalized phonemes as
other Athapaskan affricates such as e.g. [ts]. This classification seems worth
following.
According to Crowley (1981: 152), the Anguthimri affricates, which he
symbolizes as [tr, dr], are post-alveolar stops articulated slightly behind the alveolar
ridge and followed by a trill. Phonotactically, they behave like one segment.
Crowley notes that these sounds are not the same as corresponding retroflexes in
other Australian languages. As this is the only study known to me that describes the
realization of rhotically-released retroflex stops, the exact articulation and possible
variation of this class cannot be further discussed here.
The third type of affricate is the laterally released stop. The inclusion of this
type seems necessary since Ball & Rahilly (1999: 67f.) mention that laterally
released retroflex stops [, ] are possible to produce. Unfortunately, they do not
provide example languages. Since the lateral is phonetically presumably more
affricated than a lateral occurring in vowel context (due to the larger constriction in
place of articulation), I claim that the laterally released stops described by Ball &
Rahilly are actually lateral affricates such as the alveolar [t] or [d] found e.g. in
the Na-Dené languages Navajo and Tlingit (Maddieson 1984). Evidence for the nondistinctiveness of laterally released stops and lateral affricates is taken from the fact
that no phonetic description makes this distinction,11 and furthermore no language
seems to employ the two types distinctively. Masica (1991: 105) mentions that in the
northwest West Pahari dialects Bhadrawahi and Bhalesi (Indo-Aryan languages) a
‘peculiar set of laterally-released apical stops’ exists, but transcribes these sounds as
lateral fricatives, which is further evidence for the claim made here that there is no
difference between laterally released stops and lateral affricates. The segments that
occur in these dialects are [, , , ].12 According to O’Grady, Voeglin, &
Voeglin (1966) the Pama-Nyungan language Adynyamathanha (also known as
Wailpi), spoken in South Australia, also has lateral retroflex fricatives.
Lastly, the class of affricates sometimes includes stops with a nasal release (as
in Kehrein’s 2002 classification). However, I could not find any language described
as having a retroflex nasal affricate.
2.2.4.5 Laterals
As Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996: 183) point out, the place of articulation for
laterals is usually the same as the place for the corresponding stop in the same
11
12
Even Ball & Rahilly (1999), who introduce the class of laterally released stops, do not explicitly
distinguish it from lateral affricates and fail to point out the differences between them.
I transcribed the segments with the corresponding IPA symbols according to Masica’s (1991)
descriptions. Kehrein (2002: 6) employs the symbols [] and [ ] with a raising sign underneath the
retroflex laterals for these speech sounds.
Articulatory variation and common properties
25
language, though the tongue shapes behind the constriction might differ. This
suggests that retroflex laterals show the same articulatory characteristics and
variation as the stops. Svarny & Zvelebil’s (1955) x-ray study for Tamil and Telugu
confirms this hypothesis, as it shows that both stop and lateral retroflex are subapical
post-alveolar in these languages. Dixon (1980: 143) gives further evidence from
Australian languages. According to him, the Australian retroflex lateral is a
subapical post-alveolar just like the retroflex stop. Therefore, retroflex laterals are
expected to show the same kind of variation as retroflex stops.
In Indo-Aryan languages we would expect to find an apical post-alveolar
lateral. No articulatory study on Indo-Aryan laterals could be found to confirm this.
2.2.4.6 Rhotics
Post-alveolar rhotics articulated with a retroflex tongue shape are possible with three
manners. These are the post-alveolar flap (in the IPA symbol chart described as a
flap or tap), the post-alveolar central approximant, and the post-alveolar trill.
The retroflex flap [] could have been included in the class of stops, as it is
often described as a flapped stop (e.g. by Laver 1994: 221) because the active
articulator hits the passive one in passing. Phonologically, there is evidence for
sharing a class with both stops and rhotics: Masica (1991: 97) describes that in many
Indo-Aryan languages such as Panjabi, Hindi, and Sindhi the retroflex flap [] is in
complementary distribution with the voiced retroflex stop []. In Australian Warlpiri
and Maung or African Gbaya and Shona, these segments pattern with rhotics. The
retroflex flap is not articulated further back than alveolar. This is due to the tongue
movement taking place during its articulation; the tongue tip is curled inwards and
approaching the post-alveolar region but flaps out before the actual contact takes
place. See figure 2.7 of two production stages of the flap based on Laver (1994:
223).
Figure 2.7
Two stages in the production of a retroflex flap: the retroflex onset (left), and
the sliding from post-alveolar to alveolar during closure and release (right) both
based on Laver (1994: 223).
Due to this large movement towards the alveolar ridge during the articulation, the
flap [] is inherently less retroflex than e.g. a retroflex stop.
The retroflex trill is articulated with the tongue underside vibrating against the
post-alveolar region. The IPA chart does not provide any symbol for this sound;
Laver (1994: 220) represents it with an underlined apical trill [r], whereas traditional
Dravidianists such as Emeneau (1984) use the symbol for an apical trill with a
26
Chapter 2
subscript dot [r]. Spajic et al. (1996) explain that the first contact for a retroflex trill
·
takes place further back than the subsequent contacts, i.e. there is a flapping out
gesture observable during the articulation of the trill.
Some speakers of Malayalam have a retroflex trill (recall its dependence on the
speech rate illustrated in 2.2.2). Ladefoged, Cochran & Disner (1977) describe this
sound as ‘almost retroflex’, and one can conclude from this that the place of
articulation for this segment is not as far back as for other retroflexes and that it is
rather the tongue tip than the tongue underside that is involved in the articulation.
The Toda retroflex trill investigated in Spajic et al. (1996), however, is a subapical
post-alveolar. This indicates language-specific variation within the class of retroflex
trills, comparable to the one observed for stops in 2.2.4.1.
The third type of retroflex rhotic is the approximant []. This segment occurs
e.g. in Australian languages where, according to Dixon (1980), it is articulated
almost as far back as the retroflex stop. McDonough & Johnson (1997) demonstrate
that the Tamil retroflex approximant shows a constriction at the hard palate.
Contrary to the other two types of retroflex rhotics, the approximant does not show
any evidence of a forward motion during the consonant closure.
Australian languages usually employ four coronal stops, nasals, and laterals,
but only two rhotics which are both apical. Dixon (1980: 144) describes these
rhotics as apical trill and subapical post-alveolar continuant. The latter can also be
realized as a post-alveolar trill, typically at the end of a stressed syllable. The
distinction between rhotics in Australian languages therefore seems to be not in the
manner but in the place of articulation, namely apical alveolar versus retroflex. The
same variation in manner but consistency in place can be observed in Hausa rhotics,
which are apical trill versus retroflex flap or approximant (Newman 1980).
In sum, retroflex rhotics seem to be prone to a large degree of variation, not
only in the place of articulation and active articulator, but also in the actual manner
of articulation.
2.2.4.7 Retroflex vowels
Vowels can show some kind of secondary articulation as ‘apico-post-alveolarized,
advanced velar approximants’ (Catford 1977: 150), a variant that is often referred to
as ‘retroflex vowels’. Catford (1977: 192) points out that these vowels involve a
raising and sometimes drawing backwards of the tongue tip, which results in apicopost-alveolarized or sublamino-prepalatalized articulations. American English is
reported to have retroflexed vowels such as [ ] in board. Further instances can be
found in the Uto-Aztecan language Serrano and the Mesoamerican Tarascan (Bhat
1973: 38). Vowel retroflexion typically occurs pre-rhotically as e.g. in British and
American English (Wakelin 1972), see section 4.4 below. These vowels are also
called ‘r-coloured’ or ‘rhotacized’ (e.g. by Wells 1982: 139 and Laver 1994: 270).
An illustration of such an articulation is given in figure 2.8.
The tongue configuration for a retroflex vowel is nearly identical to the first
stage of the retroflex flap, see the left side of figure 2.7, which is also based on
Laver.
Articulatory variation and common properties
Figure 2.8
27
Retroflex vowel, based on an x-ray tracing by Laver (1994: 271).
Catford (1988: 161f.) distinguishes between retroflexed and rhotacized
vowels, the former affecting only open vowels, and the latter referring to the sound
[‘] as in the American English word bird. Rhotacized vowels are, according to
Catford, articulated with a redrawn tongue tip or with a bunched, retracted tongue
body. The latter do not show any retroflex articulation and are therefore not topic of
the present dissertation. Trask (1996: 310) unites both articulations, referring to both
as retroflexed or ‘r-coloured’, and defines them as having the distinct acoustic
quality of a lowered third formant.
The present dissertation is primarily concerned with retroflex consonants.
Where retroflex vowels are discussed, they are assumed to be articulated as in figure
2.8.
2.2.5 Language family
The association of degree of how far the tongue tip is bent backwards with a specific
language family has been made repeatedly in phonetic and phonological literature.
Catford (1977: 153) e.g. distinguishes two types of tongue tip bending (retroflexion)
and correlates them with the two language families found on the Indian
subcontinent, namely Indo-Aryan and Dravidian. He states that the retroflex stops in
Indo-Aryan languages are articulated with the underside of the tongue (subapical
area) against the back of the alveolar ridge, an articulation he terms ‘sublaminopost-alveolar’. Retroflex segments in Tamil and other Dravidian languages however
are articulated with the underside against the prepalatal area, according to Catford,
and termed by him ‘sublamino-prepalatal’. He calls this type of gesture ‘the most
retroflex of retroflex articulation’. The distinction between moderately retroflexed
segments in Indo-Aryan languages and extremely retroflexed segments in Dravidian
language is supported by phonetic studies by e.g. Sv arny & Zvelebil (1955),
Balasubramanian (1972), and Ladefoged & Bhaskararao (1983). Svarny & Zvelebil
compare X-rays of retroflex stops of Tamil, Telugu (both Dravidian) and Hindi
(Indo-Aryan). They used one speaker of each language, and the results show that the
Tamil speaker has the most retroflexed stop with an apical-prepalatal articulation.
Ladefoged & Bhaskararao (1983) evaluate the articulation of retroflex stops of
several Hindi and Telugu speakers via x-rays and found a systematic difference. The
Telugu stops are all considerably retroflexed, with a subapical articulator and
prepalatal place of articulation, whereas the Hindi stops are mostly apical and post-
28
Chapter 2
alveolar. Thus, the difference in the degree of tongue tip bending in languages
spoken on the Indian subcontinent can be correlated with the distinction of the two
language families.
Ladefoged (1964) examines sounds of the West-African language Ewe and the
neighbouring Central Togo languages Logba, Siwu, and G! palatographically. He
notes that the retroflex voiced stops occurring in these languages are less retroflexed
than those of many Indian languages such as Hindi. The exact articulatory
description, however, shows that the retroflex in Ewe has “the tip of the tongue
against the alveolar ridge (usually the posterior part)” (Ladefoged 1964: 20).
Ladefoged’s generalization seems to indicate that West-African languages show a
common characteristic of apical post-alveolar articulation in the retroflex voiced
stops.
The two North-Germanic languages Norwegian and Swedish that employ
retroflexion both show apical articulation and a place of constriction that is rarely
further back than the post-alveolar region (see Simonsen et al. 2000 on Norwegian,
and Lindblad & Lundqvist 1994, 1997 on Swedish). This could be interpreted as a
trait of North Germanic.13
Retroflex segments in Australian languages do not show one homogenous
articulation. According to Butcher (1992: 14), some languages such as Western
Arrernte (belonging to the Arandic branch of Pama-Nyungan), Yindjibarndi (southwestern branch of the Pama-Nyungan subfamily) and Tiwi (Tiwian subfamily) have
retroflex sounds that are articulated with the tongue tip just behind the alveolar
ridge, whereas others such as Adynyamathanha (south-western branch of PamaNyungan) and Gupapuyngu (Yuulngu branch of Pama-Nyungan) have subapical
retroflex segments. Hence, no common retroflex characteristic for all Australian
languages can be stated. Further investigations into the numerous Australian subfamilies might result in some traits that are shared within subfamilies.
In general, languages belonging to the same language family tend to employ a
similar type of retroflex segment, though this does not seem to hold for all language
families, as exemplified by the Australian languages.
2.2.6 Inventory size
The degree of articulatory variation is often influenced by the inventory size of the
respective languages. Inventory size is partly interrelated with the factor language
family described in 2.2.5, as related languages often show similar segment
inventories.
Both English and French have only one anterior coronal for any manner (apart
from the fricatives) and therefore allow a considerable amount of articulatory
13
Optional retroflex articulation of rhotics is reported for various Germanic languages such as English
(Ball & Rahily 2000), German (Wiese 2001) (for the area North-East of Dresden) or Dutch
(Verstraeten & Van de Velde 2001) (as a variant in the North of the Netherlands). This variation is
ignored here as it concerns only one manner class (which might affect other manners via spreading as
is the case e.g. in some of the rhotic variants of English) and mostly only regional variants of these
languages.
Articulatory variation and common properties
29
variation, see for instance Dart’s (1991) study on alveolars in French and English.
The situation is different for languages with large coronal inventories such as
Australian languages, where the existence of other, articulatorily close classes
prohibits large variation of e.g. the retroflex class. Butcher (1992) investigated
several Australian languages and found that the only apical coronal in the Kalaw
Kawaw Ya dialect of the Western Torres Strait language is often articulated as
retroflex and displays far more variation than either one of the apical segments in
Australian languages with a two-way apical contrast. To examine how far the
inventory size can influence the degree of tongue tip bending, let us look closely at
two different retroflex manners, namely stops and fricatives, and the correlation
between their articulation and the size of the inventory.
Stops. To account for the articulatory difference between Hindi and Tamil retroflex
stops exemplified in figure 2.4 above in terms of language system, there should be a
difference in their coronal inventories. Since it shows less tongue tip bending in its
stop, Hindi is expected to have a smaller coronal inventory than Tamil. And indeed,
Hindi has two coronal plosive series,14 a dental and a retroflex (Ohala 1994), see
(1a), whereas Tamil has four: a dental, an alveolar, a retroflex, and a palato-alveolar
(Christdas 1988), see (1b).
(1)
(a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
Hindi
Tamil
Toda
Kannada
[t, ]
[t, t, , t]
[t, t, ]
[t, t, ]
(or: [t, t])
·
Thus the pressure of many segments in the coronal region seems to cause a more
retracted articulation of the retroflex plosive for Tamil. Toda supports this
hypothesis. Like Tamil, it has a subapical post-alveolar retroflex stop (Shalev,
Ladefoged & Bhaskararao 1993) and two additional coronal stops, a laminal dental
and an apical alveolar, see (1c). The existence of one further apical element in a
language, not necessarily a four-way coronal contrast, seems therefore to cause a
subapical articulation of retroflex stops. Kannada (another Dravidian language) also
has a subapical retroflex stop and the same three-way coronal distinction as Toda,
see (1d), and hence supports this point.
As all the languages characterized by a subapical stop and a large inventory
mentioned up to now are Dravidian, one could argue that these two characteristics
are specific to this language family (see 2.2.5 above) or are areal features that spread
together, but are not correlated. In order to refute this argument, languages from
families other than Dravidian have to be found that behave similarly. Australian
languages with a four-way coronal contrast of apical alveolar, laminal dental,
retroflex, and laminal post-alveolar stops (but also nasals and laterals) seem to be
ideal for this purpose. Eastern Arrernte (Butcher 1995) e.g. has such a four-way
14
‘Series’ is used to denote a voiced and/or voiceless segment in one place of articulation such as dental,
alveolar etc. The segments [c, #, , , $] are assumed to be alveolo-palatal, not palatal (following e.g.
Keating 1991, Hume 1992, Hall 1997a) and thus belong to the coronal sounds.
30
Chapter 2
contrast and a retroflex that is apical palatal, see (2), thus providing evidence for the
systemic pressure of a large inventory on subapical retroflex articulation.
(2)
Eastern Arrernte
[t, t, , t]
Other Australian languages, however, do not support this inventory theory.
Butcher’s (1992) palatographic studies of the retroflex stops in five of these
languages with the same two-way apical contrast as Eastern Arrernte (Warlpiri,
Kunwinjku, Murrinh-Patha, Nyangumarta and Western Desert) indicate that the
retroflex stops vary from apical post-alveolar in Nyangumarta to subapical palatal in
Murrinh-Patha, with the remaining three languages somewhere in-between. The
apical alveolars, however, are articulated in exactly the same way in all five
languages. Thus the larger inventory incurs a noticeable restriction on the variation
allowed within the alveolar class, but not within the class of retroflexes.
One can conclude from this that a large coronal inventory does not necessarily
impose a subapical post-alveolar articulation on the retroflex stop, though there is a
general tendency towards more retracted retroflexes in larger inventories. Small
inventories with a retroflex stop, on the other hand, seem to have retroflex segments
articulated further front and with the tongue tip. Support for this comes from the
West-African language Ewe, which has two coronal (voiced) stops, one laminal
denti-alveolar and an apical post-alveolar retroflex, see (3).
(3)
Ewe
[d, ]
(or: [d, d5] )
Here the relatively small coronal inventory allows a segment that shows almost no
bending backwards of the tongue tip. Similar observations can be made for
Norwegian, which has a laminal dental and an apical (post)alveolar retroflex as the
only coronals (Simonsen et al. 2000), see (4).
(4)
Norwegian [t, ]
(or: [t, t])
·
Fricatives. The correlation of inventory size with degree of tongue tip bending
seems also to work for fricative inventories. The difference in retroflex fricative
between the Tamil laminal post-alveolar and the apical post-alveolar in Toda as
illustrated above in figure 2.5 can be explained by differences in the inventory.
Though both languages are Dravidian, Tamil has three coronal fricatives, see (5a),
whereas Toda is the only language of this family with four coronal fricatives,
namely a laminal alveolar, an apical post-alveolar, a laminal post-alveolar, and a
subapical palatal, see (5b). Hence the subapical palatal in Toda might be due to the
large fricative inventory.
(5)
(a)
(b)
Tamil
Toda
[%, s, ]
[s, s, , ]
(or [%, s, s])
·
(or [s, s, , ])
·
The coronal fricative system of Toda is of further interest for the present study, as its
apical post-alveolar is very similar in place of articulation and active articulator to
the retroflex fricative in Tamil (depicted in figure 2.5 on the left). Based on this
similarity, one could postulate that Toda has two types of retroflex fricatives, a
Articulatory variation and common properties
31
subapical palatal (like the retroflex stop in Dravidian languages) and an apical postalveolar (like the Tamil fricative), as Ladefoged & Maddieson (1986) did. In later
work, Ladefoged (1994), Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996), and Shalev et al. (1993)
disprove this claim, arguing that there are “no two degrees of retroflexion” in Toda
(Ladefoged 1994: 20). As elaborated in the present chapter, degree of tongue tip
bending is variable, and some retroflex sounds do not even show a bending of the
tongue tip at all, recall the Ewe stop. Especially fricatives typically have a lesser
degree of tongue tip bending than plosives or nasals, recall 2.2.4.3. Thus, Ladefoged
et al.’s argumentation that Toda does not have two retroflexes because there are no
two ‘degrees of retroflexion’ is not convincing as no definition of retroflexion is
given by them. According to the phonetic descriptions of retroflex segments made
above, the articulation of retroflexes can range from apical to subapical and from
alveolar to palatal place of articulation. Both the Toda apical post-alveolar [s] and
the subapical palatal [] fall into this range and thus can be classified articulatorily as
retroflex. Chapter three will show that both Toda fricatives also comply with the
acoustic criteria for retroflex. It remains to be discussed whether it is phonologically
necessary or useful to distinguish two retroflex categories, and how they could be
represented. This question will be dealt with in chapters 4 and 5.
As in the retroflex stop systems, the existence of four coronal fricatives in a
language is not the decisive factor for the degree of tongue tip bending. The Bzyb
dialect of Abkhaz, a Northwest Caucasian language, has according to Ladefoged &
Maddieson (1996: 161f.) four coronals, three of them laminal (an alveolar, a postalveolar, and an alveolo-palatal) and one apical, see (6).15
(6)
Abkhaz
[s, , , ]
(or: [s, , s, ])
·
The apical [] is a post-alveolar with slight tongue tip bending only. This might be
due to the fact that there is no second apical and therefore no need for maximizing
an articulatory difference between apical alveolar and subapical post-alveolar.
To complete this section on retroflex fricatives and their correlation with
inventory size, a discussion of the post-alveolar fricatives in the Slavic languages
Polish and Russian has to be included. As indicated already in 2.2.4.3, the Polish
and Russian sounds are articulated with a flat, non-domed tongue middle and with a
raised tongue tip (see also section 2.4 about the extent to which Polish fricatives
conform to retroflex characteristics). With such a variant of retroflex, we would
expect the coronal fricative inventory to be small.
(7)
(a)
(b)
Russian
Polish
[s, , &]
[s, , ]
Both Russian and Polish, however, have a fricative inventory with three segments, a
dental [s ], a retroflex [], and a further post-alveolar fricative. In Russian, the
additional fricative is a laminal post-alveolar, see (7a). This segment is usually
15
Ladefoged & Maddieson use a different symbol for the laminal post-alveolar in Abkhaz because this
sound differs from e.g. English [ ] in that it is articulated with the tongue tip behind the lower teeth,
and does not have a sublingual cavity.
32
Chapter 2
transcribed as [ '&] but referred to as [ &] in this study, because it does not consist of a
post-alveolar plus an additional short glide, i.e., it is not a real secondarily
palatalized post-alveolar.16 Polish has the additional alveolo-palatal [ ], see (7b).
Russian and Polish thus have two laminal coronal fricatives besides the apical
retroflex. The absence of a second apical coronal can explain why these two
languages employ a retroflex that shows no backwards bending of the tongue tip,
because there is no need for a maximal distinction between two apicals.
In sum, it has been shown that the articulatory variation of retroflex stops,
fricatives, and also of other manners, if the findings can be transferred to them, can
be partly accounted for by referring to the inventory system in which the retroflex
segment occurs. Whereas small inventories with only one apical, namely the
retroflex, allow considerable variation of the retroflex and also a place of
constriction further front than the post-alveolar region, large inventories (i.e., those
with two apicals and one or more laminals) generally show less variation and an
articulation with a bent backwards tongue tip. It has to be pointed out that this is
only a tendency and no universal regularity, as illustrated by the counterexamples
above.
2.3 Common articulatory properties of retroflexion
As was shown in the previous section, the exact place of contact and the exact
articulator of retroflex segments are subject to a large amount of variation,
depending on several parameters. Furthermore, it was shown that the traditional
property of retroflexes as a bending backwards of the tongue tip is not universally
valid. Variation in the class of retroflexes seems to be larger than in any other
articulatory class, which makes it difficult to find common properties that hold for
all retroflexes.
The present section is concerned with defining more or less invariant
characteristics that can be used as articulatory defining criteria for a retroflex
articulation. Four such properties are proposed here, namely apicality, sublingual
cavity, posteriority, and retraction. Not all of them occur in the same degree in all
instances of retroflex segments, but they can be viewed as defining characteristics of
a prototype retroflex; the more of these properties a segment has, the more retroflex
it is.17 Interrelations between these properties proposed here are developed in the
following sections and summarized in 2.3.5. Seeming counterexamples of languages
such as Polish, Russian, and Mandarin, which have retroflexes that do not have all
four properties, are discussed in 2.4.
16
17
The Russian laminal post-alveolar is not an alveolo-palatal [ ], either, see figure 2.13 below.
Shalev et al. (1993: 106) point out that retroflex stops have a lower jaw position than the other
coronals. This property is not further developed here.
Articulatory variation and common properties
33
2.3.1 Apicality
As could be seen in the phonetic descriptions above, retroflexes mainly involve the
tongue tip in their articulation, either its upper side (apical articulation) or its lower
side (subapical articulation). Even in cases where no judgement on the involvement
of the tongue tip can be made from the articulatory data, as is the case for some
fricatives (e.g. the Tamil fricative in figure 2.5 left or the Mandarin fricative in
figure 2.6), the tongue tip is not in resting position, i.e. behind the lower teeth, but
raised. Usually, however, the tip of the tongue forms the actual constriction for
retroflexes.
In the present study, the term ‘apicality’ is defined to refer to this involvement
of the tongue tip. Apicality is meant here in a strict phonetic descriptive way. It is
not used as a phonological feature such as e.g. ‘apicality’ in Williamson’s (1977)
feature system or [–distributed] in traditional featural theories (e.g. Chomsky &
Halle 1968 and Sagey 1986), since these feature values do not allow the inclusion of
cases as the Tamil retroflex which does not show an active articulation with the
tongue tip.
Retroflex segments share the characteristic of apicality with front apicals in the
dental/alveolar region. All apical segments have in common that they consist of an
extremely rapid gesture, as the tongue tip is the most flexible and quickest active
articulator. In fast speech, this quick gesture is often reduced or lost, which explains
the decrease of tongue tip bending with increased speed of speech described in 2.2.2.
Retroflexes differ from front apicals in the place of constriction, which is defined as
the second characteristic criterion, ‘posteriority’.
2.3.2 Posteriority
Retroflexion in the traditional sense refers to an articulation behind the alveolar
region, usually described as ‘post-alveolar’ or sometimes ‘palatal’ (e.g. Catford
1977). This is termed ‘posteriority’ below, and is employed as one of the
articulatory characteristics of retroflexion. Besides retroflexes, the segmental classes
of palato-alveolars and alveolo-palatals are also posterior. Posteriority thus
corresponds to [CORONAL, –anterior] in Feature Geometric notation (Sagey 1986),
i.e., only segments articulated with the tongue tip or blade can be posterior. In order
to distinguish the three posterior classes retroflex, palato-alveolar, and alveolopalatal, further characteristics have to be used.
The two non-retroflex posterior segment classes, palato-alveolars and alveolopalatals, are both laminal, thus differ from retroflexes in the characteristic apicality.
Furthermore, they occur only with a fricative manner, whereas retroflexes can occur
with all possible manners of articulation (see description in 2.2.4). A third point
distinguishing these two classes from retroflexes is the shape of the tongue middle
during their articulation: palato-alveolars and alveolo-palatals have a domed, i.e.
raised, tongue middle; retroflexes a flat one (which is defined as belonging to the
fourth property, ‘retraction’, see section 2.3.4).
34
Chapter 2
Section 2.4 will discuss some retroflex sounds that lack the characteristic of
posteriority, e.g. the Polish fricative. These exceptions only involve fricative
manners of articulation.
Retroflexes typically involve a movement of the tongue tip from a posterior to
a more anterior position during their articulation, a feature referred to above as
“flapping out”. This flapping out could be treated as another, separate property of
retroflexion. The present study does not adopt this view because flapping out, in
contrast to the other characteristics introduced here, does not occur in the same way
for all manners of retroflex articulation. Fricatives, for example, allow flapping out
only at the onset of a following segment but not during the fricative constriction.
Introducing this gesture as a separate property would thus imply that specific
manners like fricatives are inherently less retroflex than e.g. stops or flaps, an
implication avoided here. The flapping out gesture occurs almost exclusively with
an apical articulation on a posterior place, and hence is assumed a possible
concomitant of the properties ‘posteriority’ and ‘apicality’.
2.3.3 Sublingual cavity
A unifying criterion for retroflex segments seems to be their sublingual cavity,
visible in all the x-ray tracings of the retroflexes given above. All sounds articulated
with the tongue tip or blade on or behind the alveolar ridge evince a cavity beneath
the tongue, due to the backwards displacement of the tongue front (Sundberg &
Lindblom 1990: 1316). Keating (1991: 43) points out that this cavity increases in
volume from palatal, alveolo-palatal (“hissing-hushing” in her terms), palatoalveolar, apical retroflex, to the sublaminal retroflex. The property of sublingual
cavity is thus not unique to retroflex segments, but judging from Keating’s
description it is largest for any kind of retroflex articulation. All segment classes
with a sublingual cavity share also the property of ‘posteriority’, according to
Keating’s definition above. They differ, however, in the criterion apicality, which is
unique to retroflexes.
‘Sublingual cavity’ differs from traditionally employed articulatory
descriptions of segment classes, which are usually restricted to the place of
articulation and the active articulator.
Vowel context seems to affect the size of the front cavity systematically. As
Sundberg & Lindblom (1990: 1315) point out, “everything else being equal, that
cavity tends to be larger for [retroflex] tokens surrounded by /u/, intermediate for /a/,
and smallest for samples with /i/.” The dependence of the sublingual cavity on
context proves the aforementioned impossibility to find a property that is invariant
for all retroflexes in all contexts.
2.3.4 Retraction
The property ‘retraction’ to be discussed in this section is not as obvious a
characteristic of retroflexion as those proposed up to now and is the most
controversial of all. Bhat (1974a) writes that retroflexion cannot be equated with
Articulatory variation and common properties
35
retraction (defined by him as backing of the tongue body), because retraction does
not occur exclusively with retroflexion. Furthermore, some retroflexes are said to
occur without retraction, namely Lardil retroflex consonants and Badaga retroflex
vowels.18 It is argued here that all retroflexes - but also other sounds - show
retraction.
In the phonetic literature, retraction is usually defined by the place in the vocal
tract where the tongue retracts to, and distinguished into pharyngealization or
velarization. The term pharyngealization is mostly used for a secondary vowel
articulation (e.g. by Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996: 365) where the root of the
tongue is drawn back towards the back wall of the pharynx. Velarization is
understood as a secondary articulation where the tongue dorsum is raised towards
the velum (e.g. definition by Trask 1996: 374). Brosnahan & Malmberg (1970: 67)
define velarization as ‘the elevation of the back of the tongue toward the soft palate
or rear wall of the pharynx’, which actually covers both velarization and
pharyngealization as defined previously. Ladefoged (1971) points out that there is
little difference between velarized and pharyngealized sounds19 and says that no
language distinguishes between these two. He goes on to say that
[I]t is interesting to note that there is some similarity in quality between retroflex stops
and velarized or pharyngealized stops. This is due to the fact that in all these sounds the
front of the tongue is somewhat hollowed (Ladefoged 1971: 208)
The property ‘retraction’ introduced here is thus defined as a displacement of the
tongue back towards the pharynx or velum. A schema of these possible
displacements is given in figure 2.9, based on x-ray tracings of velarized and
pharyngealized segments in Laver (1994: 326ff.) and Ladefoged & Maddieson
(1996: 365).
Figure 2.9
Retraction, i. e. velarization (upper movement) and pharyngealization (lower
movement) in comparison to neutral tongue position
These displacements of the tongue towards the velum or the pharynx are sometimes
referred to as ‘tongue backing’, as by Bhat (1974a) and Stevens (1998).
18
19
For a detailed discussion of Bhat’s claims the reader is referred to Hamann (2002a).
An interesting language-specific case of the similarity between pharyngealization and velarization is
Russian. Whereas Russian consonants are traditionally described in terms of the opposition palatalized
vs. velarized, Bolla (1981: 70) describes the latter as ‘pharyngealized’ because he ‘found the
movement of the root of the tongue and the postdorsum towards the pharyngeal wall to be more
important than that towards the soft palate’.
36
Chapter 2
It is difficult to see retraction in x-ray tracings of retroflex segments clearly, as
most of these pictures do not even show the pharyngeal area and the tongue position
therein. Furthermore, often no comparison to the normal, non-retracted tongue
position can be made, as x-ray studies usually do not include a figure of the
articulators at rest.
The legitimacy of combining the distinct secondary articulations of
pharyngealization and velarization as a single property is further confirmed by the
fact that they are described as resulting in the same acoustic effects, see section
3.2.4.
The property ‘retraction’ is not identical to the feature ‘retracted tongue root’,
henceforth RTR (opposed to ‘advanced tongue root’, see Halle & Stevens 1969),
because this articulatory setting involves a pharyngeal constriction at a lower level
than for pharyngealization, according to Laver (1994: 411). Furthermore, RTR is
usually only used for vowel articulations (see Ladefoged’s 1964 description of Igbo
vowels). Retraction as defined here is also different from McCawley’s (1966)
feature ‘retracted articulation’ by which he distinguishes dentals from alveolars and
retroflexes from palatals (amongst others), the second item in each pair being
[+retracted] (and thus ascribing retroflex a non-retracted status). The property
‘retraction’ is best captured by the traditional phonological feature [+back] (e.g.
Clements 1991, Hall 1997a, Lin 1989, Rubach 1984).
Cooccurrence of retroflexion with retraction can be articulatorily explained.
The tongue, in order to be able to move its tip upwards and into a displaced position
in the post-alveolar region, stretches and pulls backwards (Spencer 1984: 30), which
results in a lowered tongue middle and retracted tongue back. Tongue lowering is
thus a concomitant of the property ‘retraction’, or the reverse.
‘Lowering’ describes the flat tongue middle that is found with retroflex
articulation. In many phonetic descriptions the lowering of the tongue middle in
retroflexes is described as a concaving of the dorsum (Brosnahan & Malmberg
1970: 46) or a less convex shape of the tongue than in lamino-post-alveolar
articulations (Catford 1977: 157). The shape of the tongue middle is actually one of
the main differences between retroflexes and palato-alveolars or alveolo-palatals:
the latter two have a raised tongue middle, sometimes called “domed” (e.g.
Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996: 148), in contrast to the lowered one of retroflex
segments.
The retraction of the tongue back and the lowering of the tongue middle that
occurs with retroflex articulation is given schematically in figure 2.10, where the
grey underlying figure indicates the tongue position at rest.
Bhat (1974a) argues that retraction is not restricted to retroflexes but mostly
occurs with apicals in general. Stevens, Keyser & Kawasaki (1986: 436), however,
claim that a fronted tongue body provides a more favourable posture for an apicoalveolar articulation, i.e. that a non-retroflex apical usually occurs with fronted
tongue body.
Articulatory variation and common properties
37
Figure 2.10 A schematic illustration of tongue gestures co-occurring with tongue tip
retraction. Underlyingly grey are the tongue parts at rest, from left to right: tip,
blade, middle (or pre-dorsum), and back (or post-dorsum) as defined in section
2.1.
From comparing the realization of apico-alveolars in different languages we know
that this class can be non-retracted. For example, British English, Dutch, and
Catalan have a non-velarized lateral in the onset and a velarized lateral in coda
position.20 Thus retraction cannot be assumed to occur exclusively with retroflexes.
Nevertheless, as retraction is not used as the single defining criterion for retroflexion
but along with the other three properties developed above this does not cause any
definition problems.
Further evidence in the phonetic literature for the correlation of retroflexion
and retraction besides Bhat (1974a) is Catford (1977: 157) who writes that the
tongue body for retroflex segments shows some velarization. Language-specific
descriptions of a correlation between retroflexion and retraction are, for example,
Hamilton (1980: 21), who uses the terms velarization and retroflexion
interchangeably for the post-alveolar fricative in Polish, and Wood (1996), who
writes that the Bulgarian retroflexed /r/ involves a pharyngeal tongue body gesture.
In Ponapean, a Micronesian language spoken on the Pohnpei Island, the velarized
counterpart of the dental t is a retroflex affricate (Rehg 1973), giving further
evidence for the inherent retraction of retroflexes (see section 4.4.1 below).
Using ‘retraction’ as a defining characteristic of retroflexion poses problems
with languages that are said to have a distinctively non-retracted (i.e. non-velarized
or non-pharyngealized) retroflex segment. The Australian language Lardil is the
only language known to me which is supposed to have a phonetically and
phonologically non-velarized retroflex fricative (Hall 1997a, 2000a, and Wilkinson
1988, both based on Stevens et al. 1986). The phonetic evidence for this claim
comes from Hall (1997a: 49) who gives midsagittal tongue tracings of non-velarized
and velarized retroflexes from the languages Lardil and Polish, respectively, to
20
Retraction of the tongue body (towards the velum) also seems to occur distinctively in the articulation
of velar consonants, though for velars this is the primary constriction in the vocal tract, not an
additional secondary one.
38
Chapter 2
illustrate the difference. The source for the figure of the non-velarized retroflexes in
Lardil is given as Stevens et al. (1986). Stevens et al., however, do not provide any
graphic illustrations of the Lardil coronal. The only figure that might have served as
the basis of Hall’s figure is a schematized retroflex stop in figure 20.4 on page 433,
based on Ladefoged & Bhaskararao (1983) and Wierzchowska (1965). These
sources, though, investigated retroflexes in Hindi, Tamil, and Polish. The schematic
retroflex sound in Stevens et al. shows a distinct backing of the tongue body and is
explicitly described as ‘more backed’ than an apical dental or a laminal post-alveolar
in the text. In their phonetic description of Lardil sounds at a later point, Stevens et
al. (p. 444f.) observe that [+distributed, − anterior] segments, i.e. laminal postalveolars, are [−back] in this language. Hall (1997a) presumably misapplied this
correlation to [−distributed, −anterior], i.e. retroflex, segments and concluded that
Lardil is a language with [−back], i.e. non-velarized, retroflexes. A discussion of the
phonological inadequacy of the distinction between velarized and non-velarized
retroflexes in Lardil can be found in Hamann (2002a: 17ff.).
Though the primary concern of the present study is retroflex consonants, let us
shortly look at the vowel system of the Dravidian language Badaga (spoken in the
Indian state of Tamil Nadu), which is another apparent counterexample to the claim
that velarization always co-occurs with retraction. According to Bhat (1974a: 234),
Badaga contrasts plain, retroflex, and retracted vowels. Using retraction and
retroflexion contrastively implies that they do not co-occur together in this language.
Bhat bases his description of Badaga vowels on Emeneau (1939), who, however,
describes the three-way contrast as one of ‘non-retroflexed, half-retroflexed, and
fully-retroflexed vowels’. Bhat does not motivate his reanalysis of these vowels, so
we follow Emeneau’s description, which poses no counterevidence for the
assumption that the characteristic ‘retraction’ holds for retroflex vowels, too.
Summing up, no language seems to exist with a non-retracted retroflex.
Besides the claim that non-velarized or non-pharyngealized retroflexes do not
occur, the retroflex property ‘retraction’ introduced here has a further implication. If
retroflex segments are inherently retracted, they should not be compatible with
secondary palatalization, because a simultaneous articulation of palatalization and
velarization or pharyngealization is articulatory impossible. The palatalization of
segments involves a raising of the middle of the dorsum and a lowering of the
tongue back, whereas retraction has the opposite articulatory consequences of
flattening the middle of the dorsum and raising the back. Both gestures cannot cooccur, as the non-existence of a segment with secondary palatalization and
velarization or pharyngealization in the languages of the world attests. Nevertheless,
one finds references in the literature to palatalized retroflexes in languages such as
Toda (Shalev et al. 1993; Spajic, Ladefoged & Bhaskararao 1996) and Kashmiri
(Bhat 1987). I will argue that these segments are not retroflex (namely that
palatalization of retroflexes triggers a change from a retroflex to a palato-alveolar
articulation). This point will be developed in detail in section 2.5 below.
Articulatory variation and common properties
39
2.3.5 Summary of characteristic properties
In sections 2.3.1 to 2.3.4 above, four articulatory characteristics for retroflex
segments were described, namely apicality, posteriority, sublingual cavity, and
retraction. Looking at them separately, none of them is totally new for defining
retroflexion, as they have been mentioned in connection with retroflexion in the
phonetic and phonological literature before, though not necessarily with these terms
and with a narrower definition in the case of retraction and apicality. Using the four
of them together as defining criteria for retroflexion, however, is a novel approach.
The bending backwards of the tongue tip and the flapping out of the retroflex
articulation have not been introduced as separate properties for the following
reasons. As has been shown in 2.2, the tongue tip often fails to bend backwards in a
retroflex, and hence this property would be violated by a large number of segments
traditionally considered retroflex. Furthermore, if the two properties apicality and
posteriority are met, this includes a possible bending backwards of the tongue tip, as
the property apicality includes subapical, but it does not necessarily require it. Thus,
the bending backwards of the tongue tip does not have to be stated as a separate
property. Concerning the flapping out, it has been shown in 2.2 that not all retroflex
manners include this gesture.
As has been indicated in their definitions, the proposed four properties are
interrelated, i.e. some of them imply the presence of others. Posteriority implies the
presence of a sublingual cavity, which means that a posterior articulation always cooccurs with a sublingual cavity. This entailment is given in (8a). Furthermore, the
combination of some pairs of properties automatically entails the presence of the
remaining two: apicality and posteriority imply a retracted articulation with
sublingual cavity, see (8b), and posteriority and retraction imply apicality and a
sublingual cavity, see (8c). Finally, two properties imply the presence of a third, thus
a sublingual cavity plus retraction implies an apical articulation, see (8d), and
apicality plus sublingual cavity implies retraction, see (8e).
(8)
(a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
(e)
posteriority
apicality & posteriority
posteriority & retraction
subling. cavity & retraction
apicality & sublingual cavity
→
→
→
→
→
sublingual cavity
retraction, sublingual cavity
apicality, sublingual cavity
apicality
retraction
The criterion ‘retraction’ makes some implications on the articulatory restrictions for
retroflex segments that have not been proposed before. It predicts the absence of
non-retracted (i.e., non-velarized or non-pharyngealized) retroflexes in languages of
the world, which could be attested by showing that Lardil, the only known
counterexample, does not have such a segment, either. A second implication is that
secondary palatalization of retroflexion is impossible, because the result would have
to be apical and laminal at the same time. This point will be further developed in 2.5
below.
40
Chapter 2
2.4 Retroflex fricatives in Slavic languages
Having established four properties for retroflexion, this section applies these
properties to two languages of a single family, namely Slavic, as a contribution to
the analysis of retroflex consonants in this group. The post-alveolar fricatives21 in
the two Slavic languages Polish and Russian will be shown to be retroflex. Though
traditionally described as palato-alveolar [ ], it has been argued by e.g. Keating
(1991) and Hall (1997a) that the Polish fricative should be considered phonetically
and phonologically to be retroflex. Keating (1991) argues similarly in favour of the
retroflex quality of the Russian segment, though only on acoustic grounds, without
giving a proper definition of retroflex. Whether these fricatives in Polish and
Russian behave as retroflex phonologically as well, will be dealt with in chapter 4.
The difference between retroflex and non-retroflex post-alveolar fricatives in Slavic
languages was discussed in Hamann (2002b), where I show that Bulgarian, in
contrast to Polish and Russian, has a non-retroflex laminal post-alveolar fricative.
Hamann (2002b) also deals with Czech, but found no coherent phonetic results.
Furthermore, there was no phonological evidence but also no counterevidence for
concluding that the Czech post-alveolar is retroflex.
2.4.1 Polish
Polish has a fricative articulated in the post-alveolar region, as exemplified by the
data in (9).22
(9)
Word-initial
szal [ al] ‘scarf’
word-medial
kasza [(ka a] ‘groats’
word-final
lekarz [l!ka ] ‘physician’
Traditionally, this sound is described as apical palato-alveolar, e.g. by Rubach
(1984) and Wierzchowska (1980), and referred to with the IPA symbol [ ], e.g. by
Dogil (1990). Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996: 155) compare this segment to the
post-alveolar fricative in Mandarin, see figure 2.6, but argue that both sounds do not
belong to the class of retroflexes, as they are laminal flat post-alveolars. At a later
point (p. 154) Ladefoged & Maddieson refer to these sounds as ‘flat post-alveolar
(retroflex)’ and use the alveolar symbol with a subscript dot for them, which is a
traditional way of transcribing retroflex sounds used in studies of the languages of
the Indian subcontinent. Thus Ladefoged & Maddieson’s classification of the Polish
(and Mandarin) sound with respect to retroflexion is unclear, and even less so are
the criteria for their classification. Keating (1991) argues that the Polish sound gives
an acoustic impression similar to that of other retroflexes, hence can be included into
the class of retroflex sounds. But she does not give any articulatory criterion either.
21
22
The discussion in this section is restricted primarily to the voiceless post-alveolar fricative in the
respective languages, but the argumentation can be extended to the voiced counterpart and to the
affricate series as well (recall section 2.2.4.4 on the articulatory similarity of retroflex fricatives and
affricates).
The IPA symbol [ ] is used for the Polish and Russian examples in order to avoid any implications on
their status before looking at their exact articulation.
Articulatory variation and common properties
41
Let us apply the four properties of retroflexion discussed in 2.3 to the Polish
sounds, starting with apicality. The literature differs on the description of the active
articulator of the Polish fricative. Biedrzycki (1974: 20ff.), Catford (1988: 90f),
Dogil (1990), and Spencer (1986) all describe it as apical, Ladefoged & Maddieson
(1996: 154) call it laminal. Ladefoged (2001: 151) describes it as a sound produced
with a raised tongue tip, and Keating (1991) says it is variable. An x-ray tracing of
the Polish post-alveolar voiceless fricative, given in figure 2.11 (based on
Wierzchowska 1980: 64), shows that the tongue tip is not in resting position for this
sound, thus ‘apicality’ (as defined in 2.3.1) is satisfied. Tracings from other sources
(e.g. Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996: 154) show a similar articulation.
Figure 2.11 Polish post-alveolar with apical alveolar articulation (based on Wierzchowska
1980: 64)
Concerning ‘posteriority’, the Polish sound is generally described as post-alveolar.
The x-ray tracing in figure 2.11, however, indicates a place of articulation that is at
the alveolar ridge, and thus further front than defined for ‘posteriority’. The property
of posteriority thus seems not to be fully fulfilled by the Polish sound.
The sublingual cavity of the Polish segment is clearly discernible from figure
2.11, and the literature (such as Keating 1991) agrees on its existence, thus the
criterion of ‘sublingual cavity’ is fulfilled. In cases where the shape of the tongue is
described (e.g. in Keating 1991 and Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996), there is
agreement on its lowered, backed nature, which fulfils the definition of ‘retraction’.
Though not fulfilling all four parameters of retroflexion, the Polish sound can
be classified as retroflex on the grounds of its apicality, its retraction, and the
existence of a sublingual cavity in its articulation.
2.4.2 Russian
The Russian post-alveolar fricative, exemplified by the data in (10), is also
traditionally described simply as a post-alveolar fricative.
(10) Word-initial
s*ag [ ak] ‘step’
word-medial
pos*el [p,( ol] ‘went’
word-final
nas* [na ] ‘our’
The notion of retroflexion is never mentioned in the literature, with the only
exception of Keating (1991). The sound in question is illustrated in figure 2.12
(based on Bolla 1981: plate 60).
42
Chapter 2
Figure 2.12 Russian post-alveolar fricative, based on an x-ray tracing by Bolla (1981: plate
60).
Bolla (1981: 71) describes this sound as a mediodorsal alveolo-palatal,23 but the xray tracings from the same source show that this sound-class is articulated with a
raised tongue tip. Keating (1991: 35) even states that some x-ray tracings of the
post-alveolar fricatives in Russian made by Oliverius (1974) show a bending
backwards of the tongue tip. Hence the criterion of apicality is fulfilled.
Though described as post-alveolar, the place of articulation in figure 2.12
seems to be the alveolar region (according to the definition and illustration given in
2.1), so the Russian fricative is not for certain posterior. Its sublingual cavity is
visible in the x-ray tracing of figure 2.12.
Regarding retraction, phonetic descriptions on the Russian sounds explicitly
mention a velarized and flat tongue shape, e.g. Bolla (1981: 90), and Jones & Ward
(1969: 134). Maddieson (1984: 226) even uses special diacritics to indicate
velarization for these sounds: [ .] and [ /]. Catford (1977: 192) says that the Russian
apico-post-alveolars have “the part of the tongue immediately behind the apex and
the blade slightly hollowed, and the back slightly raised, giving a somewhat
velarized effect.” In addition, the x-ray tracing in figure 2.12 shows a flat tongue
shape and a distinct retraction of the tongue body, so there is no doubt about the
retraction of this sound.
Like the Polish post-alveolar, the Russian fricative definitely fulfils three
properties of retroflexion, although the property of posteriority could not be clearly
determined.
2.4.3 Slavic post-alveolar fricatives as non-prototypical retroflexes
Summing up the results of the comparison in this section, all three Slavic fricatives
fulfil the criteria of apicality, sublingual cavity, and retraction. Posteriority,
however, is a property that is not consistently present in the Slavic sounds. Whether
the post-alveolars segments in Polish and Russian behave phonologically like postalveolars, i.e. [-anterior], is a point that will be discussed in chapter 4. The present
discussion looks at the phonetic criteria only. Since three criteria are fulfilled, the
Slavic post-alveolar fricatives can be classified as retroflex, though they do not
23
This description by Bolla refers to both [ ] and [ &], which differ strongly in their articulation, cf.
section 2.5 below, where I argue that the first one is retroflex and the second one laminal postalveolar.
Articulatory variation and common properties
43
conform to the most prototypical retroflex fricative such as the fricative in e.g. Toda,
which satisfies all four criteria but which is crosslinguistically extremely rare.
Altogether, a segment is still retroflex if it has all criteria except posteriority
satisfied. The question has to be posed here whether any three criteria are sufficient,
or whether it is only posteriority that can be lacking for a retroflex class. To answer
this question, all other combinations of three of the four properties have to be
examined. These possibilities are given in (11).
(11) (a)
(b)
(c)
posteriority & subl. cavity & retraction
apicality & posteriority & retraction
apicality & posteriority & subl. cavity
violates (8c), (8d)
violates (8a), (8b), (8c)
violates (8b), (8e)
A sound with all retroflex properties present except for apicality, see (11a), would
be a laminal post-alveolar with a sublingual cavity and retraction. Posteriority with a
sublingual cavity is possible in a laminal post-alveolar, but this cannot be combined
with retraction, since a laminal articulation always involves a raised tongue middle,
i.e. a bunched tongue (see e.g. x-ray tracings of laminal consonants in general in
Laver 1994, or language-specific for Toda in Shalev et al. 1993), which cannot be
combined with a flat tongue middle and a retracted tongue back. This means that a
segment with the criteria posteriority, sublingual cavity, and retraction but without
apicality is articulatorily impossible. See also the implications (8c) and (8d) which
require the presence of the property apicality in this combination.
The second property that could be missing is the sublingual cavity, see (11b).
As a posterior articulation always entails a sublingual cavity (recall the entailment in
(8a)), such a segment is articulatorily impossible. Finally, retraction of the tongue
body could be missing, see (11c), but apicality and posteriority is only possible with
a retracted tongue back, see (8b), hence also the third possibility of one missing
property cannot be articulatorily realized. Thus, only ‘posteriority’ can be lacking
from a retroflex, as all other combinations of one missing criterion are articulatorily
impossible.
What happens now if two characteristic properties are not present in a
segment; can this still be a retroflex? Combining two properties of the set of four
gives us six possibilities, all listed in (12).
(12) (a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
(e)
(f)
apicality
apicality
apicality
posteriority
posteriority
subl. cavity
&
&
&
&
&
&
posteriority
subl. cavity
retraction
subl. cavity
retraction
retraction
violates (8a) and (8b)
violates (8e)
realized as velarized alveolar apical
realized as laminal post-alveolar
violates (8a) and (8c)
violates (8d)
Four of them, (12a), (12b), (12e), and (12f), are articulatorily impossible because of
the entailments stated in (8), e.g. the combination of posteriority and retraction in
(12e) to the exclusion of apicality and sublingual cavity is impossible, as apicality is
entailed by the implication (8c) and sublingual cavity by (8a) and (8c).
44
Chapter 2
The remaining two possibilities (12c) and (12d) are non-retroflex. Apicality
and retraction alone (12c) yield a dental or alveolar apical with secondary
velarization or pharyngealization. And a segment that fulfils the properties of
posteriority and sublingual cavity exclusively, see (12d), is a laminal post-alveolar
with a non-retracted tongue back. Toda contrasts such a fricative with a retroflex
fricative that meets the requirements of all properties, and hence these two criteria
are also not sufficient for defining retroflexion. The overall conclusion is that a
segment satisfying two properties only of the four defined here for retroflexion
either is unpronouncable or does not fall into the category retroflex.
2.5 Secondary palatalization of retroflexes
In this section it is argued that palatalized retroflex segments do not exist
phonetically, as the two articulatory gestures of palatalization and retraction cannot
be produced at the same time. It is shown that instead, the process of palatalization
triggers a change in the retroflex segment from apical to laminal (as proposed
already in Hall 2000), from flat, low tongue middle to bunched, raised tongue
middle, and from retracted tongue back to fronted tongue back; i.e. from a retroflex
which satisfies all four properties of retroflexion defined in 2.3 to a segment which
satisfies only two of them (namely posteriority and sublingual cavity) and thus is
non-retroflex. The analysis of secondary palatalization in section 6.3.3 will show
that in some cases secondary palatalization of retroflexes is phonologically possible,
though it still remains phonetically impossible.
Evidence for the claim that palatalized retroflexes are non-existent is found in
Maddieson’s (1984) typological study, which lists no language with a phonemic
palatalized retroflex segment. Only two counterexamples could be found in the
phonetic and phonological literature, namely Toda (Emeneau 1984; Spajic et al.
1996) and Kashmiri (Bhat 1987), which are both said to have palatalized retroflexes.
This section proceeds as follows. First, traditional definitions of palatalization
as mere additional articulations are shown to be inadequate for coronal sounds.
Then, to illustrate the change from retroflex to non-retroflex occurring with
palatalization, the Russian retroflex fricative and its palatalized counterpart are
discussed. In subsection 2.5.2, the alleged palatalized retroflex segments in Toda
and Kashmiri are discussed and the status of these segments is analysed. Alternative
descriptions for these supposedly palatalized retroflexes are proposed and it will be
hypothesized that there are no counterexamples to the claim that secondary
palatalization of retroflexion does not occur phonetically.
2.5.1 Palatalization as change in primary articulation
Palatalization in traditional articulatory terms is defined as the superimposition of an
[i]-like gesture upon a labial, dental, alveolar, or post-alveolar consonant (cf.
Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996). This superimposition of a gesture is undoubtedly
the case for labials with a secondary palatalization, where the tongue dorsum gesture
can take place independently and at the same time as the labial closing gesture. But
Articulatory variation and common properties
45
for primary gestures with the tongue (either coronal or dorsal), the primary and
secondary gestures are not independent of each other and therefore are expected to
influence each other, which results in a change of the primary place of articulation.
Support for this assumed change can be found in Ladefoged (1971: 207) who points
out that “the terms palatalization and palatalized may also be used in a slightly
different way from a secondary articulation, namely as describing a process in which
the primary articulation is changed so that it becomes more palatal.” Ladefoged &
Maddieson (1996: 365) further specify this by stating that for all coronal
consonants, secondary palatalization always involves a displacement of the surface
of the tongue. This displacement is said to produce a slightly different primary
constriction location (ibid.). We conclude from this that the traditional description of
a secondary palatalization is inaccurate in the case of coronal segments, as this
process always involves a change in the primary articulation for coronal sounds.
Articulatory evidence for a change of place in palatalized apical dentals is
given in Scatton (1975) for Bulgarian, and C0avar & Hamann (2002) for Polish. Hall
(2000) argues that apical stops in general either turn into laminal stops when
palatalized (in a synchronic or diachronic process) or resist palatalization.
For retroflex segments, it is proposed here that the addition of a palatalization
gesture involves not only a change in primary articulation from apical to laminal but
also in the articulatory class from retroflex to non-retroflex, since retroflexes not
satisfying the criterion of apicality do not exist, see section 2.4.3. Support for this
proposal can be found in Ladefoged (1971: 208), who mentions that the secondary
articulations of palatalization, velarization, and pharyngealization involve different
shapes of the tongue that cannot occur simultaneously. As velarization and
pharyngealization were defined as realizations of the retroflex criterion ‘retraction’
in 2.3.4, Ladefoged’s remark can be interpreted as an articulatory incompatibility of
retroflexion and palatalization.
The incompatibility of gestures and the change in primary place is exemplified
with the Russian fricatives in the post-alveolar region. Figure 2.13 is based on x-ray
tracings of the Russian retroflex fricative (solid line) and its palatalized counterpart
(dashed line) (both based on Bolla 1981: 159). As discussed before (see 2.4.2), the
Russian retroflex fricative satisfies at least three of the four properties for
retroflexion, namely apicality, sublingual cavity, and retraction, and is therefore
assumed to be retroflex in this study (see also Hamann 2002b for a more detailed
discussion of the Russian sound and its palatalized counterpart).
Figure 2.13 Russian retroflex fricative (solid line) and palatalized post-alveolar fricative
(dashed line).
46
Chapter 2
Comparing now the palatalized variant to the retroflex one, some major differences
can be observed. First of all, the place of articulation changes for the palatalized
segment; it moves further backwards to the post-alveolar region, which gives
evidence for the assumed change in primary place of articulation for palatalized
retroflex segments. Furthermore, the articulator is now the tongue blade, and the
shape of the tongue middle changes to bunched and raised. The changes occurring
for the palatalization of a retroflex are depicted in figure 2.14 with the four tongue
parts as assumed already for a retroflex articulation in figure 2.10.
Figure 2.14 Palatalized ‘retroflex’, schematic movements of the tip, blade, middle, and
back, underlyingly grey are the tongue parts at rest.
In terms of retroflex properties this means that the palatalized segment does not
satisfy the properties of apicality and retraction, but only those of posteriority and
sublingual cavity. As defined above in 2.3.5 und 2.4.3, a segment that has fewer than
three retroflex properties does not belong to the category of retroflexes, thus the
palatalized version of the retroflex fricative is claimed to be non-retroflex.
As palatalization in general involves the addition of or change towards an [i]like gesture, and [i] and other front, high vowels are always articulated with a
bunched tongue middle, this implies that secondary palatalization of retroflexes
always results in a change in the property retraction from retracted to non-retracted
articulation. But as apicality and posteriority without retraction is not possible (see
implication in (8b)), a further change from apical to non-apical is necessary. The
resulting segment satisfies the two properties of posteriority and sublingual cavity,
and is according to (12d) not retroflex but a laminal post-alveolar. For the fricative
[] the process of secondary palatalization thus results in the palato-alveolar fricative
[ ], see (13a). The secondary palatalization of a retroflex stop and nasal is assumed
to trigger similar changes, see (13b) and (13c), respectively.
(13) (a)
(b)
(c)
['] = [ ]
['] = [t] or [c]
[1'] = [n] or [$]
Hume’s (1994) observations on the palatalization of the post-alveolar fricative in
Polish can be interpreted to support the claim made here. Hume follows the
Articulatory variation and common properties
47
traditional descriptions of the Polish post-alveolar fricative and refers to it as postalveolar [ ] instead of retroflex. Furthermore, she claims that the segment resulting
from the palatalization process, i.e. [ '], is articulatorily identical to the laminal
alveolo-palatal [ ].24 Thus Hume describes the post-alveolar palatalization in Polish
as in (14), which differs from the proposal made here in (13a).
(14) [ '] = [ ]
Though she does not explicitly mention a change in articulatory class, Hume’s
description of the segments indicates such a change. She refers to the palatalized
sound with the features [–anterior, +distributed], which describe a laminal postalveolar, and to the non-palatalized sound as [–anterior, –distributed], which is a
retroflex segment in traditional featural accounts. Therefore Hume’s description
implies that the process of palatalization in Polish actually changes a retroflex
fricative into a laminal post-alveolar, as stated in (13).
Besides the categorical change described above, another possible outcome of
the secondary palatalization of retroflexes is to resist palatalization altogether, as
pointed out by Hall (2000a). He gives an example from Scots Gaelic, where nouns
usually undergo palatalization in the genitive singular: [kat] ‘cat’ (nom. sg.)
surfaces as [kat'] (gen. sg.). Nouns with retroflex consonants, however, remain
unpalatalized, e.g. [pa&] ‘a poet’ (both nom. and gen. sg.) (Borgstrøm 1940: 76). A
resistance to palatalization is otherwise only reported for apical alveolars or dentals
(e.g. Hall 2000a), which are also inherently retracted in some languages (recall
2.3.4). The property retraction can thus be made responsible for the blocking of
palatalization, for the same articulatory reason that causes this property to change
into non-retraction in secondarily palatalized retroflexes, namely articulatory
incompatibility. It has to be tested whether the apical alveolars that show a
resistance to palatalization are retracted and thus provide further evidence for the
claim of articulatory incompatibility.
In sum, it was shown that retraction is incompatible with palatalization, which
results in two possible outputs for retroflex palatalization, either a corresponding
palatalized laminal, or a plain retroflex without palatalization.
2.5.2 Counterexamples: Toda and Kashmiri
According to Emeneau (1984), and Spajic et al. (1996) the Dravidian language Toda
has palatalized counterparts of all its three rhotics, including the retroflex flap //.
Toda has minimal pairs such as [o] ‘to cook’ vs. [o'] ‘foot’, or [to] ‘thigh’ vs.
[to'] ‘pole used at funeral’. Interestingly, Spajic et al. could elicit retroflex rhotics
and their palatalized counterparts only from some of their subjects; the three
speakers of the Kas mund (a tribal location). The three speakers of the Melgas mund
did not produce any of these forms. Sakthivel (1976, 1977) transcribes the
palatalized retroflex rhotics, like all palatalized segments, with a sequence of rhotic
24
The only difference between the palatalized retroflex and the alveolo-palatal is, according to Hume, a
secondary labialization of the palatalized sound, which the alveolo-palatal does not share.
48
Chapter 2
(or other segment) plus palatal glide, indicating that the sounds in question consist of
a sequence of two different articulations.
Though presenting a detailed phonetic study of the rhotics in question, Spajic
et al. unfortunately do not include any palatographic or linguographic measurements
of the palatalized flap /'/ from which the exact articulation and the correlation of the
gesture of retroflexion and that of palatalization can be judged.
Palatalized retroflex segments are also said to occur in the Indo-Aryan
language Kashmiri (Bhat 1987: 43ff.). Kashmiri has the phonemes /', ', '/. In
Maddieson’s phoneme inventory of Kashmiri (based on Kelkar & Trisal 1964),
these segments are not included, the only retroflexes given there are the plain
plosives /, , /. Morgenstierne (1941) proceeds similarly and does not mention
palatalized retroflexes. The reason for this discrepancy in the description of the
Kashmiri phoneme inventory is probably the class of the so-called matra vowels in
Kashmiri. Matra vowels are extremely short (Maddieson 1986: 271 terms them
‘overshort’) or ‘whispered’ vowels (Masica 1991: 121). One of them is the –i-matra
which is said to leave a palatalizing effect on the preceding consonant. The
assumption of this short /i/ vowel makes the statement of separate palatalized
consonants redundant. Thus, in some descriptions of Kashmiri (e.g. Grierson 1911
and Morgenstierne 1941), the use of a retroflex segment with a following –i-matra
stands for what is described as palatalized retroflex in e.g. Bhat (1987). But this
poses the question whether it is really a secondary palatalization of retroflexes that
occurs in Kashmiri. These doubts are supported by Bailey (1937) who uses a
retroflex plus a vowel /i/ in his transcriptions of Kashmiri in the place where the
other researchers used either the –i-matra or the palatalized consonant. In (15), a
comparison of Grierson’s and Bailey’s transcriptions is given with the masculine
singular forms of the adjective ‘big’.
(15) NOM
DAT
AG
ABL
Grierson
bou
bais
bai
bai
Bailey
bo
b3is
b3i
bai
As in the case of Toda, no articulatory data could be found for Kashmiri to illustrate
the simultaneous combination of the retroflex and the palatalization gesture.
As the present study assumes that a simultaneous articulation of retroflexion
and palatalization is impossible, the segments in these two languages have to be
accounted for in another way. I propose that these segments are actually not
retroflexes with a superimposed palatalized gesture, but sequences of a retroflex
articulation followed by a short glide /j/. This proposal does not imply that the
palatalized segment, which consists of two successive gestures, should be
phonologically interpreted as two phonemes instead of one. I propose that Toda and
Kashmiri are languages that chose to interpret the two gestures as belonging to one
category. Only articulatorily do they make up two gestures.
Articulatory variation and common properties
49
Support for these separate gestures can be seen in the diachronic development
of the alleged palatalized retroflexes in Kashmiri. Diachronically, the matra vowels
in Kashmiri stem from vowels which have been shortened word-finally
(Morgenstierne 1941: 89). Kashmiri hence had two separate gestures that were
assigned to different phonemes, a consonantal and a vocalic one. These gestures
were categorized at a later stage as belonging to one category (not consistently by
every author, though, as we saw above). Furthermore, whereas the –u- and –ü-matras
(both causing velarization of the preceding consonant) are said to be inaudible
nowadays (Morgenstierne 1941: 87), the –i-matra still sounds like a very short [i],
indicating a separate, additional i-gesture (ibid.).
Further evidence for the claim that there are two gestures instead of one may
be found in the acoustic signal of the Toda trills. Spajic et al.’s (1996: 19) data of
one speaker shows a difference between palatalized and non-palatalized retroflex
trill: the duration of /'/ is 190 ms and that of // only 100 ms. The palatalized
version is thus nearly twice as long. There is no articulatory explanation why a
palatalized segment should take longer to articulate than a non-palatalized one if one
assumes that the two gestures co-occur. Assuming, however, that two gestures are
produced successively, the nearly double length of the palatalized segment
compared to the non-palatalized is explained.
In order to judge the values for palatalized and non-palatalized segment
lengths, we will compare them to duration measurements (in ms) of the palatalized
and non-palatalized segment pairs in Russian from Bolla (1981), see the tables 2.1,
2.2, and 2.3 below, giving the labial, velar and coronal (the latter tongue dependent)
articulations, respectively.
Table 2.1 Duration measurements (in ms) of the plain and palatalized labials in Russian
from Bolla (1981).
plain
palatalized
p
b
f
v
m
ratio
116
170
120
140
128
130
115
125
97
97
1
1.14
Table 2.2 Duration measurements (in ms) of the plain and palatalized velars in Russian
from Bolla (1981).
plain
palatalized
Table 2.3
k
9
x
ratio
169
184
150
160
110
120
1
1.08
Duration measurements (in ms) of the plain and palatalized coronals in Russian
from Bolla (1981).
plain
palatalized
t
d
n
134
190
102
120
105
98
s
z
195
177
ratio
96
160
1
1.17
50
Chapter 2
The average ratio between plain and palatalized segments in Russian is 1 : 1.12. The
Toda retroflex rhotics have a ratio of 1 : 1.9 for plain vs. palatalized signal length,
which is far higher than the Russian ratio. This difference sustains the claim made
before that there might be two successive gestures involved in the articulation of the
palatalized retroflex in Toda.
Unfortunately, we do not have any further measurements for palatalized
rhotics, so the present data merely hint at the correctness of the hypothesis made
here. Further research has to be conducted on the exact articulation and gestural
timing of palatalized rhotics in general and palatalized retroflex in particular. This
may shed light on the articulatory timing of the gestures and further properties of
their articulation.
A formal account on how an articulatory sequence of retroflexion and
palatalization can be phonologically interpreted as one segment will be given in
section 6.3.3.5.
2.6 Summary and outlook
In this chapter, it has been shown that the class of retroflex consonants is one with
enormous articulatory variation. This variation can be systematically accounted for
by such parameters as speaker-dependence, context, manner of segment, language
family, and inventory size.
Despite this large variation, there are some characteristics that can be found in
nearly every instance of a retroflex. These properties have been defined as apicality,
posteriority, sublingual cavity, and retraction. In order to belong to the retroflex
class, a segment does not have to meet all four of these criteria: posteriority can be
lacking. This is the case with the post-alveolar fricatives in Mandarin, which have
traditionally been described as retroflex. It becomes obvious from this exception that
a more restricted definition of retroflexion, one that requires all four articulatory
properties to be present in a retroflex segment, would not include all segments
traditionally described as retroflex. Because of this new definition, the non-posterior
Polish and Russian fricatives had to be included into the retroflex class as well. We
will see in chapter 4 that there is phonological evidence for such a wide definition,
since all of the segments included here share some phonological behaviour.
Furthermore, the broad definition of retroflex as applied here results in two retroflex
fricative classes for Toda (see section 2.2.6). How this causes problems for
traditional featural representations but can be dealt with in the featural approach I
follow is a topic of chapter 5.
Apart from posteriority, it was shown that all other criteria have to be present
in a segment to belong to the retroflex class. This was shown by the process of
palatalization which causes a change towards non-apical and non-retracted
articulation and thus from a retroflex to a non-retroflex segment.
Several sections of this chapter indicated topics that have to be further
investigated. With respect to the variation of the retroflex class, the different
Articulatory variation and common properties
51
reduction mechanisms of retroflex gestures applied in specific languages for
instance could be studied; we saw in section 2.2.3 that Swedish retroflexes show
fronting of the retroflex category in fast speech, whereas Hindi retroflexes show
retraction in fast speech. On what factors do these reduction strategies depend, and
do related languages show similar strategies? Another topic of retroflex variation
mentioned in section 2.2.5 was the language-specific realizations of retroflex
classes. Especially the sub-families of the Australian languages are worth
investigating since traits might be found that are not shared by the large group of
Australian families.
More important for the present study are additional studies on the allegedly
palatalized retroflexes in Kashmiri and Toda, and evidence for their actual nonretroflex status in order to sustain the claim of non-existing palatalized retroflexes
made here. In addition, other possible non-posterior retroflex fricatives should be
tested, as for instance the Serbian post-alveolar fricatives as indicated by Keating
(1991), to further attest that segments which lack the criterion of posteriority still
behave phonetically and phonologically as retroflex. These issues I leave open for
future research.
3
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
of retroflexes
Chapter 2 dealt with the fact that retroflexes of all manners can differ largely in their
exact place of articulation and active articulator. Nevertheless, all retroflexes were
shown to share three articulatory characteristics: raising of the tongue tip (apicality),
a sublingual cavity, and retraction of the tongue body. In addition to that, all retroflex segments apart from some fricatives have the characteristic of posterior place of
articulation (posteriority). The present chapter deals with the question of whether the
phonological class sharing these articulatory properties also has some acoustic properties in common. In order to give an answer to this question, the acoustic correlates
of the four articulatory properties are described and compared to the acoustic cues of
retroflexes in natural languages. It will be shown that all retroflexes have a lowered
third formant and often a raised second formant, as can be predicted from the articulatory characteristics, in addition to their manner-specific cues. The second and
third formant cues will be compared to cues of other places of articulation (both coronal and non-coronal), to determine the extent to which they refer exclusively to this
class and hence are sufficient to define retroflexion. A special section deals with the
context dependency of retroflex cues and the resulting variation. Furthermore, the
asymmetrical spread of cues for a retroflex place of articulation is discussed. It will
be checked whether the cues hold only for retroflexes in specific positions where
these cues are more salient, or are context-free. Retroflexion belongs to the so-called
‘weak’ acoustic cues according to Stevens & Keyser (1989) (see the discussion of
weak features in section 1.2), who furthermore claim that these weak cues can be
enhanced by certain articulatory additions such as rounding or velarization. The insufficiency of the notions ‘cue weakness’ and ‘cue enhancement’ with respect to retroflexes will be demonstrated. Finally, the question will be discussed why the
acoustic cues of retroflexion and palatalization are incompatible, supporting the
same claim made on articulatory grounds in chapter 2.
This chapter is structured in the following way. In 3.1 a short description is
given of the type of cues that are necessary for the description of articulations across
different manners. This is followed by section 3.2 on the acoustic characteristics of
retroflexion to be expected from their articulatory properties stated in chapter 2, and
is subdivided into sections on apicality, posteriority, sublingual cavity and retraction
(sections 3.2.1 – 3.2.4, respectively). Section 3.3 looks at the cues of retroflexes
found in natural languages and compares them to the predictions made in the previous section. This part is subdivided into sections on formant frequencies (section
3.3.1), which deals primarily with stops but also nasals and liquids, and spectral cues
(section 3.3.2). The latter is mainly concerned with fricative spectra, and deals with
the question whether Mandarin, Slavic, and Toda post-alveolars are retroflex from
an acoustical point of view (subsections 3.3.2.1 and 3.3.2.2). In section 3.4, the
54
Chapter 3
vowel-dependency of retroflex acoustic cues is discussed, and section 3.5 elaborates
on the asymmetrical behaviour of retroflex cues. This includes the results of a perception test I conducted using Norwegian retroflex and dental segments. Section 3.6
contains a discussion on the usefulness of the notion of weak cues and cueenhancement for retroflexion. In 3.7, the incompatibility of the acoustic cues of retroflexion and palatalization are described. Section 3.8 is a summary of the main
findings.
3.1 Acoustic cues used for the description of retroflexes
In order to be able to describe the acoustic cues for retroflexion, it first has to be determined which cues are relevant for the distinction among places of articulation for
consonants. In general, there are two kinds of acoustic information available, namely
static and dynamic. Static information is given for instance by short-term spectra of
the signal, which illustrate the energy spread of a signal for a specific point in time.
Dynamic information is given for instance by formant transitions, which track the
change of vocal tract resonances from preceding and following vowels into the consonant. Any kind of temporal changes in the acoustic signal is dynamic. Whether
dynamic or static information is needed by the speaker to detect a specific place of
articulation depends partly on the manner of articulation. All kinds of speech signals
with irregular noise patterns such as fricatives or the burst noise of a plosive can best
be captured by spectral information. But whereas for example short time spectral
analyses (STS) are sufficient for the correct perception of fricatives, plosives cannot
be detected on the basis of STS alone, as Walley & Carrell (1983) showed in their
perception tests. They confronted listeners with STS and formant transition stimuli
and found that the cues given by STS are not sufficient for detecting the place of articulation for stops; the listener needs further information on the change over time in
the form of formant transitions. A similar result was obtained by Lahiri et al. (1984),
who showed that the STS of an alveolar stop is very similar to that of a labial, and
listeners therefore tend to confuse the two.
A perception test by Stevens & Blumstein (1975) for Hindi speakers, on the
other hand, attested that for plosives the formant frequencies were not sufficient but
that the burst has to be added to obtain consistent retroflex responses. The same result was obtained by Ohala & Ohala’s (2001) perception test of Hindi plosives. The
correct classification for retroflexes and dentals in their test dropped from an average of 84.9 percent to 66.4 percent when the release was not present in the signal.
Thus, static information (burst) or dynamic information (transitions) alone do not
seem to be sufficient for a correct recognition of the place of articulation for a stop:
both types of information are necessary.1
Besides information on the distribution and change of distribution of energy,
durational cues are also important for the distinction of place contrasts. Ladefoged &
1
Information obtained from such perception tests is not consistent, however. Winitz, Scheib & Reeds
(1972) found that listeners are very well capable of classifying the place of articulation correctly when
presented with the burst of the stop alone, without any information about the formant transitions.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
55
Maddieson (1996: 30) refer in their description of the four coronal stops of Eastern
Arrernte to the closure duration and to the voice onset time (VOT) to distinguish all
four segments.
The following sections of this chapter look both at static cues of retroflexion in
the form of spectral shapes and at cues that describe the change over time in the
form of formant transitions leading into and out of retroflex consonants. The first
formant (F1) will be ignored in the following descriptions, as it is lowered into the
segment for any kind of constriction in the vocal tract (Lindblom & Sundberg 1971),
and the release of this constriction is always accompanied by an increase in F1 (Stevens 1998). Instead, the present study will focus on the transition of the second and
third formants (F2 and F3). Durational cues such as VOT or closure duration will
not be treated separately but referred to whenever relevant.
3.2 Acoustic realizations of the articulatory properties of retroflexes
In chapter 4, four articulatory properties were used to define the class of retroflex
segments, namely apicality, posteriority, sublingual cavity, and retraction. The present section describes the acoustic realizations of these four articulatory characteristics (subsections 3.2.1 – 3.2.4) as predicted from acoustic modelling and as found in
language-specific speech sounds with such characteristics. Subsection 3.2.5 summarizes the acoustic effects of all four properties and serves as a prediction to be compared to the acoustics of retroflex segments in natural languages as discussed in the
sections after that (3.-1 - 3.7).
3.2.1 Apicality
Stevens (1996) and Ohala & Ohala (2001), among others, have stated that coronal
articulations, i.e. articulations with the tongue tip or blade, generally result in a
raised F2. Apical articulations, though, show a lower F2 than laminals (Dart 1991).
Hamilton (1996: 47) found that both apical series in Australian languages have very
similar F2 attributes, which are lower than those of the laminals in the studies he
consulted.2 A similar observation was made by Shalev et al. (1993) for Toda. The
difference between apicals and laminals has been associated with differing tongue
body positions behind the constriction; laminals are assumed to have higher F2 values because their tongue body is in a higher position than for apicals (Dart 1991:
63). The exact position of the tongue body depends largely on the vowel context the
segments occur in; thus, the F2 transition is very much vowel dependent, as pointed
out by for instance Stevens & Blumstein (1975: 219). A detailed description of the
effect of the vowel contexts on the formant transitions of retroflexes will be given in
section 3.4.
2
Hamilton bases his observations on the studies by Busby (1979) on several Australian languages,
McGregor (1990) on Gooniyandi, Evans (1985) on Kayardild, Bradley (1980) on Yanyuwa, and
McDonald (1977) on Yaraldi.
56
Chapter 3
A more reliable cue than the F2 transitions for distinguishing apicals from
laminals seems to be the duration of the transitions. Stevens et al. (1986: 432) point
out that an apical movement can be achieved much quicker than a laminal one. Formant transitions of apicals are thus shorter than those of other consonants. Furthermore, the quickness of the apical gesture results in a more abrupt onset for an apical
release compared to a laminal release.
Besides the distinction by form and duration of transitions, apicals can be differentiated by their spectral shape. The spectra of apical sounds show a strong midfrequency peak, in contrast to laminals, which have monotonously decreasing spectra with increasing frequency, as Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996: 30) illustrate for
Eastern Arrernte.
3.2.2 Posteriority
The criterion of posteriority holds for retroflexes as well as for palato-alveolars and
alveolo-palatals, because all three articulations are coronal and non-anterior, since
all are articulated behind the alveolar region.
The acoustic equivalent of posteriority cannot be treated independently from
the articulatory criterion sublingual cavity, as a coronal posterior articulation automatically produces a cavity under the tongue due to the tongue tip or blade displacement (see implication (8a) in chapter 2). The exact acoustic consequences of
the joint characteristics posteriority and sublingual cavity will therefore be described
in section 3.2.3 below.
It is interesting to note the effects of a shift in posteriority in articulatorily
synthesized speech. Narayanan & Kaun (1999) found that moving the place of constriction further backwards for a coronal lateral results in a lowered F3, whereas the
forward movement from a post-alveolar place of articulation (from retroflex to alveolar) results in an increase in F3. From this it can be assumed that retroflex posterior articulations in natural languages probably show some difference in F3 from
laminal posterior articulations (though we cannot be sure about the direction yet).
Furthermore, the two types of retroflexes as distinguished in chapter 2, one satisfying all four articulatory properties, the other lacking posteriority, might show a difference in the acoustic signal in the degree of F3 lowering: a non-posterior retroflex
should have a less low F3 than a retroflex articulated more back. This hypothesis
was not tested in the present study, since the exact articulation of retroflexes in natural speech is expected to differ in more than the property of posteriority. Other articulatory properties that can vary in retroflexes (though not to a degree that their
retroflex status is at stake) are the size of the sublingual cavity (see 3.2.3), the degree
of tongue retraction (as described in 3.2.4 below), and additional lip-rounding, all affecting the acoustic signal. To actively control these parameters in natural language
in an experiment was beyond the scope of the present dissertation but is obviously a
topic for future research.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
57
3.2.3 Sublingual cavity
The presence of a sublingual cavity has the same effect as a posterior coronal articulation; both add length to the front cavity of the vocal tract, and thus lower its
resonance frequencies (Johnson 1997: 119). Again, retroflexes, palato-alveolars, and
alveolo-palatals share this property. The difference between a palato-alveolar and a
retroflex lies in the size of the sublingual cavity, which increases the further back the
tongue tip or blade closure is located (Sundberg & Lindblom 1990: 1313). In addition, the size of the cavity is partly vowel-dependent, with back vowels allowing a
bigger cavity than front vowels.
For retroflex laterals, Stevens (1998) states that a sublingual cavity has two
acoustic effects, namely the introduction of a low-frequency resonance FR and the
introduction of a zero ZR . The FR is located between F2 and F3, at approximately
1800 Hz and is therefore often associated with the F3 of the adjacent vowel (Stevens
et al. 1986: 436). This resonance frequency does not form a separate, distinguishable
formant during the constriction, but results in a greater bandwidth of F2 or a replacement of F3, which is possible as the F3 is weak during closure of the consonant. FR is therefore sometimes interpreted as a low frequency F3 of the lateral consonant.
The zero ZR introduced via the sublingual cavity is located around 2000 Hz,
but as Stevens (1998) points out, the exact trajectories of FR and ZR show considerable variability cross-linguistically (depending on such factors as elaborated in
chapter 2). The effect of ZR is a weakening of the amplitude of F3 and higher formants. FR and ZR are depicted schematically in figure 3.1, together with the trajectories of the first three formants of a retroflex lateral.
Figure 3.1
Language-independent schema of F1, F2 and F3 trajectories for a lateral retroflex released into a vowel (Stevens 1998: 538).
At the release of the retroflex closure, FR and ZR merge, which results in an increase
in the amplitude of F3, F4, and F5 (Stevens 1998). In sum, the additional formant
and the zero introduced by the sublingual cavity are interpreted as a high second
58
Chapter 3
formant (in cases where FR is associated with F2) and low third and higher formants
(due to ZR ) during the consonant constriction. The transitions into the following
vowel are therefore a falling F2 and a rising F3. A larger sublingual cavity, for instance in palatal retroflexes or in retroflexes in a back vowel context, results in a
lower F3 than for the average retroflex.
These generalizations hold for all retroflex consonants that show continuous
formant structure during the consonant, such as laterals, rhotics, and nasals. For
stops, parts of the formant trajectories are not observable, due to the closure silence
and the stop burst.
Shalev et al. (1993: 117) point out that lowered F3 and F4 values are not restricted to retroflexes but hold for all apicals, as apical alveolars also have a sublingual cavity, though it is considerably smaller (recall discussion in 4.3.3). Dart
(1991) confirms this for languages that contrast apical and laminal articulations, e.g.
the Amerindian language ’O’odham and the Dravidian language Malayalam: in
these languages the apical series have lower F3 and F4 than the laminals.
3.2.4 Retraction
As defined in section 4.3.4, the articulatory characteristic of retraction (or ‘tongue
backing’) subsumes the secondary articulations of velarization and pharyngealization. An indication for their acoustic and auditory similarity can be found in Laver
(1994: 327), who states that pharyngealization gives a similar auditory effect as velarization; “Like velarization, pharyngealization is often described impressionistically as imparting a ‘dark’ quality to segments” (ibid.).
Velarization is usually correlated with a lowered F2. In Marshallese, for example, the second spectral peak of the velarized nasal is low (Ladefoged & Maddieson
1996: 361). Brosnahan & Malmberg (1970: 67) add that velarization also involves a
slight lowering of F3 besides considerable lowering of the frequency of F2. Pharyngealization, the second articulation subsumed under retraction, is also described as
causing a distinct lowering of F2 (Stevens 1998: 365). Ohala (1985) mentions that
the lowered F2 explains why velarization is not perceivable on segments that have a
lowering of F2 anyway, such as labials or back vowels. Ohala’s observation can be
extended to apicals, which were shown above in 3.2.1 to have a lower F2 than laminals. The non-perceptibility of velarization in lower F2 segments might explain why
retroflexes are usually not perceived and thus not described as having distinct velarization or pharyngealization.
3.2.5 Summary of acoustic consequences
In 3.2.1 – 3.2.4 the realizations of the four articulatory criteria of retroflexion were
discussed. It is clear from these descriptions that it is difficult if not impossible to
ascribe a specific acoustic effect to a single articulatory property, as the interaction
of sublingual cavity and posteriority illustrated. Summing up, apicals have a lower
F2 than laminals, but still a higher F2 than non-coronals, in order to indicate a coronal articulation. Both posteriority and sublingual cavity cause a rising of F2 and a
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
59
lowering of F3, and retraction causes a lowering of F2 and F3. A retroflex consonant
is therefore expected to show a lowered F3. For F2, both raising and lowering occur,
but as raising is predicted by three properties, and lowering only by one, we expect
the second formant for a retroflex to be raised or to be neither raised nor lowered,
probably depending strongly on the vowel context.
Whether these cues are actually present in retroflex consonants, i.e., whether
the articulatory defining criteria developed in chapter 2 are realized in the respective
cues in natural languages, will be discussed in the following section.
3.3 Cues of retroflex consonants
In this section, the acoustic properties of retroflexes in natural languages will be
analysed. This description is subdivided according to the type of cues into formant
transitions and spectral shape. These cues are compared to the acoustic realizations
predicted on the basis of the articulatory characteristics of retroflexes, namely low
F3 and high or mid values for F2. Section 3.3.1 on formant transitions is primarily
concerned with retroflex stops, but also describes the formant transitions of retroflex
laterals, rhotics and nasals. Furthermore, a comparison of retroflex transitions to
those of other coronals and to non-coronals will be given in this section. Section
3.3.2 on spectral shape is restricted to fricative spectra. It specifically deals with the
question of what acoustic cues are characteristic for a retroflex fricative. 3.3.2 consists of three subsections in which the retroflex fricative cues are compared with the
acoustics of post-alveolar fricatives in Polish, Mandarin, and Toda, respectively. It is
shown that they all share a low F3, and thus support the hypothesis postulated in
chapter 2 that these segments can be considered retroflex. Additional cues are discussed briefly in section 3.3.3, especially in view of the question of how to distinguish coronal articulations in general.
3.3.1 Formant frequencies
This section deals with the trajectories of F2 and F3 in retroflex stops and other
manners.3 The second formant of retroflexes in natural languages shows considerable variation. Stevens & Blumstein (1975: 219) state that F2 is the same for retroflex and non-retroflex consonants, as this formant is associated with tongue body
movement and therefore largely depends on the vowel context, which is in line with
the predictions made on the basis of the articulatory characteristics above. However,
Ohala & Ohala (2001) found in their investigation of Hindi stops that retroflexes
show a convergence of F2 and F3 in all vocalic environments, whereas Dart & Nihalani (1999) found that the retroflex stops and nasals in Malayalam are generally
characterized by a low F2. From this, one can assume that the height of F2 might be
partly language-dependent in addition to such factors as vowel context. Further in3
The description of formant transitions with the help of locus equations and studies on locus equations
of retroflexes, e.g. Sussman et al. (1993), are ignored here. This method might yield acoustic information on co-articulation, but this information on the place of articulation is not invariant (Fowler 1994).
60
Chapter 3
formation on the variability of F2 and the vowel dependency of this cue is given in
subsection 3.4 below.
Concerning the third formant, the articulatory characteristics in section 3.2
predicted a lowering from the vowel to the consonant and a raising from the consonant constriction to the vowel. This is in accordance with the descriptions of the
class of retroflexes in natural languages. Retroflexion is said to affect mainly higher
formants, which are generally lowered (e.g. Ladefoged 1971: 180 or Ladefoged and
Maddieson 1996: 27). Stevens & Blumstein (1975) state that specifically the third
formant shows a characteristic lowering for retroflexes. This observation is attested
by language-specific investigations of retroflexes. Hamilton (1996: 47) illustrates in
his investigation of Australian languages that a lowered F3 is the most distinctive
acoustic feature of retroflexion. Similarly, both the study by Shalev et al. (1993) on
Toda stops and the one by Dart & Nihalany (1999) on Malayalam stops (and nasals)
show that the retroflexes have the lowest F3 of all coronals.
The low F3 for retroflexes can also be shown to hold for other manners of articulation than stops. Ladefoged and Maddieson (1996) for example state that the
retracted rhotic trill in Malayalam has a lowered third formant (p. 222) and that the
retroflex articulation of the rhotic in American English4 also shows a low F3 (p.
234). Spajic et al. (1996) investigated the trills of Toda, and found that the retroflex
rhotic is distinguished from the dental and alveolar by a lower F3 (and a lower F4).
The formant frequencies of Tamil liquids, as measured by Narayanan & Kaun
(1999), show that the lateral retroflex [] has a F3 – F2 difference of 573 Hz versus
1360 Hz for the lateral alveolar [l], indicating a lowered F3 for the retroflex.5
Stevens & Blumstein (1975: 219) describe retroflexion as showing a clustering
of F3 and F4 in a relatively narrow frequency region. Fant (1968) observes that retroflexion results in the F4 coming down to F3, which is lowered as well. Thus, there
is general consensus on the retroflex property of low F3, which is in accordance
with the acoustic predictions of the articulatory characteristics of chapter 2.
In sum, only one stable acoustic characteristic for retroflexion in the formant
transitions can be manifested, namely a lowered F3, since the F2 seems largely
context-dependent. Hamilton (1996) claims that the lowered trajectory of F3 is what
actually distinguishes retroflexes from other coronals. Whether this is really the case
can be tested by looking at frequency values of the second and third formants in languages with a four-way coronal contrast. The following tables illustrate such data:
table 3.1 gives the average values of stop loci in Gooniyandi based on McGregor
(1990: 56), and table 3.2 the average nasal and lateral coronals formants for several
Australian languages based on Busby (1979: 163 and 161, respectively). When
4
5
The retroflex articulation of American English r exists in addition to several other articulations, cf.
Hagiwara’s (1995) study. Interestingly, Hagiwara points out that a lowering of F3 can be achieved by
three strategies: a constriction of the lips, in the pharynx, or somewhere in the oral cavity around the
velum (p.12), see the discussion of enhancing cues in section 3.6.
Narayanan & Kaun’s study (1999) further shows that there are differences in the actual F3 even
within the class of retroflex liquids. Whereas the lateral [] had an average F3 value of 2321 Hz, the
rhotic [] had an average F3 value of 2082 Hz only. This might be due to manner-specific differences
in the formant trajectories.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
61
comparing the data in the two tables it has to be kept in mind that stop loci are a different kind of information than formants: loci are the assumed frequency of formants
when tracing them backwards, i.e. before the stop burst. Thus, stop loci have more
extreme formant values than continuing formant trajectories of sonorants.
Table 3.1
Stop loci averages for Gooniyandi ( McGregor 1990: 56).
F2
F3
t
t
t
1600
2500
1750
2750
1600
1800
2200
3000
Table 3.2 Coronal nasal and lateral formants averaged over several Australian languages
(Busby 1979: 163 and 161, respectively).
F2
F3
n
n
1654
2499
1502
2279
1735
2129
2156
2936
l
l
1611
2589
1509
2632
1493
2377
2009
2900
For all three classes, the retroflex has by far the lowest F3, though the actual value is
lower for the stop loci (1800 Hz) than for the nasal (2129 Hz) or the lateral formants
(2377 Hz). The formant frequencies of F2 do not show invariant patterns. For the
stops in table 3.1, the retroflex F2 has the same locus as the dental, and both are
lower than the alveolar or palato-alveolar. The nasal retroflex in table 3.2 has F2
values that lie between apical alveolar and palato-alveolar. Still another ordering
emerges from the second formants of the laterals in table 3.2, in which the retroflex
has lowest values. These data are averaged across different vowel contexts, which
might account for these large differences. The language-specific data henceforth
confirm the statement made above that only a low F3 is a reliable and contextindependent cue for a retroflex place of articulation.
Let us look briefly at the difference between the remaining coronals. Comparing the formant frequencies of all four coronal places, the laminal post-alveolar articulation has the most distinctive formant transitions, namely high F2 and high F3.6
The apical alveolar and laminal dental cannot be easily distinguished on the basis of
F2 and F3 trajectories alone. Whereas in Busby’s data on nasals and laterals (see table 3.2) the apical alveolar has a lower F2 value than the laminal dental, McGregor’s
data for Gooniyandi stops (see table 3.1) show a higher value for the apical alveolar
locus. Concerning the third formant, the values for the apical alveolar are lower than
that of the laminal dental in the averaged data by Busby, the reverse relation holds
for McGregor’s data of Gooniyandi stop loci. The similarity in formant transitions
for these two segment classes is shown also by Bradley (1980) for Yanyuwa and by
6
The laminal postalveolars in Malayalam (Dart & Nihalani 1999) seem to depart from the general pattern of a high F3 for palato-alveolars, as these sounds show a low F3 and high F2, i.e. a convergence
of both formants. Dart & Nihalani (1999: 138f.) provide a possible explanation for this behaviour: the
Malayalam post-alveolars might have a higher tongue position behind the constriction than the average post-alveolar, which could account for the high F2.
62
Chapter 3
Evans (1985) for Kayardild. According to Hamilton (1996: 49) the formant transitions of the two classes are essentially identical. Other cues like the duration of the
segments, duration of adjacent vowels, voicing, and spectral properties of the burst
are necessary to distinguish laminal dentals from apical alveolars, see section 3.3.3
below.
Is the property of low F3 sufficient to distinguish the retroflex articulation also
from non-coronal consonants? Let us look at data that compare retroflexes to labials
and velars. Table 3.3 gives the results of Busby’s (1979: 163) measurements of nasal
formants, and table 3.4 those of McGregor’s (1990: 56) measurements of stop loci.
In the latter, a subdivision is made between velars with following front versus velars
with following back vowel.
Table 3.3 Nasal formants averaged over several Australian languages (based on Busby
1979: 163)
m
F2
F3
Table 3.4
1285
2211
1735
2129
1441
2215
Stop loci averages for Gooniyandi (McGregor 1990: 56)
F2
F3
p
ki
ku
1000
2500
1600
1800
1500
2000
750
−
For the labial, retroflex, and velar nasals in table 3.3, the values of the third formant
are all very close together, around 2200 Hz, with the retroflex slightly lower. The
difference in F2 values is more prominent, since the retroflex value is much higher
than that of the other two places of articulation (on average 372 Hz). This is in accordance with Stevens (1996) and Ohala & Ohala’s (2001) observation that coronal
articulations generally have a higher F2 than non-coronals. A similar tendency can
be found in table 3.4, where the F2 locus of the retroflex stop is higher than that of
the other articulations, though only 100 Hz higher than that of the velar in front
vowel context. The F3 value of the retroflex stop locus is lower than that of velar
and labial, but again rather close to the velar in the front vowel context. This similarity between retroflex formant cues and formant cues of velars in front vowel
context has been also observed by Ohala & Ohala (2001: 273) in their measurements
of Hindi consonants. Nevertheless, their perception test (results are presented in 3.4)
did not show any misclassification of retroflex as velar or vice versa in high front
vowel context (for Hindi; the Dravidian or Australian apico-palatals could be very in
this respect). Table 3.5 sums up the formant values of retroflexes compared to other
coronal and non-coronal articulations.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
Table 3.5
63
Height of formant frequencies in comparison to each other as concluded from the
information in table 3.1 – table 3.4 (stop symbols are taken exemplarily, these
generalizations are not meant to be restricted to stops).
F2
F3
b
t
t
t
k
mid
low
high
mid
high
mid
high
lowest
highest
highest
low
low
Altogether, a low F3 is sufficient to distinguish retroflexes from other coronals. This cue might be adequate for a distinction between retroflex and noncoronal places of articulation, though an additional specification as high F2 (indicating a coronal articulation) improves the distinguishability. Furthermore, the perceptual salience of F3 can be very much enhanced by having it coalesce with F2 into
a single peak (see Stevens 1998: 238ff. for a similar observation on vowel formants).
3.3.2 Spectral shape
The shape of the spectrum is another way of describing the acoustic difference between the places of articulation. Spectral information is of use in cases where formant trajectories are not easily detectable, for instance in fricative signals. Though
formant transitions for fricative consonants are similar to those for plosives with the
same place of articulation, Stevens (1996: 484) points out that the transitions for
fricatives tend to be less extreme. Spectral information might therefore be more useful than vowel transitions for classifying fricatives. Furthermore, the spectral shape
is sometimes used for phonological classification. For example Jakobson, Fant &
Halle (1952) use short time spectral analyses (STS), which are measured at the moment of burst release (Stevens & Blumstein 1975), for a classification of stops, and
introduce phonological features based on this information. The correlates of Jakobson et al.’s features are mentioned below, when the features themselves will be discussed in chapter 5. Before describing spectral shapes in detail, it has to be pointed
out that spectra vary considerably from speaker to speaker, and thus a cross-speaker
comparison is often of limited use, see for instance to Shadle et al. (1992) on fricatives in general and Lindblad (1980) on Swedish fricatives.
Coronals in general have an energy distribution across the whole spectrum,
with at least two peaks between 1200 and 3600 Hz. Jakobson et al. express this with
the feature [diffuse]. The class of coronals shares this property with labials, but in
contrast to labials the coronals have a rising shape of spectrum ([acute] in Jakobson
et al.’s terms). Laminals are distinguished from apicals by showing a monotonic decrease of amplitude as the frequency increases, whereas apicals have a more abrupt
decrease. This results in a more spread spectral energy for laminals than for apicals.
Apicals in general show a strong mid-frequency peak (Ladefoged & Maddieson
1996). This peak is narrower for retroflexes, and hence the spectrum of a retroflex is
less spread than that of an apical alveolar (Fant 1974: 138). Jakobson et al. use the
feature [flat] to describe this downward shift or weakening of the upper frequency
64
Chapter 3
components of retroflexes. Lip rounding, velarization, and pharyngealization have
the same acoustic effect of ‘flatness’.
Dart & Nihalani (1999: 139) measured the stop burst spectra for Malayalam
coronals, which illustrate the observations on coronal spectra made here. The spectrum of the Malayalam retroflex fricative has more energy in the lower frequencies
(below 2 kHz) and drops off above 4000 Hz. The dental shows slightly higher energy spread (between 2.5 and 3.5 kHz). The alveolar and the palato-alveolar show
considerably higher energy, with the palato-alveolar having an energy concentration
between 4-5 kHz, and the alveolar having a flat spectrum.
The concentration of energy in the lower frequency is hence a distinguishing
criterion for retroflex fricatives. Stevens & Blumstein (1975) point out that the
frication noise for retroflex obstruents also starts lower than for their non-retroflex
equivalents. This is observable in the Malayalam case and also in Ladefoged and
Maddieson’s (1996: 163) description of the four places for coronal fricatives in the
Caucasian language Ubykh. The retroflex fricative in Ubykh has the lowest frequency of all the sibilants found in this language. As this sound is less retroflexed
(i.e. satisfies only the articulatory characteristics apicality, sublingual cavity, and
velarization, but not posteriority) than for example the Toda subapical palatal fricative,7 it indicates that any kind of retroflex fricative has a very low frequency start.
Shalev, Ladefoged & Bhaskararao (1993) associate the first peak of frication
noise with the third formant of adjacent vowels, thus a low start of frication noise
and a low first peak in a fricative spectrum are equivalent to a low F3 in a vowel and
and can be treated as the same acoustic criterion for retroflexion: where retroflex
vowels and vowel transitions of a retroflex consonants show a low F3 (which is often enhanced by a high F2), the retroflex fricative shows a first, high-amplitude peak
at roughly the same frequency as this F3.
It has to be added that not all retroflex fricative spectra have the lowest start of
friction noise and the lowest first peak compared to other coronals articulated by the
same speaker. For example Lindblad’s (1980) study on Swedish sibilant fricatives
based on his own pronunciation shows some irregularity in this respect. The palatoalveolar [] in this study has a lower first peak and start of the friction noise than the
retroflex [], the difference amounting to roughly 500 Hz. Two possible explanations can be given for the lower first peak in []. First, the sounds might have been
recorded in different vowel context, namely the palato-alveolar in a more back
vowel context than the retroflex, resulting in a larger sublingual cavity and thus
lower first peak (i.e. lower F3). Second, the retroflex in Swedish is non-posterior
(i.e. fulfils only three articulatory characteristics of retroflexion), and thus has a
smaller sublingual cavity as the palato-alveolar, which results in a higher first peak
than for the palato-alveolar. As no information on the exact articulation and context
of these segments is given, we cannot explain at this point why Lindblad’s data depart from the general tendency of lowest friction noise for retroflexes.
7
Ladefoged and Maddieson transcribe the Ubykh retroflex as [s5] because its articulation lacks a bentbackwards tongue tip.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
65
3.3.2.1 Mandarin apical post-alveolar fricatives
Chapter 2 and especially subsection 2.2.4.3 already addressed the extent to which
the Mandarin post-alveolar fricatives can be called retroflex, as these sounds do not
show any bending backwards of the tongue tip but instead a flat tongue middle. In
this section we will compare the acoustic characteristics of these sounds to those of
retroflexion developed above. Lee (1999) measured the frequency ranges of the
spectral energy of the sibilants [s, ts, , t, , t]8 in Beijing Mandarin. This investigation involved two female and two male speakers, table 3.6 gives the mean values
(in kHz) for both groups.
Table 3.6
Female
Male
Frequency ranges (in kHz) of the spectral energy of Mandarin sibilants (Lee
1999: 416).
s
ts
t
t
7 – 14
4.5 – 10.5
6.5 – 13.5
4.5 – 9
4 – 11.5
3 – 9.5
3.5 – 11
3 – 8.5
2 – 10
2 – 7.5
2 – 10
2–7
Comparing the apical, retroflex, and alveolo-palatal fricatives, the retroflex series
has the lowest frequency range, around 2 kHz for both male and female speakers.
This is in accordance with the criterion of low frequency start for retroflex frication
noise given in 3.3.2 above. Unfortunately, no description or data on the shape of the
fricative spectra is given by Lee, thus a further comparison to typically retroflex
spectral shapes cannot be made. But as the findings conforn to traditional (e.g. Chao
1948; 1968) and recent descriptions (Ladefoged & Wu 1984 and Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996) of the Mandarin post-alveolars as retroflex, this does not seem necessary.9
3.3.2.2 Polish post-alveolar fricatives
Similar to the Mandarin sounds, the Polish post-alveolar apical fricatives have been
claimed by some authors (e.g. Keating 1991, Hall 1997a) to be retroflex, though the
traditional Slavic literature does not apply the term retroflex to these segments. Articulatory reasons for treating these sounds as retroflex were discussed in section
4.4.1.
Halle & Stevens (1997) compare the acoustics of the Polish post-alveolars [,
]10 by measuring the two major spectral peaks for each segment in the frequency
range 2 – 4 kHz. Halle & Stevens associate these two peaks with F3 and F4 because
they seem to be contiguous with the third and fourth formants of the adjacent vowels. The values (in Hz) for the major peaks, split according to speakers’ gender, are
given in table 3.7.
8
9
10
The aspirated retroflex and palatal affricates were not a topic of Lee’s study.
It has to be pointed out, however, that Lee (1999) does not classify the Mandarin post-alveolars as retroflex because they do not show any curling backwards of the tongue tip.
The symbols used here differ from Halle & Stevens’s symbols, who transcribe the retroflex fricative
as an alveolar fricative with a subscript dot.
66
Table 3.7
Chapter 3
Average frequencies of the two major lower peaks of the Polish fricatives [] and
[] (from Halle & Stevens 1997: 182).
F3
F4
Female
2590
3510
Male
2470
3100
Female
2480
3490
Male
2520
3150
The values for both sounds are extremely close together and the retroflex does not
show a generally lower F3. In addition to these measurements, Halle & Stevens give
some additional descriptions of the spectral shape of these two sounds. The F3
prominence of the retroflex is narrower than for the alveolo-palatal, and the spectrum amplitude decreases more rapidly for the retroflex, both characteristics being in
accordance with the spectral shape of retroflex sounds as established above.
Dogil (1990) reproduced Halle & Stevens’ study; his findings are summarized
in table 3.7.11
Table 3.8
Average frequencies of the first three peaks for the Polish fricatives [] and []
(Dogil 1990: 49ff.).
F2
F3
F4
Female
1365
2685
2995
Male
1265
2560
2910
Female
–
3055
3560
Male
–
2695
3280
In Dogil’s data, the lower F3 for the retroflex fricative is clearly detectable. Comparing these to Halle & Stevens’s data in table 3.7, it becomes obvious that the two
studies do not differ in F3 values of the retroflex, but in that of the alveolo-palatal.
Halle & Stevens did not find a lower F3 for retroflexes, because the F3 values for
their alveolo-palatals were unusual low. I cannot provide an explanation for this deviation, other than that the alveolo-palatals might have been rounded or in a rounded
context.
Summing up, the acoustic data supports the claim made in chapter 2 that the
apical post-alveolar fricative in Polish is a retroflex. For phonological evidence of
this point, see chapter 4.
3.3.2.3 Toda apical post-alveolar sibilants
The Toda fricative system poses a somewhat different question than that of Mandarin or Polish. As we saw in section 2.2.4.2, Toda is the only Dravidian language with
11
In contrast to Halle & Stevens, Dogil included the allophone [] in his measurements, though the values for this segment are not reproduced here. Furthermore, Dogil investigated both voice and voiceless retroflex and alveolopalatal fricatives, whereas a mean value for both is given in table 3.7. The
alveolopalatal fricatives have no F2 value in this table, as they do not have a recognizable peak in this
frequency range. Dogil did not employ the retroflex symbol for the sounds in question, but the traditionally used palato-alveolar [] instead.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
67
four coronal fricatives, namely a laminal alveolar [s], an apical post-alveolar [s], a
laminal palato-alveolar [], and a subapical palatal []. According to the articulatory
criteria developed in chapter 2, both the subapical palatal and the apical postalveolar fricative can be classified as retroflex. In the present section, it has to be
tested whether both also comply with the acoustic criteria for retroflexion.
Shalev, Ladefoged & Bhaskararao (1993) looked at the acoustic characteristics
of the four coronal fricatives [s, s, , ] in Toda. Table 3.9 gives their measurements
of the mean frequencies for the first peaks of the four sounds, subdivided according
to context. No recordings were made for the palato-alveolar fricative in front vowel
context, indicated by the dash in the corresponding cell.
Table 3.9
Mean frequency values (in Hz) of first peaks of Toda fricatives (based on Shalev,
Ladefoged & Bhaskararao 1993: 114f.)
o_
æ_ / e_
s
s
2883
2967
2192
1742
1725
–
1433
1150
Compared to the subapical retroflex fricative (fourth column), the apical postalveolar (second column) has a higher first peak (676 Hz on average), but still a
much lower one than the laminal alveolar fricative in the first column (on average
958 Hz lower). The peak of the apical post-alveolar is also well above that of the
palato-alveolar in the back vowel context (third column). This is no counterevidence
for a retroflex nature of this segment, but indicates only that the sublingual cavity of
the palato-alveolar is bigger than that of the apical post-alveolar retroflex.
Shalev et al. (1993: 114f.) also provide information on the most prominent
peak in the four coronal fricatives of Toda. The mean values for these data are given
in table 3.9, again split according to context. The dental fricative [s] is not included,
as no information on its most prominent peak was given for more than half of the
speakers (due to the fact that this sound usually does not have a most prominent
peak in the spectrum). Again no data were available for the laminal palato-alveolar
in front vowel context.
Table 3.10 Mean values (in Hz) for the most prominent peak of Toda fricatives (based on
Shalev et al. 1993: 114f.)
o_
æ_ / e_
s
2320 – 3400
3100 – 4350
2350 – 3267
–
1716 – 2707
2269 – 3730
The subapical retroflex (third column) has the lowest peak in both contexts, in accordance with the observation made above that retroflex sounds have the lowest
spectral peaks of all coronals. The peak values for the apical and the laminal palatoalveolar fricative are very close together, both slightly higher than those of []. We
noted earlier that the actual starting frequency of the friction noise and the first peak
68
Chapter 3
are not always the lowest for the retroflex (recall Lindblad’s 1980 data), and that
laminal palato-alveolar and retroflex can have very similar values in this respect.
More precise statements on the retroflex status of the Toda apical post-alveolar can
be given by looking at its spectral shape. Shalev et al. give only two sample spectra
for each of the four fricatives of Toda (p. 112ff.). From these spectra it is obvious
that the apical and subapical sounds share a downward sloping spectral shape, and a
narrow range of peaks, without any prominent peaks in high frequencies (flat spectra). This is in accordance with the spectral characteristics of retroflexes as described
in 3.3.2. Hence the acoustic data confirm the assumption made in chapter 2 that
Toda has two retroflex fricatives.
3.3.3 Further acoustic cues
It has been pointed out in 3.3.1 above that formant transitions (and also spectral information) are not sufficient to differentiate all coronals from each other, in particular the laminal dentals and apical alveolars need additional cues to be distinguished.
This section presents briefly possible additional cues, but does not develop them in
detail, since they are not as relevant as formant frequencies and spectral shape for
the perception of retroflexes and the description of their phonological behaviour.
In the following, the duration of segments and the voice onset time (VOT) will
be treated as acoustic cues that enable the listener to recognize the places of articulation. Further examples of cues that are not discussed here include the duration of
the transitions and of the burst, see for instance the measurements by McGregor
(1990) on stop bursts in Gooniyandi, and the location of the burst noise, cf. Stevens
& Blumstein (1975), among others.
Anderson & Maddieson (1994) state in their study on Tiwi coronal stops that
the closure duration of retroflex segments is the shortest of all coronals. This remains unsupported by Dart & Nihalani’s (1999: 137) measurements of coronal stop
geminates in Malayalam, given in table 3.11.
Table 3.11 Duration of coronal geminates in Malayalam (mean values across nine speakers)
in ms (Dart & Nihalani 1999: 137).
tt
tt
tt
165
210
250
230
As can be seen from this table, the retroflex closure is the longest of all coronals.
But as Dart & Nihalani (p. 137) point out, the test words used for these recordings
had different vowel lengths: the dentals and half of the alveolar test words contained
a long vowel, whereas the remainder contained short ones. This might have had an
effect on the closure duration, as exactly these tokens had shortest closure durations.12
12
Notice the cross-linguistic tendency to shorten consonants after long vowels and lengthen consonants
after short vowels.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
69
McDonough & Johnson (1997) measured the closure duration of Tamil liquids. In their study, the retroflex lateral [] had an average value of 47 ms. Compared
to 71 ms for the alveolar lateral [l], the retroflex closure duration seems rather short.
The values for retroflex and alveolar rhotics from the same study do not form a homogeneous pattern, either. The retroflex flap [] is 50 ms long, the retroflex approximant ["] 65 ms, and the alveolar tap [#] 17 ms, but this difference is most
probably due to a difference in manner of articulation rather than in place.
Thus there seems to be some indication that a retroflex closure is shorter than
that of other coronals, but further studies have to be conducted to confirm this hypothesis.
VOT is another cue that can be used to distinguish the four coronal places of
articulation. Table 3.12 gives the mean VOT of coronal geminates from Dart & Nihalani’s (1999: 137) study on Malayalam.13
Table 3.12 Mean VOT (in ms.) of coronal geminates in Malayalam (Dart & Nihalani 1999:
137).
tt
tt
tt
20
25
15
55
In this study, the retroflexes have the shortest VOT of 15 ms., the laminal postalveolars by far the longest, and dental and alveolar VOT are fairly close together.
Shalev et al. (1993) confirm the clear cue of short VOT for retroflexion. In their
study of Toda the retroflex stops have a significantly shorter VOT than any of the
other stops.
3.4 Vowel dependency of cues
The description of the formant transitions in 3.3.1 indicated already that acoustic
cues are to some degree dependent on context. Stevens & Blumstein (1975: 219) describe that the second formant in coronals depends very much on the vowel context.
They observe a general tendency for back and low vowels to cause a lowering of the
second formant, whereas front vowels keep F2 constant (around 1800 Hz), as the
tongue front stays close to the place of the consonant constriction. The adjacent
vowel also influences the size of the front cavity, which has a systematic effect on
the adjacent consonants. Sundberg & Lindbloom (1990: 1315) point out that this
cavity tends to be larger for consonants adjacent to /u/, intermediate for those adjacent to /a/, and smallest for /i/. Sussman, Hoemeke & Ahmed (1993) found that
variation according to vowel context is to some degree language-dependent, a factor
neglected in the discussion below.
13
No study on the VOT of non-geminate retroflexes could be found. The gemination, however, is expected to prolong all segments similarly, thus the relations between the durations of the coronals can
be taken as representative for both geminates and non-geminates.
70
Chapter 3
Let us look at studies that investigated the vowel dependency of retroflexes in
particular. Ohala & Ohala (2001) measured the vowel-consonant (henceforth: VC)
transitions of the Hindi consonants [p, t, , t, k] after the vowels [i, a, u]. They found
that in all three contexts, the third formant of the retroflex is slightly falling into the
consonant (the low F3 characteristic for retroflexion). Differences can be observed
in the VC-transitions of the second formant. F2 adjacent to /i/ stays almost the same
(at around 2200 Hz), adjacent to /a/ and /u/ it rises into the retroflex (with different
degrees). These observations support the tendencies described above that a front
vowel context does not change the coronal F2, whereas low and back vowels cause a
lowering in F2. It is interesting to observe that all three coronals [t, , t] in Ohala &
Ohala’s study show the same F2 transitions, but differ in the transitions of the third
formant. The differences in F3 are consistent across all vowel-contexts. Furthermore, this supports the observation made above that only the lowered F3 is a stable
and reliable criterion for retroflex articulation, and F2 is shared with all other coronals.
In the same study, Ohala & Ohala also conducted a perception test to see in
how far the vowel context influences perception of place of articulation. The results
for this test are presented in table 3.13 – 3.15, subdivided according to vowel context.
Table 3.13 Confusion matrix (in percent) for Hindi stops after the vowel /a/ (Ohala & Ohala
2001: 275).
response →
stimulus↓
p
t
t
k
p
t
t
k
93.7
0.0
0.0
0.0
0.0
4.8
90.5
0.0
0.0
0.0
1.6
9.5
95.2
0.0
0.0
0.0
0.0
4.8
95.2
0.0
0.0
0.0
0.0
4.8
95.2
In the /a/ context, the listeners generally performed well, with a slight difference
between dental and retroflex recognition, as [t] was miscategorized as [] in 9.5 percent of the cases. Furthermore, the retroflex was misperceived as palato-alveolar affricate in 4.8 percent of the cases. These miscategorizations might be due to missing
consonant-vowel transitions, as the consonant was word-final in the test words. The
results for the /u/ context are given in table 3.14 on the next page.
In this table, only one point is of interest for the class of retroflexes. The labial
plosive is perceived as retroflex in 9.5 percent of the cases, though no indication for
a similarity between these two sounds in /u/ context was given in the acoustic analysis of the vowel transitions.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
71
Table 3.14 Confusion matrix (in percent) for Hindi stops after /u/ (Ohala & Ohala 2001:
275).
response →
stimulus↓
p
t
t
k
p
t
t
k
84.1
31.7
0.0
0.0
1.6
6.3
65.1
0.0
0.0
0.0
9.5
3.2
95.2
0.0
1.6
0.0
0.0
4.8
95.2
0.0
0.0
0.0
0.0
4.8
92.1
The results for the /i/ context are represented in table 3.15. They show an
asymmetry in the categorization: whereas the retroflex was never misclassified as
dental, the dental was misclassified as retroflex in 23.8 percent of the cases.
Table 3.15 Confusion matrix (in percent) for Hindi stops after /i/ (Ohala & Ohala 2001:
275).
response →
stimulus↓
p
t
t
k
p
t
t
k
95.2
4.8
1.6
0.0
0.0
4.8
71.4
0.0
1.6
0.0
0.0
23.8
92.1
17.5
0.0
0.0
0.0
4.8
76.2
0.0
0.0
0.0
1.6
4.8
95.2
Furthermore, the palato-alveolar affricate was classified as retroflex in 17.5 percent
of the cases, and the retroflex as palato-alveolar in 4.8 percent. This confusion might
be explained by the similarity in formant transitions for both segments in /i/ context,
as both show a near convergence of F2 and F3, as Ohala & Ohala indicate. It can be
concluded from the results for the /i/ context, that an adjacent front vowel with its
high F2 value yields more confusion between the coronal segments than any other
vowel context. This might be due to the fact that the closeness of F2 to F3 might
mask the actual transition of the F3. Interestingly, the similarity between velar and
retroflex (and partly labial and retroflex) in the /i/ context did not have any influence
on the perception test. The distinctions in the open and back vowel were generally
better, as in this context the coronals have slightly diverging F2 values, which
helped identifying the place of articulation.
McDonough & Johnson’s (1997) and Narayanan & Kaun’s (1999) experiments on Tamil liquids support the findings for the stops, though manner-specific
differences could be observed: the retroflex lateral generally showed a lower F2
value and a higher F3 value than the retroflex rhotic. The differences in formant
transitions depending on vowel context can also be observed in fricative spectra.
Lindblad (1980) compared the spectra of the Swedish fricatives in different vowel
contexts. Though they differed quite extensively in the shape of the spectra even for
72
Chapter 3
the same place of articulation, the retroflex spectra consistently show the lowest cutoff frequency of all fricatives (across different speakers).
3.5 Symmetrical spread of retroflex cues
The acoustic signal of a retroflex segment shows some asymmetry between the transitions into and transitions out of the segment, as noted e.g. by Dave (1977) and Steriade (1995, 2001a). Whereas the transitions from a vowel into a consonant (henceforth: VC transitions) for retroflexes show some distinct lowering of the third formant (and mid to high F2 according to the vowel context), the CV transitions are far
less extensive, and are closer to those from other coronals, especially apicals. This
acoustic similarity was attested in several phonetic studies. For example, Spajic e t
al. (1996: 17) found that the retroflex trill in Toda “starts off retroflexed and ends up
in a position more like that of the alveolar rhotic”. Their comparison of the formant
frequencies of the retroflex at onset, middle, and offset (in postvocalic context) in
the study on Tamil shows this difference: the onset frequency is on average (across
five vowel contexts) 340 Hz lower than the offset frequency (ibid.). Similar observations were made by Dave (1977) for Gujarati and Anderson & Maddieson (1994)
for Tiwi.
The class of retroflexes differ very much from other consonants in this respect,
which usually show stronger CV cues than VC ones, as attested by Ohala (1990),
Sussman et al. (1997), and Warner (1999), among others.
The similarity in CV cues for retroflexes and apicals can be explained by examining the articulatory characteristics of the retroflex. A retroflex articulation involves a movement of the tongue tip towards the post-alveolar place of articulation
before the actual stop closure. This movement results in a sizeable lowering of the
VC transitions. During the constriction phase, the tongue tip is moved to a less displaced position (the so-called flapping out gesture as described in chapter 2), so that
by the time of the constriction release it is in a position close to that of an apical alveolar, at least for stops and flaps. This results in CV transitions similar to that of an
apical alveolar (Ladefoged and Maddieson 1996: 28). Krull et al. (1995) studied
Tamil, Hindi, and Swedish retroflex stops and found that in all three languages this
class showed a more posterior articulation at the beginning of the closure than at the
release.
The asymmetry of cues can explain why retroflex neutralization occurs often
in word-initial position and post-consonantally, but less often in post- or intervocalic
position, as Steriade (1995, 1998, 2001) argues. Nevertheless, some work shows that
the CV cues of retroflexes are strong enough to distinguish them from apical alveolars in non-VC prominent positions, see e.g. McGregor (1990: 70f.) on Gooniyandi,
Platt (1972: 7) on Gugada, and Blake (1979: 190) on Pitta-Pitta. Evidence for this
strength can be gleaned from the fact that there are several languages like Hindi that
distinguish between retroflex and non-retroflex apical phonemes in word-initial position, where VC cues are not available to the listener. A list of such languages from
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
73
Australian and some examples illustrating this phenomenon are given in section 4.5
on neutralization.
Whether VC cues are really more dominant than CV cues can be tested in perception experiments. Öhman (1966) tested all Swedish stops (including dentals and
retroflexes) in the context of the vowel /a/. The listeners had to judge four types of
signals; the whole, the VC part, the CV part, and the release only. The CV signals
for retroflexes were poorly perceived. Öhman speculates that this is due to “the nonSwedish syllable structure of these utterances” (1966: 988). Krull (1990) constructed
a test similar to Öhman’s: she tested Swedish voiced stops only, with the same four
types of signals. Her results are 11.8 percent misperception of retroflex VC signals
as dental, whereas only 7.3 percent of the retroflex CV signals were miscategorized
as dentals. Ahmed & Agrawal (1969) tested the perception of CVC words with all
Hindi consonants. The words were presented to Hindi listeners as CV and VC signals. Their results show that CV cues are more reliable for retroflexes, as they
yielded only one percent of misclassification, opposed to 7.5 percent of retroflex
misclassification in VC condition. Summing up these perception studies, there is a
large inconsistency in the results. Öhman (1966) shows that retroflex VC cues yield
fewer miscategorizations than retroflex CV cues, indicating that VC cues are
stronger. Krull (1990) and Ahmed & Agrawal (1969), on the other hand, found that
CV cues of retroflexes fair better in a classification task. The outcome of these experiments cannot be attributed to language-specific differences, as the two opposing
results for the tests on Swedish (Öhman 1966; Krull 1990) show.
Hamann (2003) conducted a so-called cross-splicing perception experiment (as
used by Repp 1978 and Ohala 1990) with Norwegian dental and retroflex stops and
nasals. In this test, dental and retroflex signals in the same vowel contexts were cut
in the stop phase of the plosive and in the middle of the nasal and re-combined in
such a way that the resulting signal had VC cues of one place of articulation and CV
cues14 of the other. Six Norwegian and six German native listeners were then asked
to categorize these signals (four repetitions of each stimulus item in intervocalic [a]
and [i] context) as either one segment or the other, or neither.15,16 The results of this
study for the voiceless plosives are presented in tables 3.16 and 3.17 on the next
page. The data is split into the /a/ and /i/ contexts, respectively, as there is considerable difference between both. Answers by the Norwegian subjects are given on the
left, those by the German subjects on the right.
14
15
16
CV cues’ of the plosives here and in the following is meant to include the stop burst, which has additional information on the place of articulation.
My experiment also included gated signals, i.e. stops without burst and nasals without sufficient CV
cues. As this condition is not relevant for the present argument, the data are not included.
The original test was planned to include fricatives, but the cross-spliced fricative stimuli yielded perceptually non-homogeneous, disturbing signals that could not be classified as either dental or retroflex.
74
Chapter 3
Table 3.16 Confusion matrix (in percent) for stops in the /a/ context (Hamann 2003).
response →
stimulus ↓
t
t
t
90
0
Norwegian subjects
∅
10
100
0
0
t
German subjects
∅
71.4
28.6
28.6
71.4
0
0
The results for the stops in the /a/ context show that VC cues dominate the classification for both German and Norwegian native speakers. For the Norwegians, the retroflex CV cues affected the categorization slightly because 10 percent of the [t] signals were classified as retroflex, whereas the denti-alveolar CV did not have any influence, as all [t] signals were categorized as retroflex. This is in accordance with
Ohala & Ohala’s (2001) findings. The German subjects were affected by the presence of the CV cues in both denti-alveolar and retroflex cases, as 28.6 percent of the
signals were classified according to these cues.
The responses to stop signals in an /i/ context are presented in table 3.17.
Table 3.17 Confusion matrix (in percent) for stops in the /i/ context (Hamann 2003).
response →
stimulus ↓
t
t
Norwegian subjects
∅
t
73.3
13.3
13.3
0
80
20
t
German subjects
∅
47.6
28.6
52.4
61.4
0
10
The Norwegian listeners again paid more attention to the VC cues of the retroflex
than to the VC cues of the dentals. This time, the dental CV cues led to some confusion and a choice of ‘neither’ in 20 percent of the cases. German listeners were
strongly influenced by the retroflex CV cues, which determined their choice of category in 52.4 percent of the cases. The influence of the dental CV cues did not
change compared to the /a/ context, but in 10 percent of the answers to these cues
the listeners did not classify the signals. The /i/ vowel context thus resulted in quite
different answers compared to the /a/ context, particularly the retroflex and dentialveolar VC cues are less distinct in this context (for an explanation, recall the influence of the vowel context on the coronal cues described in 3.4). The responses to
nasal signals are presented in the following two tables. The signals in the /a/ context,
given in table 3.18, show a clear dominance of retroflex VC cues for both subject
groups. The denti-alveolar VC cues are overridden by the CV cues of the retroflex in
46 percent of the cases for the Norwegian listeners, and in 38.1 percent of the cases
for the German listeners.
Table 3.18 Confusion matrix (in percent) for nasals in the /a/ context (Hamann 2003).
response →
stimulus ↓
n
n
Norwegian subjects
∅
n
54
46
0
0
100
0
German subjects
n
57.1
14.3
38.1
85.7
∅
4.8
0
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
75
The general result for nasals in an /a/ context differs from that for stops in the same
context (see table 3.16), in as far as the VC cues are less dominant. This is probably
due to the strong internal cues of nasals, which give contradictory information of
two places of articulation to the listener. For the nasals in /i/ context, see table 3.19,
the VC cues dominated clearly.
Table 3.19 Confusion matrix (in percent) for nasals in the /i/ context (Hamann 2003).
response →
stimulus ↓
n
n
n
66.7
0
Norwegian subjects
∅
13.3
100
0
0
German subjects
n
95.2
4.8
4.8
80.9
∅
0
14.3
The results of Hamann’s test show that retroflex VC cues determine the classification of the sound much more often than retroflex CV cues. But one has to keep in
mind that the retroflex cues always had to compete with dental cues. Thus, the results cannot be interpreted as retroflex VC cues being stronger than retroflex CV
cues. Rather, retroflex VC transitions dominated denti-alveolar CV transitions. In
the reverse case of signals with denti-alveolar VC and retroflex CV, the dentialveolar VC cues were dominated by retroflex CV cues in 25.8 percent of the cases
(compared to 9.5 percent of dominating CV cues of denti-alveolars). This indicates
that both retroflex transitions are stronger than the corresponding dental ones.17 Furthermore, the vowel context had some impact on the results, as it added some indecisiveness (the choice of the option ‘neither’ rose from 0.6 percent in /a/ context to
7.2 percent in /i/ context). The nasals were influenced by the vowel context to a
smaller degree because of their strong internal cues.
Thus in contrast to the previous tests, the cross-splicing experiment by Hamann (2003) showed that for both apico-dentals and retroflexes the VC cues dominate the perception, despite the fact that the CV cues of retroflexes are sometimes
strong enough to override dental VC cues. This implies that though there is an
asymmetry in the signal of retroflex segments in the shape of more extensive VC
transitions than CV ones, the CV cues of retroflexion are still strong enough to give
the listener information on the place of the articulation. Hence, the asymmetrical
spread is an interesting phonetic effect, but has no direct impact on the occurrence of
retroflex segments. Furthermore, it should be noted that retroflexes are not the only
place of articulation that display this kind of asymmetrical behaviour with respect to
VC cues. According to Hamilton (1996: 47), laminal palato-alveolars have more robust cues in VC transitions than in CV transitions, which often condition a diphthongization of the preceding vowel, as in Djabugay (Patz 1991: 254).
17
For a discussion of the language-specific differences in categorization, see Hamann (2003).
76
Chapter 3
3.6 Retroflexion as a weak cue and its possible enhancement
Some phoneticians and phonologists, such as Stevens et al. (1986) and Stevens &
Keyser (1989), make a distinction between robust (basic) and weak (redundant)
acoustic features, recall the discussion in section 1.2. Robust features are defined as
features that are manifested in a very short time (within a few miliseconds), whereas
so-called weak features require much more time to be detected. According to Stevens et al. and Ohala (1993: 89), [coronal] is a robust feature, but its dependent [retroflex], which is said to have a rather slow acoustic modulation and to be detected
later by the auditory system, is a weak cue. Ohala (1985) claims that languages with
small inventories utilize segments which consist of robust features, and only languages with larger inventories use the less salient features. Ohala therefore argues
for the weakness of retroflex cues as an explanation for their restricted occurrence:
“if a language has one apical stop, it is generally not retroflex” (p. 225).
This section argues against the claim that retroflexion is a weak cue and that
the reason for the scarcity of inventory segments with a retroflex as the only coronal
is to be found in the acoustic cues for retroflexion. It is assumed instead that the articulatory complexity of the retroflex gesture accounts for its scarcity crosslinguistically. Furthermore, this section deals with another concept associated with
acoustic cues, namely that of ‘cue enhancement’, as described by Stevens et al.
(1986). It will be shown that the notion of cue enhancement is problematic for several reasons.
Let us look at the supposed weakness of the retroflex cues first. It was illustrated in section 3.3.1 that the main acoustic cue for retroflexes, namely the lowering
of F3, by no means takes more time, nor is it harder to detect, than that of any other
coronal place of articulation. On the contrary, the trajectories of F3 for a retroflex
are very distinct, especially in VC transitions. This means that already at the beginning of the acoustic signal the cues are strong enough for a correct identification of
retroflexion. Support for this claim can be derived from Hamann’s (2003) crosssplicing experiment, where the retroflex place of articulation could be detected more
reliably than the dental place, only on the basis of a part of the cues (VC or CV).
The phonetic grounding of a supposed ‘weakness’ of retroflex cues is therefore
missing, unless one argues that all coronal sub-articulations, i.e. retroflex, apical alveolar, laminal dental, and palato-alveolar, are detected only after a general cue for
coronality is perceived. This, however, raises the question of whether the auditory
system really detects some superset such as ‘coronal’ prior to the subset ‘retroflex’
or ‘palato-alveolar’. If it does, what are the cues triggering the coronal place of articulation in general? We saw in section 3.3 that a high F2 seems to be the unifying
acoustic feature for coronals. But this cue is shared with other places of articulation,
e.g. with velars or labials in front vowel context. In addition, one would expect subjects in perception experiments to miscategorize within the coronal class, if ‘coronal’ were to be detected prior to the exact coronal place of articulation. However,
perception experiments do not attest such a behaviour, recall Ohala & Ohala’s
(2001) results in section 3.4 above.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
77
Ohala’s argument that languages tend to utilize strong features in small inventories and weak ones only redundantly or in larger inventories might be understood as evidence for the weakness of retroflex cues. But as was illustrated in section 1.1, the restricted occurrence of retroflexes is rather due to its articulatory complexity then to its perceptual weakness. Thus, there is no need and no evidence to
postulate weak cues for retroflexion in order to explain their cross-linguistic scarcity.
Moving on to the second point, cue enhancement, Stevens et al. (1986) claim
that languages can choose among a variety of articulatory gestures or secondary articulations to enhance the perception of retroflexion. Both rounding (or labialization)
and velarization are assumed to serve this purpose by Stevens et al. and Flemming
(2002). As was argued extensively in chapter 2, retroflexion is assumed to always
co-occur with retraction, i.e. velarization, in the present dissertation. It is therefore
difficult to decide in how far velarization serves to enhance the retroflex cues. The
secondary articulation of rounding, on the other hand, is not a concomitant feature of
retroflexion. Rounding enlarges the front cavity, resulting in a general lowering of
the frequencies of all formants (recall the discussion in 3.2.3 on the sublingual cavity). Retroflexes are characterized by a mid to low F2, lowering of F2 is thus no additional cue for detecting retroflexion and does not enhance the retroflex cues. Only
the lowered F3 caused by rounding might serve this purpose.
These observations lead to the question of how a segment can be enhanced, if
it has already a certain degree of the cue to be enhanced. Enhancement then seems to
be only possible if the degree of the cue is not maximal. Applied to retroflexes this
means that only those retroflexes might undergo enhancement which do not show a
strong lowering of their F3 already. This is the case for non-posterior retroflexes and
retroflexes in a front vowel context. Both can undergo a further lowering of the F3
either by enlarging the sublingual cavity (i.e., changing into a retroflex with posteriority, which applies to the non-posterior retroflex only) or by rounding (which enlarges the front cavity and applies to both). The latter can be found for example in
the Polish retroflex fricatives, which lack the criterion of posteriority and are reported to be rounded (recall the description in section 3.3.2.2).
Furthermore, it is not clear how one can distinguish which articulatory configuration is responsible for enhancing an acoustic cue, if complex tongue positions
such as those for retroflex segments are assumed. If retraction is assumed to be a
concomitant property of retroflexion as done in the present dissertation, both articulatory movements result in similar acoustic consequences, and it cannot be decided
which gesture enhances which (see Boersma 1998: 359f. for a similar argumentation). This casts doubt on the usefulness of the notion cue enhancement, at least for
the retroflex class.
3.7 Acoustic incompatibility: Retroflexion and palatalization
It has been argued in section 2.5 that the palatalization of retroflexion, which is
sometimes claimed to exist for certain segments in Polish and Kashmiri, is articula-
78
Chapter 3
torily impossible, as the two gestures of flat, retracted tongue body and raised
tongue body cannot be produced at the same time. Instead, it was argued that the
secondary palatalization of a retroflex segment triggers a change from an apical
post-alveolar (i.e., retroflex) to a laminal palato-alveolar (for fricatives) or postalveolar (for other manners).
This section will show that there are also acoustic reasons why a palatalized
retroflex is impossible. We saw above that retroflex segments are characterized by a
low F3 and an average or high F2 value. Secondary palatalization has the acoustic
property of a considerable raising of F2, and a slight raising of F3 (Brosnahan &
Malmberg 1970: 67). If we apply these changes to a retroflex segment, the resulting
sound would be expected to show a high F2 and an average or high F3 (in addition
to further cues that indicate the manner of articulation). High F2 is a characteristic of
all coronals as was shown in section 3.3.1, though retroflexes often only have a high
F2 in a front vowel context (recall the discussion in section 3.4). A non-low F3,
however, cannot be interpreted by the listener as retroflex anymore when the articulatory change for secondary palatalization occurs, as a low F3 is the only stable
acoustic characteristic of retroflexion. Instead, the listener will perceive a sound
with a high F2, i.e. a laminal coronal, since laminals have higher F2 frequencies than
apicals (see 3.2.1), with a middle to high F3, which indicates a laminal palatoalveolar.18 Thus, the secondary palatalization of retroflexion results acoustically in a
non-retroflex segment, supporting the claim made in section 2.5 that articulatory
demands change the retroflex into a non-retroflex segment under secondary palatalization.
Languages that have a palatalized counterpart to a retroflex, such as [] in
Polish and Russian as the palatalized counterpart of [], show a kind of repair
mechanism to maintain some similarity to the original sound: they add distinctive
rounding to the palato-alveolar. As we saw in the previous section on cue enhancement, rounding has the effect of lowering the third formant. The rounded, palatalized palato-alveolar thus preserves one cue of retroflexion.
3.8 Summary
It was shown in this chapter that the four articulatory properties of retroflexes proposed in chapter 2 yield exactly those acoustic characteristics that can be observed
for this class in natural languages, providing additional evidence for the proposed
properties. Furthermore, retroflexes could be shown to have the common acoustic
characteristics of a low F3 (which is also observable in friction noise as a lower first
peak in the friction energy than is found for example in other sibilants), and a mid or
high F2, the latter depending on the vowel context. The low F3 shows some variation in the degree of lowering, depending on two factors: the language in which the
segment occurs and its vowel context. Languages differ in the exact place of articulation of a retroflex, and the further back the place, the lower the F3. A non-posterior
18
Note that the laminal palato-alveolar that is argued to emerge acoustically from the secondary palatalization of a retroflex is identical to what has been argued to be the articulatory result in chapter 2.
Acoustic cues and perceptual properties
79
retroflex therefore has a less lowered F3. Furthermore, retroflexes in a back vowel
show a lower F3 than those in any other context.
Though retroflex cues are not symmetrically spread, and the VC cues seem to
be more distinct than the CV cues, it was argued that retroflexes cannot be detected
by their VC cues alone and that the CV cues play a role in distinguishing retroflexes
from other places of articulation. Therefore it is assumed that low F3 holds for all
retroflexes in all positions. As argued already in chapter 1, no indication could be
found for the alleged weakness of retroflex perceptual cues.
Both chapters 2 and 3 make up the phonetic part of this dissertation. These
chapters give an articulatory and an acoustic definition of the class of retroflexes as
they are going to be used in the phonological part in chapters 4 to 6. The definitions
and thus the phonetic space of this class (as depicted in figure 1.2 in chapter 1) were
chosen as unrestricted as possible to include borderline cases of retroflexion. Alternative, more restricted definitions and their consequences for phonological classification will be shortly discussed in chapter 7.
4
Retroflex processes and their phonetic
grounding
In this chapter, cross-linguistically very common phonological processes involving
retroflex segments are discussed. These processes are: retroflexion in a rhotic
context and in a back vowel context, de-retroflexion in a front vowel context (and in
secondary palatalization) or retraction of the front vowel, retroflexion of velarized or
labialized segments, retroflexion of vowels before retroflex segments, nonoccurrence of retroflexes word-initially and post-consonantally, and (local and nonlocal) assimilation of non-retroflex coronals. They are represented in this order in
(1), where t is a cover symbol for a [+anterior] coronal, and for a retroflex segment.
C is any kind of consonant, V any kind of vowel, and (ω indicates the left boundary
of a prosodic category higher than the syllable (phonological word, phrase
boundary). The first column gives the inputs, the second the outputs, and the third
gives these processes terms that I will continue to use in this and following chapters.
(1)
input
/rt/
/ut/
/i/
/ /
(d) /V/
(e) //
(f) /t/
/Vt/
(a)
(b)
(c)
output
[]
[u]
[it] or [ ]
[t ] or []
[V ]
(ω[t], C[t]
[]
[V]
process
retroflexion in rhotic context
retroflexion in back vowel context
deretroflexion or vowel retraction
deretroflexion or depalatalization
retroflexion of adjacent vowel
phonotactic restrictions on retroflexes
local assimilation of retroflexes
non-local assimilation of retroflexes
Bhat (1973) already described some of the processes in (1), namely the retroflexion
of dentals or alveolars by a preceding /r/ (1a), by a preceding back vowel (1b), and a
preceding retroflex consonant (1f). Retroflexion caused by implosion, another
process elaborated by Bhat (p. 43), will not be treated here, as I want to restrict my
investigations to normal egressive airflow. Two further processes of retroflexion that
I found when collecting and analysing the data are not included in the descriptions
below and the analysis in chapter 6: these are retroflexion via secondary velarization
and via secondary rounding, see (2a) and (2b), respectively.
(2)
(a)
(b)
/t/
/t/
[]
[]
retroflexion of velarized segments
retroflexion of labialized segments
These processes are phonetically motivated, since retroflexion via velarization is a
type of articulatory assimilation if one assumes retroflexes to be inherently retracted,
and retroflexion via rounding has obvious perceptual similarities between input and
output, cf. section 4.3.2.4 on the rounding of vowels before retroflexes.
Nevertheless, the two processes are not included here, because of the lack of clear
82
Chapter 4
supporting examples. Ponapean is the only language I found that might have
introduced retroflexes via velarized alveolars, though the literature (Rehg 1973,
Harrison 1995) is not very explicit on this point. Retroflexion via secondary
rounding seems to have been a diachronic process in Athapaskan; the affricate [ ] in
Minto-Nenana originates from a rounded segment, though the actual realization of it
is a topic of unclarity. Krauss (1973) describes the respective Proto-Athapaskan
segment as */k/, Tharp (1972) refers to it as */t/, and Rice as (1989) */t/. I leave
the clarification of these data and the collection of further examples of both
processes open for future research.
Bhat (1973) found that rules of retroflexion are typically caused by a preceding
non-retroflex sound or by the retroflexion spreading leftward, i.e. the expected order
in the input is a non-retroflex followed by a retroflex segment. This will be shown to
be a general tendency for the examples given in this chapter, but even so most of the
phonological changes in (1) will be shown to occur also in reverse segmental order
of input (and also output), namely the retroflexion in rhotic context (1a) and in back
vowel context (1b), avoidance of front vowel context (1c), and assimilation
processes (1f). Examples in the subsections below will illustrate this point. Not all of
the processes have retroflex segments as outputs (as (1a), (1b), and (1f)); some of
them have retroflexes as input (for instance, the process of de-retroflexion in (1c)
and (1e)), others are only triggered by retroflexes (for instance, the vowel changes in
(1c)).
For each change in (1), evidence from several language families is given in
order to show the universal validity of the process. This universality is argued to be
based on the phonetic grounding of these processes, explained by the articulatory
and acoustic characteristics of retroflexion as elaborated in chapters 2 and 3,
respectively. The present chapter gives no phonological account of the processes
involving retroflexes. The data collected here show general, recurring patterns that
will be represented in the phonological formalizations of retroflexes in general in
chapter 5 and of these processes specifically in chapter 6.
The present chapter is structured as follows. Section 4.1 deals with retroflexion
induced by rhotic segments (1a). In section 4.2, the retroflexion of coronals in back
vowel context is discussed (1b), whereas section 4.3 deals with the opposite process,
non-retroflexion in front vowel context (1c). Vowel retroflexion (1d) is the topic of
section 4.4. In section 4.5, the phonotactic restrictions of retroflexes (1e) are
discussed. Lastly, section 4.6 deals with local and non-local assimilations of
retroflexes (1f). All of these sections are subdivided into two subsections, where the
first presents the examples and the second gives the phonetic grounding of the
process under discussion. Section 4.7 concludes.
Before starting with the detailed discussion, a note on the transcription is
necessary. In the examples throughout the chapter, the retroflexes are represented by
their respective IPA symbols, which sometimes depart from their description in the
original sources. I also transferred non-retroflex segments into respective IPA
symbols if the sources used other transcriptional systems.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
83
4.1 Retroflexion in rhotic context
The first process to be discussed here is the change of an anterior coronal into a
retroflex caused by a rhotic, as illustrated in (1a). This process can be found in
North-Germanic languages, in Australian languages and Indo-Aryan languages. It is
also diachronically attested in some of these languages, and is responsible for the
introduction of retroflex phonemes into a number of languages, as illustrated below
in 4.1.1. Section 4.1.2 will provide a possible explanation based on the acoustic
characteristics of the segments involved in this process.
4.1.1 Examples
Before examples of this process are given, one problem in its description has to be
pointed out. Due to the lack of appropriate graphemes for retroflex sounds in Latinbased writing systems, different methods for representing these sounds are
employed in languages with retroflex phonemes. One of them is to write a sequence
of r plus t, d, s, n, l, or r. This is often found in the literature on Australian
languages, see for example McKay (2000) on Ndjeb bana.1 Such a transcription
convention should not be confused with an underlying sequence of two phonemes,
namely a rhotic and an alveolar or dental, which can be realized with one phoneme
via a phonological contraction rule, as demonstrated below. The difference of these
two representations is of particular importance for the North-Germanic languages
Swedish and Norwegian, which are assumed to have a retroflex phoneme and a
sandhi-process of retroflexion, both represented graphemically in the same way.
In Norwegian,2 the so-called ‘retroflex rule’ merges clusters of apical alveolar
/r/3 or retroflex flap / / plus all laminal dentals /t, d, s, n, l/ into corresponding
retroflexes across morpheme and word boundaries. Examples from Kristoffersen
(2000: 96f.) are given in (3) (in Kristoffersen’s transcription).
(3)
input
/sr-t/
/bar-n/
Derivation /or-li/
Clitics
/brur-s/
/bær-n/
Compounds /or-tejn/
/or-da%/
Inflection
1
2
3
output of RR
[s]
[b]
[o.i]
[bu ]
[bæ]
[o.æjn]
[o.&%]
surt
baren
vårlig
brors
bœr han
vårtegn
vårdag
gloss
‘sour’AGR
‘bar’ DEF-SG
‘spring-like’
‘brother’ POSS
‘carry him!’
‘spring sign’
‘spring day’
Other systems employed to represent retroflexes in the literature on Australian languages are the usual
grapheme for coronals (d, n, l, r) with a subscript dot or an underlining. A further option is to
transcribe retroflexes with capital coronals, as used for example in the Dravidian language Kannada
(Schiffman 1983, Sridhar 1990).
The term “Norwegian” is used here and below to cover the variety that is defined as urban East
Norwegian speech, a notion explicated for example by Vanvik (1972, 1973), Endresen (1974), and
Kristoffersen (2000).
The underlying apical alveolar /r/ is realized as a tap [] in Norwegian (Kristoffersen 2000: 24).
84
Chapter 4
The geographical domain of the retroflex rule does not hold for the whole of
Norway, but extends only over the eastern part of South Norway all the way up
north to the Russian border (Kristoffersen 2000: 88). This area coincides with that of
the spread of the apical rhotic, which led some scholars, such as Torp (2001), to
propose that retroflexion can only occur with coronal /r/, whereas the uvular /'/,
which is used in the remaining part of the country, blocks retroflexion.
Swedish has a similar rule of retroflexion, which is sometimes called postalveolarization or supradentalization (Eliasson 1986: 278). Examples of this process
are given in (4) (in Eliasson’s transcription).
(4)
input
/før−t/
/før−s/
Derivation
/før−tal/
/før−sorj/
Compounds /før−t.r/
/før−sal/
Across words /før t.n/
/før sen/
Inflection
output
[fœ+]
[fœ+ ]
[fœ+,l]
[fœ+, -rj]
[,fœ+/.r]
[,fœ+/ l]
[fœ+,0n]
[fœ+, en]
fört
förs
förtal
försorg
förtur
försal
för tunn
för sen
gloss
‘brought’ SUP
‘is brought’ PASS
‘slander’
‘taking care’
‘priority’
‘entrance hall’
‘too thin’
‘too late’
According to Eliasson (1986: 282), the sandhi rule of retroflexion in Swedish is
sensitive to the type of boundary between the /r/ and the dental. The higher up in the
prosodic hierarchy the two categories are, the less likely retroflexion occurs across
their boundary.
Both Norwegian and Swedish also have retroflexes which cannot be the result
of the merger in (4) because no morpheme boundary occurs between /r/ and the
dental, e.g. kart/karta [kh a(a)]4 ‘map’, or kors [kh - ] ‘cross’. In most cases these
retroflexes derive historically from the same retroflexion rule as those described
above. Due to this common origin, Endresen (1974) among others proposes that
there are no underlying retroflex phonemes and that all retroflexes are derived via
the same rule. Since the rule is not triggered by a morpheme boundary in examples
like /kh-rs/, Endresen assumes that it applies across the board. This assumption is
problematic for the following reasons. First of all, not all word-internal retroflexes in
Norwegian can be derived from rhotic plus dentals: the retroflex fricative in words
like skje [ e] ‘spoon’, and the retroflex flap in words like sol [su ] ‘sun’
(Kristoffersen 2000: 23 and 24, respectively) have a different historical origin. To
account for these forms with a context-free retroflexion rule, one has to assume the
underlying forms /rse/ and /surr/, respectively, which would violate the sonority
sequency generalization.
Furthermore, the graphemic sequence rd is often not retroflexed, and some
words exist that show variation between retroflexion and non-retroflexion
(Kristoffersen 2000: 89). Bård, for instance, a male Christian name, can be
pronounced as [bo&] or [bo], whereas other words can only be pronounced with
the retroflex voiced stop, e.g. [f-.,&i] fordi ‘because’ or [g.,&in] gardin ‘curtain’.
4
The form with the /a/ is the Swedish, that without /a/ is the Norwegian.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
85
In addition, the retroflexion of rd is stress-dependent: after stressed vowels one finds
almost only [d], after unstressed vowel only [&], see for example the alternations:
garde ‘guard’ [,g.d1] and derived gardist ‘guardsman’ [g.,&ist], where the stress
is moved to the suffix. /rd/ after long vowels can be pronounced as so-called ‘thick l’
[ ], e.g. ard ‘plough’ [ ] (Popperwell 1963: 83).
For these reasons, the present study follows Kristoffersen (2000) among others
in assuming an underlying retroflex phoneme and a sandhi rule of retroflexion that
applies across morpheme and word boundaries in both languages.
Retroflexion does not necessarily have to be induced by an apical rhotic, as the
following description of a Swedish dialect suggests. Svantesson (2001) describes the
phoneme inventory of his own southern Standard Swedish idiolect. Instead of the
apical trill or fricative, this variety has a uvular fricative [2], like all Southern
Swedish dialects. Svantesson’s idiolect shows deletion of the rhotic when occurring
in coda position as the first member of a cluster. A following dental consonant is
retracted, and becomes ‘alveolar’ in Svantesson’s terms (p. 157), which he
represents as /t, d, s, n, l/. His study unfortunately does not contain any articulatory
data to infer the exact articulation of these retracted segments, and no other study on
the articulation of Southern Swedish dentals and alveolars could be found to serve
this purpose. However, the description of these segments allows the interpretation
that they are non-posterior retroflexes (see the definition in section 2.3.5): the
segments in question cause preceding vowels to lower and rhotacize, and therefore
behave phonologically like retroflexes, cf. sections 4.3.2.2 and 4.4 below. The
retraction of dentals in Svantesson’s data suggests that the retraction of coronals in
r-context is not caused by coronal rhotics only, but can be triggered by uvular
rhotics as well. It remains open for future articulatory studies to determine whether
the resulting segments are indeed retroflexes (i.e., apical post-alveolars). If that were
the case, it would show that retroflexion can be induced by non-coronal rhotics, too.
Furthermore, it would provide evidence against claims that retroflexion does not
occur in regions with a uvular rhotic, as proposed by Torp (2001) and others.
Australian languages are another language-family that shows retroflexion in a
rhotic context. Bhat (1973: 35) points out that in these languages the
morphophonemic sequences of /r/ + /t/ are generally realized as retroflexes. An
example of this is Ndjebbana (McKay 2000: 175), spoken in Central Anrhem Land.
Ndjebbana has a contraction process for the ‘realis prefix’ rra [ra] followed by a
root with an initial apical alveolar /n/: this may show vowel deletion to /rn/ and
subsequent realization as a retroflex nasal [], see the example in (5).
(5)
ba-ra-nmaramao-4a
( > ba-rnmaramao-4a )
3rd PS-AUGS-RE-swim-REM
>
bamaramao4a
‘they swam’
Another example of morphophonemic retroflexion is Watjarri (Douglas 1981),
spoken in South-Western Australia, which has the ergative and locative affixes –tu
and –ta, respectively, which change their initial apical alveolar /t/ into a retroflex
when the preceding stem ends in an alveolar /r/. An example for this process is the
sequence /maju makur/ ‘the three children’, which changes into [maju makua]
86
Chapter 4
‘on the three children’ when locative -ta is added (Douglas 1981: 208). Note that the
rhotic does not have to be retroflex.
Diachronically, old Indo-Aryan (i.e. Sanskrit) developed a retroflex fricative
partly by a rhotic /r/ which changed a following /s/ into a retroflex. For discussion
see section 4.3.4 on the ruki rule below. Another sub-branch of the Indo-Iranian
language family, namely Iranian, shows a similar retroflexion process. Several
Modern Iranian languages developed retroflexes via rhotics.5 In the Iranian language
Yidgha, spoken in Pakistan, the retroflex [] developed from the sequence /rn/, and
the phoneme [ ] from /r/ (Bhat 1973: 34, Skjærvø 1989c: 411). Interestingly, /rt/
developed into [ ], not into a retroflex stop.6 Yidgha also introduced the phoneme [ ]
via the sequences /str/ and /sr/ (Skjærvø 1989c: 413), an instance of regressive
influence of a rhotic. This development is rather common in Modern East Iranian
languages. Pashto, for example, shows exactly the same process causing the
retroflex fricative to become a phoneme, as corresponding words in the predecessing
language Avestan indicate: Pashto [ a] ‘good’ and [ na] ‘hip bone’ correspond to
Avestan [srao] and [sraoni], respectively (Morgenstierne 1927). Further examples
for such a development can be found in Wakh67, Sangl677c86,7 and Is8kas8m67 (Payne 1989),
and in Mun9:67 (Skjærvø 1989a). The Indo-Aryan language Sindhi developed
retroflexes from dentals preceding r, but here the rhotic remained: Old-Indo-Aryan
traya- ‘three’ or draka ‘grape’ changed to Sindhi [re] and [&rakha] (Masica 1991:
210).
A rhotic causing a segment to the right to change into a retroflex as in the
Iranian Pashto and the Indo-Aryan Sindhi is a process that occurs crosslinguistically less often than a rhotic causing a change of the segment to its left (as in
Norwegian, Swedish, Ndjebbana, and Watjarri). A further instance for this latter
assimilatory direction is Cham, spoken in Vietnam (Bhat 1973: 36), in which
retroflexes were diachronically introduced via an anterior coronal plus following /r/.
The changes that occurred here were: tr > , tr’ > ’, and sr > . A similar process
can be observed in some Southern dialects of the Dravidian language Tamil, which
realize literary Tamil /nr/ as [&], e.g. anru ‘that day’ is [a&u] in Ceylon Tamil, or
/munru/ ‘three’ [mu&u] (Zvelebil 1970: 173).
In some central African languages, which sometimes have retroflexion of the
voiced coronal stop, this retroflexion is triggered partly by a following /r/ as for
example in the Nilo-Saharan Lugbara, where the stops in tr and dr can be
retroflexed without deletion of the rhotic (Bhat 1973: 40).
The diachronic development of a retroflex from a following rhotic can be
observed in the Sino-Tibetan language Tibetan and its closely related neighbouring
languages. Bhat (1973: 34) gives examples of this development, which took place in
syllable-initial consonant clusters, see (6).
5
6
In some of the Iranian languages, the introduction of a retroflex via a sequence of rhotic plus dental
was accompanied by a change in manner. Sangl677c867, for example, spoken in Tadz:ikistan, introduced a
retroflex lateral from the sequence *rt (Payne 1989: 424).
The retroflex stops /, &/ occur in Yidgha in loanwords only (Skjærvø 1989c: 411).
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
(6)
Class. Tibetan
/%rod-pa/
/drun%-du/
/phru-%u/
/krad-pa/
/khron-pa/
/skra/
/%ru/
/bran/
gloss
‘belly’
‘before’
‘child’
‘leather’
‘a spring’
‘hair’
‘ship’
‘slave’
Modern languages
Central Tibetan
Central Tibetan
Central Tibetan
Spiti
Spiti
Jad
Jad
Jad
87
/&>ø-pa/
/un%-du/
/hu-%u/
/ad-pa/
/hon-pa/
/a/
/u/
/an/
It is interesting to note that in these examples not only coronals plus rhotic changed
into a retroflex, but also velars and labials plus rhotic.
The examples described in this section show several characteristics of
retroflexion in a rhotic context. First of all, the segment to be changed in the rhotic
environment does not have to be an apical coronal; it can be laminal, as in
Norwegian and Swedish, or even non-coronal, as the developments in the Tibetan
languages suggest. Secondly, there is some evidence that the rhotic causing the
change can be non-coronal, as Svantesson’s dialect of Southern Swedish show.
Thirdly, the direction of influence is not restricted, either; the rhotic might precede
or follow the target segment, though retroflexion via preceding rhotics seems to
prevail.
4.1.2 Phonetic grounding
Two possible phonetic explanations are proposed here for the change of nonretroflex to retroflex caused by a rhotic, both exemplified in (7) (again, t stands for
an apical dental/alveolar, and for a retroflex).
(7)
(a)
(b)
rt > t > > or tr > t > >
rt >
or tr >
articulatory explanation
perceptual explanation
In (7a), which exemplifies a development in several stages, the first stage is a
variation in the place of articulation of a rhotic from dental/alveolar to retroflex.
Evidence for such a variation can be found in the fact that quite a number of
languages allow a retroflex variant along with their more widespread other rhotic
articulation(s). This variant can be either areally restricted, as in British English,
where a retroflex [?] is spoken in the South-Western counties of Cornwall, Devon,
Somerset, Wiltshire, and Hampshire (Wakelin 1972: 98),7 or positionally restricted,
as in Dutch, where a retroflex variant of the usually apical rhotic can occur in wordfinal position, according to Goeman & Van de Velde (1999: 99). An example of
such a non-restricted change is the Sanskrit retroflex r which originates from IndoEuropean (presumably) alveolar /r/.
Hall (1997a: 215 footnote 21) suggests that an alveolar approximant [@] can
easily change to a retroflex approximant [?] because the tongue tip is not inhibited in
7
Wakelin transcribes the British English retroflex rhotic as the flap [ ], whereas Ball & Rahilly (1999:
125) refer to this sound as the retroflex approximant [?].
88
Chapter 4
any way during the articulation of the approximant and therefore can retract or even
curl backwards. This can result in a retroflex variant. It seems more likely, however,
that a change from alveolar to retroflex trill occurs to enhance the perceptual cue of
a lowered F3 for the rhotic (see section 3.6 on cue enhancement). Both Hall’s and
my explanation can account for the context free change of an alveolar /r/ to a
retroflex one in American English and, furthermore, for the fact that approximants
are the only retroflexes in a number of languages (Bhat 1973).
In some languages, the development of a retroflex variant is followed by
assimilation of a non-retroflex adjacent to the retroflex rhotic, see the second stage
in (7a), with subsequent drop of the rhotic gesture. Such a development is likely to
have occurred in languages where the rhotic is apical and thus can be easily
retroflexed. Furthermore, it could have taken place in the diachronic development of
retroflexion, where several stages of development are possible. Thus, (7a) can be
assumed for the diachronic development of retroflexes in Indo-Aryan and IndoIranian, and for the retroflex phonemes in Norwegian and Swedish. Furthermore, the
articulatory explanation can account for the above-mentioned development from /rt/
into [ ] in Yidgha, where instead of rhotic deletion as assumed in (7a), the stop was
deleted. Languages which developed a retroflex in a rhotic context without
subsequent deletion of the rhotic, such as Sindhi and Lugbara described above, are
further evidence for the staged development as proposed in (7a), since these
languages can be assumed not to have undergone the last stage of the development,
the rhotic deletion.
The second explanation, given in (7b), is not based on articulation as in (7a).
Instead, it refers to the acoustic similarity of rhotic plus non-retroflex and retroflex.
Both rhotic and retroflexes share a low third formant (Lindau 1985, Stevens 1998),8
which could be re-analysed or misparsed by the language learner as belonging to the
adjacent coronal segment (specified as [+1F2]), a process Ohala (1993: 89f.) calls
“false association parsing error”. A schema illustrating this misparsing is given in
figure 4.1.
V
r
t
F1, F2
[–2 F3]
[+1 F2]
V
segments the speaker tries to convey
phonetic cues of the segments
segments as parsed by the listener
Figure 4.1 Reanalysis of the sequence vowel–rhotic–anterior coronal as vowel plus
retroflex (the phonetic cues being, of course, not complete).
8
Not all rhotics show a lowered F3, though. This is usually a characteristic of coronal rhotics, whereas
uvular rhotics show a high F3 (Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996: 244). Ladefoged and Maddieson (ibid.)
even mention two cases where retroflex approximant rhotics have a high F3, namely Arrernte and
Hausa.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
89
The auditory re-analysis of the rhotic cue makes it possible to economize on the
gesture of the rhotic, as it is an additional gesture without perceivable acoustic
consequences. This explains the drop of the rhotic after causing the retroflexion, as
observable in Scandinavian or Indo-Iranian. Evidence for such a reanalysis of rhotic
plus dental/alveolar as a retroflex is the fact that sequences of vowel-rhoticdental/alveolar are often misperceived as vowel-retroflex sequences (Sharpe 1982:
17).
This second explanation of acoustically similar cues can account for the
synchronic processes described in section 4.1.1, as they show a segmental change
without any intermediate stages. Furthermore, it can be assumed to account for the
retroflexion in Svantesson’s (2001) southern Standard Swedish, which is triggered
by a uvular rhotic and thus excludes an articulatory assimilation. Both explanations
of retroflexion in a rhotic context are formalized in an OT framework in section
6.3.1. It was observed that retroflexion after rhotics is more common than
retroflexion before rhotics. This might be explained by the asymmetrical spread of
retroflex transitional cues, which have slightly stronger VC cues than CV cues (as
described in section 3.5), and are thus more influenced by preceding segments and
their cues.
Special attention should be given to the examples in (6) above on the
developments from Classical Tibetan, where retroflexion of non-coronals took
place. The occurring change of place of articulation from labial or velar to postalveolar cannot be accounted for by the explanations proposed so far. For this
special development, two explanations can be offered. The first one is based again
on perception. The acoustic similarity between velars, labials, and retroflexes (see
section 3.3.2 and 5.1.1 on Jakobson, Fant & Halle’s 1952 feature ‘flat’) might have
caused a reanalysis of velar or labial (plus rhotic) as a retroflex. A second
explanation is to assume that there was an intermediate development in Classical
Tibetan where the velar or labial changed to an anterior coronal, and only in a
subsequent process this anterior coronal followed by a rhotic changed to a retroflex,
as in the processes explained above in (7). Such a development would look like /%r/
> /dr/ > /&/. This assumption still cannot explain why a change of labial or velar to
anterior coronal should take place. Hence, the explanation based on perceptual
similarity between retroflex and velar/labial is preferred here.
Bhat (p. 44) points out that the change from Classical Tibetan was paralleled
by the reduction of other initial consonant clusters, where a two- or three-segmental
onset was reduced to the last segment of the cluster. This indicates that it might have
been the rhotic that changed into a retroflex after the velar, with a subsequent drop
of the velar, i.e. a development as described in (7a). Further studies on the
diachronic development of initial retroflexes in Tibetan languages are necessary to
answer the question of how the retroflex segments emerged in Tibetan. This process
will not be further dealt with in chapter 6.
90
Chapter 4
4.2 Patterning with back vowels
In this section the connection between retroflexes and back vowels as given under
(1b) is illustrated, where the category of “back vowels” sometimes includes the low
vowel /a/, but is often restricted to /u/. Retroflexion in a back vowel context was
already the topic of previous studies, among which Bhat (1973), and the data and
phonetic grounding of this process given below is largely in line with these studies.
Contrary to these former descriptions, it is claimed here that the articulation of
retroflexes is closer to that of the mid back vowel [o] than to that of [u], and the fact
that [u] is more often the trigger of retroflexion can be accounted for by the larger
perceptual similarity between a retroflex and the vowel [u].
The examples in 4.2.1 are mainly from Australian languages, while some
instances of Indo-Aryan and American Indian languages are also included. A
phonetic explanation for this affinity between back vowels and retroflexes based on
articulation is given in section 4.2.2.
4.2.1 Examples
In his description of Australian languages, Dixon (1980) points out that the
languages with only one apical (alveolar) of Eastern Australia often have a retroflex
allophone after a back vowel. Evidence for the change of alveolars into retroflexes
in back vowel contexts comes from the diachronic development of Australian
languages. Dixon (1980: 155) reconstructs a single alveolar series of stops, nasals,
and laterals for proto-Australian. Cognate sets in languages with one apical (dental
or alveolar) and two apicals (dental/alveolar and retroflex) suggest that this single
alveolar series in proto-Australian had retroflex allophones after /u/ and alveolar
allophones elsewhere. Several West-Australian languages developed contrastive
sequences such as [ &], [i&] and [u d], which led to a phonological distinction
between apical alveolar and retroflex. This contrast spread to cover almost all
languages in the west and the centre. Eastern Australian languages retained one
apical, with several of them showing an allophonic distribution as illustrated above.9
Nyawaygi, spoken on the coast of Queensland between Ingham and Townsville,
underwent a diachronic change of apical alveolar to retroflex in back vowel context
(Dixon 1983: 449f.). Intervocalically the plosive /d/ became the retroflex flap [ ]
before u and the apical [r] before other vowels.10 The initial d changed to [r], but as
this process does not involve a retroflex, it will not be further discussed here. The
developments of retroflex and apical flaps can be seen when comparing the language
Nyawaygi to its neighbour Wargamay, which retained the original contrasts, see (8).
9
10
Dixon (1980: 156) finds evidence for his hypothesis of one alveolar series in proto-Australian in the
statistical distribution of phonemes in modern Australian languages. The dictionary of Pitjantjatjara
for example shows that after /u/ 53 percent of the apicals are retroflex, whereas after /a/ or /i/ only 39
percent are retroflex.
Nyawaygi still has a voiced apical stop in the consonant cluster [nd], but this can be treated as an
allophone of the retroflex flap, see Dixon (1980: 148).
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
(8)
(a)
(b)
(c)
Wargamay
wudu
%idul
%adala
badi
dubi
Nyawaygi
wu u
%i ul
%arala
bari
rubi
91
gloss
‘nose’
‘cold’
‘dry’
‘to cry’
‘worm’
In (8a) the Wargamay stop /d/ before the back vowel corresponds to the retroflex
flap / / in Nyawaygi. (8b) shows that before non-back vowels Nyawaygi has an
alveolar. The example in (8c) illustrates that word-initially no retroflex occurs (but a
change towards a rhotic).
Even Australian languages that do have apical alveolar and retroflex phonemes
sometimes allow a retroflex allophone of the apical alveolar in the /u/ context. The
two closely related languages Margany and Gunya spoken in Queensland show a
retraction of the alveolar nasal following /u/, so that /%uni/, ‘to hit’ sounds like
[%ui], according to Breen (1981: 288).
The Yadhaykenu dialect of Uradhi, a Northern-Paman Australian language
spoken in Queensland, has an apical alveolar lateral which is realized as retroflex
flap [&] when following the long back vowels [a] or [u], see example in (9a) (from
Crowley 1983: 317). When following a short back vowel, lateral and flap are in free
variation, see (9b).
(9)
(a)
(b)
/analu/
/ipula/
[ana&u4]
[ipula4] ~ [ipu&a4]
‘come’-PRES
2non-SG-NOM
In the Angkamuthi dialect of Uradhi, the retroflex plosive shows additional frication
noise in the form of a rhotic (described by Crowley 1983: 316 as rhotic release).
This phenomenon is restricted to contexts where a back vowel follows, see (10).11
(10)
/antu/
/wuntu/
[aru4]
[wuru]
‘canoe’
‘crooked’
Besides Australian, several Indo-European languages display a general pattern
of retroflexion in a back vowel context. Sinhala, an Indo-Aryan language spoken in
Sri Lanka, has a retroflex-dental distinction among apical consonants. Phonetically,
the retroflex series varies in place of articulation from retroflex to alveolar (Gair &
Paolillo 1997: 11). This variation is phonologically conditioned, so that retroflex
consonants are pronounced as retroflex when preceded or followed by back vowels,
and as alveolar in most other environments (Karunatillake 1992).12 In Sri Lankan
Portuguese Creole, the dental alveolar nasal and lateral have retroflex allophones
11
12
A rhotic or retroflex release also occurs in other Australian languages, as in the Daly language
Marrithiyel, where /ma i/ ‘belly’ can be pronounced either as [maBi] or [maB?i] (Evans 1995: 739).
Evans, though transcribing it with an additional rhotic, describes this phenomenon as r-colouring of
the vowel, which could also be interpreted as retroflexion of the vowel, cf. section 4.5.
The question arises why this series is described as ‘retroflex’ with alveolar allophones, and not the
other way round, as the occurrence of the retroflex allophone is far more restricted than that of the
alveolar allophone. A reason is probably that this series corresponds etymologically to the retroflex
consonants of other Indo-Aryan languages.
92
Chapter 4
after the non-high back vowels [o, 1, a], for instance [aima] ‘animal’ (Hume &
Tserdanelis 2002). Interestingly, the remaining dental-alveolars [t d s z r] do not
have such allophones, and the back high vowel [u] is not included in the context.
Some American Indian languages also show an affinity between retroflex
sounds and back vowels. In the Molinos dialect of Mixtec, an Oto-Manguean
language spoken in Molinos in the Tlaxiaco District of Mexico, the post-alveolar
fricatives /, D/ have retroflex varieties when preceding /a, o, u/, for example zoo
[ oEo] ‘rope’ or za a [ oaF] ‘very’ (Hunter & Pike 1969: 29). When a front vowel
follows, the palato-alveolar is realized as such, e.g. zii [iF] ‘side’.13 Further Southern
American languages having a fricative or an affricate which is changed into a
retroflexed one while occurring before back vowels are Acoma, Mazatec, and
O’odam (Bhat 1974a: 234). Certain Alaskan languages of the Athapaskan family are
described to show a similar behaviour (in the same source).
In the Indonesian language Tolitoli (from the Austronesian language family)
an alveolar lateral approximant [l] is in complementary distribution with a retroflex
lateral flap [] (Himmelmann 1991). The data indicate that the retroflex lateral
occurs after the back vowels, [o, u, a], see (11a).
(11) (a) mo[]ogo
u[]ag
to[]ito[]i
lelemba[]an
(b) membembe[l]an
[l]abia
kiki[l]o
‘wash hands’
‘snake’
‘Tolitoli’
‘to carry’
‘to tremble’
‘sago’
‘firefly’
The alveolar lateral seems to occur elsewhere, see (11b).
4.2.2 Phonetic grounding
The phonetic motivation of the affinity between retroflexion and back vowels is both
articulatory and acoustic. Back vowels, as their name indicates, are articulated with
a backed, i.e. retracted, tongue body, just as retroflexes are, recall the description of
retraction as a feature of retroflexes in section 2.3.4. Thus, the cooccurrence of
retroflexes in a back vowel context can be seen as a coarticulation process of the
dental/alveolar towards the tongue body shape of the back vowels. Bhat (1974a)
proposed a similar explanation for the affinity of back vowels and retroflexes.
Since the vowel [o] is articulated with a slightly less raised tongue back and
with more retraction (see figure 4.2), it seems as if the back mid vowel is
articulatorily closer to the retroflex than [u].14
13
14
It coul be argued that the underlying fricative phoneme in Molinos Mixtec is actually retroflex with a
palato-alveolar variant in a front vowel context. One indication for this is the writing convention
according to which the fricative plus /i/ followed by another vowel is realized as [], e.g. ziau [aFEuF]
‘fifteen’ (Hunter & Pike 1969: 31).
The exact locations of [o] and [u] differ according to the language under investigation. The x-ray
tracings of the two vowels in the Akyem dialect of Akan (a Niger-Congo language spoken in Ghana)
in Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996: 301) are similar to the cardinal vowels in figure 4.2 on the left. The
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
Figure 4.2
93
Comparison of the tongue back positions for the vowels [o] and [u] (based on
x-ray tracings of the cardinal vowels by Catford 1988: 128) on the left with that
of a retroflex stop (based on x-ray tracings of a Tamil stop by Ladefoged &
Maddieson 1996: 27).
However, only one language could be found that has retroflexion in an [o] context to
the exclusion of [u], namely Sri Lankan Portuguese Creole, as illustrated in 4.2.1
above. This strongly suggests that this process is not purely articulatorily motivated.
A further, non-articulatory explanation might be that both retroflexion and
back vowels have a lowered F3.15 The development of retroflexion next to back
vowels could be caused by a reassociation of the acoustic cues of backness from
vowel to consonant, similar to that of the reassociation of rhotic cues illustrated in
section 4.1.2. This explanation is expected to hold especially for back, rounded
vowel context, as rounded back vowels show a particularly low F3. Some languages
such as Molinos Mixtec or the Yadhaykenu dialect of Uradhi show retroflexion of
consonants with a following back vowel, which indicates that the CV transitional
cues for retroflexes are important and distinctive enough to allow such a reassociation, recall the discussion of asymmetricality of retroflex cues in section 3.5.
Looking at the manner of articulation of those segments that change into a
retroflex in the languages discussed here, it is mainly nasal, lateral, and rhotic
dentals/alveolars that are influenced as the single segments in languages by the back
vowel context. In the Australian languages, this restriction holds for Nyawaygi,
Margany, and Gunya; furthermore, it can be observed in Sri Lankan Portuguese
Creole and in the Indonesian Tolitoli. In some languages, the whole dental/alveolar
series is retroflexed, as in Sinhala and the Angkamuthi dialect of Uradhi. An account
for this can be given by the different inherent cues of the manner classes involved.
Sonorants like nasals, laterals, and rhotics have continuous formants just like the
adjacent vowels. Instead of a clearcut borderline between the vowel formants and
those of the sonorants, a smooth transition is given in these segment sequences. It is
therefore more likely that a hearer misaligns a low F3 in the context of liquids than
15
tracings of these vowels in Even (Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996: 307), a Tungus language of NorthCentral Siberia, however, show that both vowels have the same degree of backing and differ only in
the degree of lowering. This interaction of retroflexes with the two back vowels is a topic that has to
be left open for future research.
The acoustic cues shared by retroflexion and back vowels certainly cannot be treated as independent of
the articulatory similarity: retraction of the tongue body causes a lowering of F3, as described in
section 3.2.4.
94
Chapter 4
in that of for instance a stop, where only short vowel transitions are available
followed by the silence of the stop closure. For a formal account of this difference
see section 6.3.2.
One systematic exception to this observation are American Indian languages
such as Molinos Mixtec and Mazatec (Oto-Manguan languages), Acoma (Keresan
language), and O’odam (Uto-Aztecan) in which a fricative or affricate are the only
segments to be retroflexed in a back vowel context. This is in line with the general
tendency of American Indian languages to show retroflexion of fricatives and
affricates only.
Further instances of the interaction of retroflex segments with back vowels are
given in subsection 4.3.2.1 below where front vowels are changed into back ones in
a retroflex environment due to a dislike of the gestures of retroflexion and front
vowels.
4.3 Non-occurrence of retroflexes in front vowel context
As was already indicated in section 4.2.2, the front vowel context is avoided by
retroflex segments. This avoidance can be observed in a large number of languages,
but it is not a universal principle as there are languages where retroflexes do occur in
a front vowel context. The dispreference of retroflexes for the front vowel context is
realized in a number of avoidance strategies, recall the description in (1c) which
gave two outputs, namely [it] or [ ]. These two illustrate the two main strategies
that languages employ to avoid the dispreferred sequence, namely a change of the
retroflex into an anterior coronal (i.e. the front high vowel gesture dominates),
repeated here in (12), and to change the front vowel (i.e. the retroflex gesture
dominates). The change of the front vowel can take on several forms: (13a) vowel
retraction, (13b) vowel lowering, (13c) vowel diphthongization, or (13d) vowel
rounding.
(12)
(13) (a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
(14) (a)
(b)
(c)
input
/i/
/i/
/G/
/i/
/i/
/j/
/j/
/j/
output
[it]
[ ] or [.]
[æ]
[i1]
[y]
[tj]
[]
[j]
process
de-retroflexion
retraction
lowering
diphthongization (schwa insertion)
rounding
de-retroflexion
de-palatalization
separate palatal realization
Due to the articulatory similarity between high front vowels and the front glide /j/,
the present illustration includes a short discussion on the incompatibility of
retroflexes with secondary palatalization, recall the development of this assumption
in section 2.5. In (14) the avoidance strategies for secondary palatalization are given:
a change into a non-retroflex coronal with secondary palatalization (14a), into a nonpalatalized retroflex (14b), or realization as a separate palatal glide (14c).
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
95
The examples given below are subdivided accordingly: section 4.3.1 deals
with processes such as (12), section 4.3.2 with processes like (13), and section
4.3.2.5 with those in (14). Mandarin, Polish and Russian are shown to apply one of
these strategies, and thus provide phonological evidence for the retroflex status of
their fricatives (recall discussion in 2.4): Mandarin retroflexes occur only in a nonfront vowel context, see 4.3.1 below, and Polish and Russian centralize a following
/i/, cf. section 4.3.2.1. The phonetic grounding of the avoidance strategies is
discussed in section 4.3.3. Section 4.3.4 deals with a counterexample for front vowel
avoidance, namely the ruki rule, where retroflexion occurs precisely in front vowel
context.
4.3.1 De-retroflexion in front vowel context
In section 4.2 it was shown mainly for Australian languages that retroflexes are
often in complementary distribution with other coronal segments (apical alveolars
for example), where the retroflex consonants occur in a back vowel context, and the
other coronal series in a front vowel context. Several other languages also show this
pattern. In Karok, for example, a Northern Hokan language spoken in Northwestern
California (around the Klamath river), the retroflex segments [ , ] are in (near)
complementary distribution with the palato-alveolars [, t].16 The retroflex fricative
and affricate do not occur after the front vowel [i] even if other consonants
intervene, while in this same environment palato-alveolars do occur, see the
examples in (15) with retroflexes in the first column and palato-alveolars in the
second (from Bright 1957).17
(15) [ ara]
[Ea ak]
[Ea ]
‘bread’
‘on a rock’
‘water’
[pikip]
[tujip]
[ti]
‘shadow’
‘mountain’
‘younger sister’
As a result of this complementary distribution, morphological processes cause a
change from retroflex to palato-alveolar after a sequence of front vowel and /p/, see
(16a), and from palato-alveolar to retroflex after a mid or back vowel, see (16b).
(16) (a)
(b)
/Earip+ uru/
/p1hip+ uruk/
/mu+ipuka/
/Eu+ikak/
[Earipuru]
[p1hipuruk]
[mu puka]
[Eu kak]
‘to cut a strip off’
‘under the pepperwood’
‘his money’
‘he jumps’
Acoma, a Keres language spoken in New Mexico, shows a similar
complementary distribution, here the retroflexes change to alveolar or palatoalveolar before front vowels (Miller 1965). The same process occurs in Molinos
16
17
Exceptions to this allophonic distribution are some loanwords which allow a front vowel-retroflex
sequence, e.g. [ ikspi ] ‘six bits’, and reduplicated forms such as e.g. [ta in ir] ‘to brush repeatedly’
from /ta ir/ (Bright 1957: 44). Bright further reports that nouns plus a possessive prefix allow some
idiolectal variation, thus /nani- ara/ ‘my bread’ can be realized either as [naniara] or [nani ara].
The restriction for retroflexes seems also to hold before non-high front vowels, as no words
containing a sequence retroflex-front vowel could be found in the data.
96
Chapter 4
Mixtec (Hunter & Pike 1969). Another Amerindian example is the Pano-Tacanan
language Chácobo spoken in Bolivia, in which the retroflex [ ] does not surface in
words when preceded or followed by the front vowel [i]. In this case, the palatoalveolar [] occurs (Prost 1967: 62).
Further evidence for the cross-linguistic validity of retroflexes dispreferring
the front vowel context comes from the Sino-Tibetan language group. Khonoma
Angami, a Tibeto-Burman language spoken in the Naja Hills in the North-Eastern
parts of India, has a retroflex approximant [?] and a voiceless counterpart of this.
According to Blankenship, Ladefoged, Bhaskararao, & Chase (1993: 132), the
retroflex approximant is laminal before high vowels and subapical otherwise. The
laminality indicates that this allophone is not retroflex, recall the retroflex criterion
of apicality as introduced in section 2.3.1.18
In several Chinese dialects the retroflex fricative and affricate series (and also
the dentals and velars) do not occur before a high front vowel, whereas the alveolopalatals occur only in these positions (Yip 1996). Table 4.1 illustrates these
phonotactic restrictions.
Table 4.1
Cooccurrence restrictions in Mandarin and other Chinese dialects (Yip 1996).
,
tI, I
_aj
_u
_i
_y
√
*
√
*
*
√
*
√
This evidence for the incompatibility of Chinese post-alveolar fricatives and
affricates supports the claim made in chapter 2 that these sounds are retroflex on
articulatory grounds.
Diachronic evidence for the avoidance of retroflexes in a front vowel context
can be found in the Indo-Aryan language Gujarati, where [ ] became alveolo-palatal
[I] before front vowels, but stayed retroflex elsewhere (Pandit 1954).
4.3.2 Change of front vowels in retroflex context
This section discusses and exemplifies several processes of how a front vowel is
changed in a retroflex context. The processes found in natural languages are
retraction, lowering, diphthongization, and rounding of front vowels, see (17a) – (d).
(17)
(a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
18
input
/i/
/G/
/i/
/i/
output
[ ] or [.]
[æ]
[i1]
[y]
process
retraction
lowering
diphthongization (schwa insertion)
rounding
As coronal rhotics are almost always apical (see Hall 2000a), the segment occurring before high front
vowels is expected to show a manner change towards a non-rhotic, as well. No indication for this
could be found in Blankenship et al.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
97
Vowel retraction, lowering, diphthongization, and rounding will be illustrated in
sections 4.3.2.1, to 4.3.2.4, respectively. Often more than one of these processes
occurs simultaneously in one language.
4.3.2.1 Retraction of front vowels
The first avoidance strategy modifying the vowel, namely retraction of front vowels
(17a), is observable in the Dravidian vowel system. Zvelebil (1970: 38) notes that
Irul5a shows retraction of the front vowel /i/ to the central [.] before a retroflex
consonant. Likewise, the front vowel /e/ is retracted to [J] in this environment. As a
consequence the high and mid back unrounded vowels (graphically represented as ï
and ë) were added as phonemes to the general Dravidian five-vowel system i, e, a, o,
u. The backing of front vowels can also be found in the South-Dravidian language
Kodagu (also called Kodava) spoken in the Coarg district (Zvelebil 1970, Emeneau
·
1970, Ebert 1996). Here the vowels /i/ and /e/ are backed before the retroflex
consonants / , & , , /. The phonetic representations of the outcome of this change
differ: Gnanadesikan (1994) uses the symbols [ ] and [K], whereas Ebert represents
them as [.] and [L], implying a more extreme retraction.19
Gnanadesikan (1994: 132) compares Kodagu with other Dravidian languages
·
to exemplify the change, given here with additional examples (based on Zvelebil
1970) in (18), where (a) involves backing in non-labial context, and (b) backing and
rounding after labials.
(18)
(a)
(b)
Kodagu
·
i
KkK
pu&i
pu&
po
Other Dravidian languages
ii
(Tamil, Malayalam, Kannada)
eu
(Tamil)
ke
pii
pi&u
pe
(Tamil)
(Tamil, Malayalam)
(Kannada)
(Tamil, Malayalam, Kannada)
‘to descend’
‘to say’ in Kodagu
·
‘to count’ in Tamil
‘to hear’
‘to catch hold’
‘to squeeze’
‘wife, female’
The backing of vowels after labials and before retroflexes can also be observed in
Colloquial Tamil, e.g. /vi&./ can be pronounced as [vK&.] (Zvelebil p. 47f.,
Bright 1975: 15).20 Toda, a Dravidian language spoken in the Nilgiri Hills in
Southern India, is a further example for a language that has a process of vowel
retraction in a retroflex context (Zvelebil 1970: 46). Here, Proto-Dravidian *i
changed to [.], e.g. ‘mountain’ is [t.] in Toda but [tiu] in Tamil, and Toda [k. ]
‘small’ is Tamil [ki u].
19
20
Zvelebil (1970) uses only the diacritic [¨] above i and e to symbolize their retraction, but gives no
indication for the amount of backing. Bright (1975: 16) explains that the retracted i is similar to the
underlying back [.].
Bright explicitly points out the difference between the two vowels in this example, the first one being
central, the second back.
98
Chapter 4
Several members of the Australian language family show vowel retraction. For
instance, Bunuba (Rumsey 2000), spoken in Western Australia, realizes the
phoneme /i/ as high central vowel [ ] before retroflexes, see (19).21
(19) /bi&i/
/%iii/
/dDi Lli/
[b &i]
[% i]
[dD Lli]
‘upper leg, thigh’
‘shoulder blade’
‘before, long ago’
Wembawemba, an Australian language spoken in Victoria (Hercus 1986: 17;
Flemming 2002: 91), shows retraction of front vowels: The mid front vowel /e/ is
retracted if preceding a retroflex, see the examples in (20).
(20) /pee /
[pMM ]
/meme il/ [mMmM il]
/we puk/ [wM puk]
‘teal duck’
‘large black cormorant’
‘tree trunk’
This vowel retraction is accompanied by vowel lowering as described in section
4.3.2.2 below. The high front vowel /i/ in Wembawemba undergoes rounding and
lowering before retroflexes, cf. section 4.3.2.4.
Vowel backing occurs also in the Micronesian language Ponapean (Rehg
1973, Gnanadesikan 1994: 133), where the front vowels /i/ and /e/ surface as backed
after retroflexes (and other consonants referred to as ‘back’ in Rehg), see (21a)
below. Back vowels remain unchanged in this environment, see (21b).
(21) (a)
K4
‘secret’
‘tight’
(b)
op
-4
‘lush’
‘burned’
The Chinese language Pingding (Lin 1989) has a process whereby the retroflex
lateral [] is inserted after the initial consonant of the stem to denote familiar usage
(originally this infix had diminutive meaning). The process triggers a change in the
following front vowel towards a back vowel, see (22a), or a loss of the front high
vowel, see (22b) (Lin 1989: 187).22
(22) (a)
(b)
/tI>yæF/
/ 1u tNyæ/
/tNi14/
[ts>ua]
[ 1u tsua]
[ts14]
‘circle’
‘handkerchief’
‘now’
Polish post-alveolar fricatives and affricates show a retraction of vowels
similar to that illustrated up to now, and thus give support to their articulatory
analysis as retroflexes in chapter 2. The process in Polish whereby /i/ surfaces as the
central high vowel [ ] after hard (i.e. velarized) dentals and retroflexes is called
‘retraction rule’ (Booij & Rubach 1987:16ff.; Rubach 1995: 858ff., Hall 1997: 44).
Examples of this rule are given in (23) with the verbalizing suffix /i/.
21
22
Interestingly, the retraction of front vowels is also triggered by velars in Bunuba, see e.g. /mi4Lli/
‘hand’ is realized as [m 4Lli] (Rumsey 2000: 44).
The cooccurring change from alveolo-palatal to alveolar affricate is due to the allophonic restriction of
alveolo-palatals to appear in front vowel contexts only, cf. 4.3.1 above.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
(23)
[tovaB tI]
[stra tI]
[mjaBdB tI]
towarzysz+y+cO
strasz+y+cO
miaz· dz· +y+cO
99
‘to accompany’
‘to frighten’
‘to squash’
The same behaviour of vowel retraction can be observed for the Russian postalveolar fricatives. Like the Polish segments they do not occur in front vowel
context. Only the central vowel /P/ is allowed after these sounds (Hamilton 1980),
see the occurring pronunciations in the first row of (24) with the impossible ones in
the second row.
(24)
[ lQ]
[B lQ]
*[ ilQ]
*[BilQ]
sil
zil
‘he sewed’
‘he lived’
Based on these data I assume a cooccurrence restriction for Russian that disallows
sequences of retroflex fricatives and front high vowels, phonologically supporting
the fricatives’ retroflex status.
4.3.2.2 Lowering of front vowels
Norwegian has a rule of e-lowering before retroflexes. Examples of this process are
given in (25), where the first column contains words with /G/ or /e/ followed by a
dental, and the second column contains an /G/ followed by a retroflex, where the
vowel is realized as [æ] (based on Kristoffersen 2000:14, 105f.).23
(25) [Gt] vett ‘intelligence’
[hGl%] helg ‘weekend’
[hel] hœl ‘heel’
[æ]
[hæ j]
[hæ ]
vert
helg
hœl
‘host’
less formal register
less formal register
With respect to [ ], Norwegian shows variation, cf. [hæ . 1] herse ‘to bully’ vs.
[hG . 1] hesje ‘haydrying rack’, but in most cases [æ] is found (ibid.).
In Svantesson’s (2001) Southern Swedish dialect, vowel lowering of [ø, , o,
G] (and the long counterparts) occurs before ‘retracted’ coronals, which is an
indication of the retroflex nature of these consonants (recall discussion in 4.1
above). Examples of this process are given in the first column of (26), compared to
unaffected vowels in non-retroflex environment, see the second column.
(26) (a) [hœ ]
[b0 ]
[læ ]
(b) [bœ1 ]
[k-1]
[væ]
(c) [lR ]
hörs
burs
lärs
börs
kårt
värt
Lars
‘is heard’
‘cage’s’
‘is learned’
‘purse’
‘short’
‘worth’
(name)
[høs]
[bs]
[lGs]
[løs]
[skot]
[vGt]
[las]
hös
bus
läs
löss
skott
vätt
las
‘hay’s’
‘mischief’
‘read’
‘lice’
‘shot’
‘wett’
‘was laid’
In (26a) the lowering of high and middle long vowels is illustrated. In (26b) the
respective short vowels are lowered. All the short vowels apart from [G] show
23
The apical tap [] also triggers vowel lowering, e.g. [tæ] tverr ‘cross’ (adj.), which is unexpected,
since it is not retroflex.
100
Chapter 4
additional schwa insertion. (26c) shows that the long [a] is raised in a retroflex
context whereas the short [a] remains unchanged, cf. kart [ka] ‘unripe fruit’.
In the Australian language Kayardild (Evans 1995: 58f.), a following retroflex
causes lowering and retroflexion of the high vowels [i] and [u]: birdiy [be &ej] ‘bad’
and kuru [ko .] ‘egg’. The two Pama-Nyungan languages Margany and Gunya
(Breen 1981: 289) also show lowering of both high vowels (plus retraction in the
case of the front vowel) before all retroflexes, see the examples in (27), where (a)
illustrates the change for the high front vowel, and (b) for the high back vowel.24
(27) (a)
/badbi&a/
/niki/
(b) /ju&i/
[bRb &R] ~ [bRb1&R]
[nik1]
[jo&i]
‘porcupine’
‘hot coal’
‘meat’
Lowering of vowels when adjacent to retroflexes also seems to occur in the
Dravidian languages Tamil, Malayalam, and Kannada. Here, standard forms with /o/
alternate with more substandard forms such as /u/, e.g. [kou] ~ [kuu]. Zvelebil
(1970: 63) mentions that there is some evidence that the forms with /u/ are older
than those with /o/, and thus vowel lowering before retroflexes took place in the
standard languages.
4.3.2.3 Diphthongization of front vowels
Diphthongization is a further strategy applied to avoid the sequence high front vowel
– retroflex. Schwa-insertion resulting in a diphthong occurs for example in the
Beijing dialect of Chinese. A high front vowel – retroflex sequence might occur in
Beijing by the morphological process of [?] suffixation (Lin 1989), which has the
same meaning of indicating familiar usage as the retroflex infix in Pingding
illustrated earlier in (22). If the stem ends in a front high vowel, then a schwa is
inserted between stem and suffix, see the examples in (28a) compared to those in
(28b) where no insertion takes place (from Lin p. 188).
(28) (a)
(b)
/phi + ?/
/y + ?/
/xua + ?/
[phi1?]
[y1?]
[xua?]
‘skin’
‘fish’
‘flower’
In the Australian language Gugada spoken in Queensland, the transitions of
vowels into the retroflex are lengthened, and the vowel quality is changed into a
lower, backer vowel (Platt 1972). This results in a schwa-like segment before the
retroflex. Platt transcribes the result as [T1].
In Svantesson’s (2001) Southern Swedish dialect, vowel diphthongization of
the high vowels [i, y, u] (long and short) and the mid vowels [e, ø, o] occurs before
all retroflexes.25 These diphthongized vowels are realized with the same quality as
their non-diphthongized equivalents, but with a schwa-like offglide of the vowel.
Examples of this are in (29) (Svantesson 2001: 157), with non-diphthongized
equivalents in the second column.
24
25
The second example, /niki/, exists only in Margany, the other two in both languages.
Recall the discussion in section 4.1 whether the segments causing this process are retroflexes or not.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
(29) [hi1 ]
[pi1 ]
[sy1 a]
[bœ1 ]
[k-1]
[ku1]
[ku1 ]
[e1 ]
hirs
pirs
syrsa
börs
kårt
kort
kors
ers
[his]
[bis]
[syst1]
[løs]
[skot]
[skut]
[kTus]
[es]
‘millet’
‘pier’s’
‘cricket’
‘purse’
‘short’
‘card’
‘cows’’
‘your’
hiss
bis
syster
löss
skått
skott
kos
es
101
‘lift’
‘bee’s’
‘sister’
‘lice’
‘shot’
‘shoed’
‘cow’s’
‘e’s’
Not all of the Southern Swedish vowels as described by Svantesson undergo
diphthongization before retroflexes; the long non-high vowels and [G] show
lowering (recall section 4.3.2.2). The short vowels /ø/ and /o/ are both lowered and
diphthongized, resulting in /œ1/ and /-1/, respectively.
4.3.2.4 Rounding of front vowels
Wembawemba, which was illustrated to have backing of vowels in (20) above, also
has a process whereby the vowel /i/ is rounded if it precedes retroflex consonants
(Hercus 1986, Flemming 2002: 89ff.), see (30).
(30) /tUjitjUi/
[tUjytjUy]
/ti1naiuk/ [ty1naiuk]
/mi kuk/
[my kuk]
‘poker’
‘new, fresh’
‘egg’
Rounding in Wembawemba does not occur in the absence of a retroflex, see for
example the word for ‘tomahawk’, /tir/, which is realized as [tir]. A similar process
of vowel rounding can be found in Wergaia (Hercus 1986, Flemming 2002: 90), a
language that is closely related to Wembawemba, see examples in (31).
(31) /%i 1m/
/dU iuk/
[%y 1m]
[dU yuk]
‘spear shield’
‘end’
Retroflexes also condition the rounding of vowels in South-Dravidian
languages. Irula, for example, shows a diachronic development from /a/ to [ø] and
·
from /e/ towards [.] or [ ø ], triggered by a retroflex that was partly lost, as
comparisons with Tamil words show, see (32) (based on Zvelebil 1970: 44, 59,
64).26
(32) Irula
·
[køtu] ~ [køtu]
[k.a]
[ødu]
Tamil
[kautu]
[kea]
[eutu]
gloss
‘neck, throat’
‘bad’
‘to write’
Bright (1975: 23) describes these vowels as retracted only; no mentioning is made of
rounding. Considering Bright’s very precise descriptions of Dravidian vowels (recall
26
The Irul a vowels also show raising, which probably occurred independently of the retroflex context,
·
as no other language to my knowledge shows vowel raising in retroflex context. The raising is not
phonetically motivated and is therefore not further discussed in this dissertation.
102
Chapter 4
footnotes 19 and 20), it does not seem accidental that he does not mention any
rounding of the Irul a retracted vowels. Further support for this non-rounded
·
description of the retracted vowels in Irul5a is Diffloth (1975: 55), who describes
these segments as “centralized (or retroflexed) vowels”. Following Bright and
Diffloth, I assume that the vowels in Irul5a are not rounded and will therefore not
further treat the Irula data in this dissertation.
·
In Kodagu, another South-Dravidian language, the process of vowel backing is
·
accompanied by vowel rounding when the preceding vowel is a labial, as illustrated
already in (18). This process can be further illustrated when compared to other
Dravidian languages: Proto-Dravidian */e:/ is changed to an /o:/ in Kodagu, cf.
·
[boe] ‘hunting’ compared to Tamil [veai] ‘id.’, or Kodagu [po ] ‘to transport by
·
pack-animal’ vs. Kannada [pe u] ‘to load’ (Zvelebil 1970: 61).
4.3.2.5 Retroflexion and secondary palatalization
Secondary palatalization of retroflexion, as discussed already in section 2.5, shows
three avoidance strategies, namely change of the retroflex into a laminal palatalized
coronal, non-palatalization of the retroflex, or sequential realization of retroflex and
palatal glide. All three processes are illustrated here in (33).
(33)
(a)
(b)
(c)
input
/ j/
/j/
/j/
output
[j]
[]
[j]
process
de-retroflexion
de-palatalization
separate palatal realization
In section 2.5 de-retroflexion as in (33a) was assumed to take place in Polish and
Russian, where / / surfaces as an inherently palatalized laminal palato-alveolar [].
Resistance towards secondary palatalization as exemplified in (33b) can be
found in Scots Gaelic, where nouns usually undergo palatalization in the genitive
singular, recall the example [k>at>] ‘cat’ (nom. sg.) versus [k>at> ] ‘cat’ (gen. sg.)
from section 2.5.1. Nouns with retroflex consonants remain unpalatalized, e.g. [pa]
‘a poet’ (both nom. and gen. sg.) (Borgstrøm 1940: 76).
As described in section 2.5.2, Toda has palatalized retroflex rhotics such as
[o ] ‘foot’, or [to ] ‘pole used at funeral’ (Spajic et al. 1996). In the same section
acoustic evidence was given for the claim that these allegedly palatalized retroflexes
are realized as retroflexes with a following front glide.
4.3.3 Phonetic grounding
The previous sections contained several processes illustrating that retroflexes
disprefer front vowel context and secondary palatalization. The processes applied to
avoid these segments or segment sequences are repeated here in (34), (35), and (36).
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
(34)
(35) (a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
(36) (a)
(b)
(c)
input
/i/
/i/
/G/
/i/
/i/
/j/
/j/
/j/
output
[it]
[ ] or [.]
[æ]
[i1]
[y]
[tj]
[]
[j]
103
process
de-retroflexion
retraction
lowering
diphthongization (schwa insertion)
rounding
de-retroflexion
de-palatalization
separate palatal realization
Whereas sequences of front vowels and retroflexes actually occur in natural
languages, it was claimed in section 2.5 that retroflexion with a secondary
palatalization is articulatorily impossible and therefore does not occur in any
language. Despite this difference, both the dislike of retroflex and front vowels and
the absolute incompatibility of retroflexion and secondary palatalization have the
same articulatory grounding: a flat tongue middle and retracted tongue back
configuration for retroflexion cannot be combined with the high tongue middle and
fronted tongue back necessary for both front vowels and palatalization. A
simultaneous production of both gestures as in secondary palatalization is
impossible, and a sequential order such as retroflex and high front vowel or high
glide is dispreferred, because it involves a major movement of the tongue from one
extreme position (namely a high fronted position) to the other (the backed, velarized
position).27
The articulatory distance between the two positions can be reduced by a
number of mechanisms. First of all, the retroflex gesture can be reduced, resulting in
a non-retroflex coronal, as in (34) and (36a). Secondly, the gesture of the vowel or
the glide can be reduced. For the glide, only a total deletion can be observed in the
languages of the world (see 36b), probably because a partial reduction is not
recognizable as secondary palatalization anymore.28 For the front high vowel, several
articulatory reductions are possible which facilitate the transition to a retroflex
gesture: the vowel can be retracted (35a), or lowered (35b), or both; or
diphthongization of the vowel can occur (35c).
Vowel retraction or lowering involves a departure from the underlyingly
specified vowel quality. The vowel realized instead has a tongue position that is
closer to that of the adjacent retroflex, and thus diminishes the distance between
vowel and consonant gestures. Only high vowels are target of lowering, and front
vowels target of retraction.
27
28
The articulatory argumentation of a dislike for high front vowels and a backed, velarized position is
not restricted to retroflexion. It can also explain why velars always have a fronted allophone when
adjacent to front high vowels.
Only one case of front vowel deletion in retroflex context could be found, namely Pingding Chinese,
recall the examples in (22b).
104
Chapter 4
Diphthongization involves the movement from the high front vowel to some
kind of neutral position (or a position as in a retracted or lowered vowel) before the
tongue position for the retroflex is assumed. Only high or mid front vowels are a
target of this process. The vowel and the retroflexion gestures show a gestural delay,
as illustrated in figure 4.3.
i
i
1
tip
neutral
closed
middle
raised
lowered
raised
neutral
lowered
fronted
retracted
fronted
neutral
retracted
back
time
Figure 4.3
neutral
closed
time
Gestural timing of vowel – retroflex sequences, without delay on the left, and
with delay and resulting vowel diphthongization on the right. The three tiers
indicate the three tongue parts tip, middle, and back. The tongue blade is not
included, since it is not relevant for the depiction of this process. Neutral
position of the tongue parts is depicted with white boxes, a positive degree of
constriction i.e. raising of the articulator with a grey box (dark grey standing for
full closure), and negative constriction i.e. lowering of the articulator with a
dotted box.
Rounding of a vowel, see (35d), is obviously not a process that facilitates the
articulation of front high vowel – retroflex sequences, since both retroflexion and
rounding are produced with independent articulators, namely the tongue and the lips,
respectively, and therefore do not affect each other. This process can be motivated
by two different explanations. In Kod5agu vowel rounding seems to be caused by the
adjacent labial consonant, and not by the retroflex. The articulation of a rounding
gesture is rather slow; according to Stevens (1998: 44) the minimal time from a
rounded to an unrounded configuration of the lips is 50 to 100 ms. This slowness
can account for the fact that the lips are still not in neutral position at the beginning
of the vowel, which can subsequently lead to a rule of full vowel rounding after
labial consonants, a process that is cross-linguistically quite common.
Vowel rounding in Wembawemba and Wergaia, however, cannot be
articulatorily motivated since it is not caused by adjacent labials. Flemming (1995
[2002], 2001) proposes that the rounding in a retroflex context in these languages is
the result of a reduction of the retroflex gesture (process (34)) with concomitant
rounding of the vowel in order to preserve the perception of a low F3 typical for a
retroflex (section 5.3.2 below gives a summary of Flemming’s analysis). This
perceptual preservation seems plausible: the listener can interpret the lowered F3 of
the rounded vowel as belonging to the coronal consonant as illustrated in Figure 4.4.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
t
rounded front high V
[–2 F1, +2F2]
front high V
Figure 4.4
[ –2 F3]
[+1 F2]
105
segments the speaker articulates
phonetic cues of the segments
segments as parsed by the listener
Listener’s interpretation of the sequence rounded front vowel – alveolar as
(rounded) front vowel–retroflex (illustration of Flemming’s proposal).
Flemming, though, fails to explain why the rounding is realized on the vowel and
not on the consonant, since it is the retroflexion of the consonant that is supposed to
be enhanced by rounding. The shift in target segment can be explained by the fact
that rounding on consonants is perceptually not as salient as rounding on vowels.
Especially stop articulations yield only transitional perceptual cues for rounding,
since no continuous formant cues during the consonant are available. In addition, the
slowness of the labial gesture probably causes distinct labialization cues only
halfway of the consonant. If, on the other hand, in a vowel-consonant sequence as in
Wembawemba or Wergaia the rounding configuration is assumed for the vowel,
perceptual cues of rounding are available throughout the vowel. Therefore rounding
is more effective if realized on the preceding vowel instead on the retroflex itself.29
Both the articulatory and the perceptual motivation for the rounding processes will
be formalized in section 6.3.3.4.
Whereas for diphthongization the vowel and consonant gestures show delay, in
vowel retraction and lowering there occurs some overlap in gestures; the tongue
position is changed already during the vowel. This gestural overlap provides the
listener with additional cues for retroflex articulation, though at the cost of the vowel
cues.
It is assumed here that languages requiring very precise vowel cues, such as
languages with large vowel inventories, do not allow articulatory assimilations of
retroflex and front vowels with resulting changes in the perceptual cues of the
vowels, because it would reduce the perceptual difference between the single vowels
and thus risk perceptual confusion. Evidence for this assumption can be found in the
fact that mainly languages with very small inventories, such as the Australian
Margany, Gunya, and Bunuba with the vowels [i, a, u] (short and long) allow the
processes in (35). An assimilation mechanism acceptable for large vowel inventories
is diphthongization, see (35c), because it preserves at least part of the cues of the
original vowel. The large vowel inventory of Swedish, for example, mainly
undergoes diphthongization before retroflexes (see (26) and (29)).
Having established the phonetic motivation of the processes involving high
vowel and retroflex sequences, we can now inspect the contextual position of the
29
The present argumentation does not exclude the possibility that rounding occurs on both the vowel
and the consonant.
106
Chapter 4
segments undergoing the changes, and the manner of the retroflexes involved. The
list in (37) – (41) gives a summary of this information from the examples given
above, where R stands for the class of retroflexes and L for labials:
vowel
retroflex
language
(37) de-retroflexion (section 4.3.1)
i_
,
_i_
,
_i
,
Karok, Acoma, Molinos Mixtec
Chácobo
Khonoma Angami, Chinese, Gujarati
(38) retraction (section 4.3.2.1)
ie
_R
ie
, _
i
_R
e
_R
i
_
y
_
Irula, Kodagu, colloqu. Tamil, Toda
·
·
Ponapean
Bunuba
Wembawemba
Russian, Polish
Pingding Chinese
(39) lowering (section 4.3.2.2)
e
_R
G G o ø
_R
iu
_R
u
_R
Norwegian
Swedish
Kayardild, Margany, Gunya
Tamil, Malayalam, Kannada
(40) diphthongization (section 4.3.2.3)
i
_?
Beijing Chinese
i
_R
Gugada
high Vs + e ø o
_R
Swedish
(41) rounding (section 4.3.2.4)
i
_R
e
L_R
Wembawemba, Wergaia
Kodagu (plus retraction)
·
The process of palatalization is not included here, because it differs from the vowel
– retroflex sequences in that it always involves a palatalization context, i.e., a high
front vowel, which usually follows the consonant.
In general, it can be observed that the retroflexes involved in the processes are
almost always the whole retroflex class of the specific language. In some cases I
specified the retroflex class of specific languages, as for Russian, Polish and
Ponapean, to show the restricted set of retroflexes in these languages. The only
exceptions to this are the Beijing Chinese rhotic and the Pinging Chinese lateral,
which are not the only retroflexes in these languages, but the only segments
involved in the processes. This is due to the morphological conditioning of the
processes: diphthongization in Beijing occurs only after the suffixation of the
retroflex rhotic, and backing in Pingding occurs only after infixation of the lateral
retroflex.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
107
The direction of influence is often from retroflex to preceding vowel. Vowel
lowering and rounding occurs only before retroflexes in our examples. This
behaviour is in accordance with the stronger cues of retroflexion leading into the
segment (VC cues) than those leading out of it (CV cues), as illustrated in section
3.5. Furthermore, some studies (e.g. Krakow 1999) prove that there is a difference in
the synchronization of CV gestures, which are well synchronized, and that of VC
gestures, which are less synchronized. Therefore retroflexes and preceding vowels
show more gestural overlap than retroflexes and following vowels.
In the cases where retroflexes influence following vowels or where vowels
influence preceding retroflexes (namely in de-retroflexion) this is restricted to
retroflex fricatives, affricates, and liquids (see the examples from Gujarati, Chinese,
Ponapean and Russian). These segment classes have strong internal cues as
described in section 3.1. Furthermore, they have stable articulations without a
flapping out gesture as observable in retroflex stops (see discussion in 2.2.4.1),
which leads to less reduced CV cues than for stops. These factors can explain why
retroflex fricatives, affricates, and liquids but not stops or flaps show perceptual
influence on following vowels.
Considering the targets of vowel changing processes, it is mainly the high
front vowel or the class of front vowels that is changed, in line with the articulatory
incompatibility of these vowels with retroflexes. Lowering in (35b) is the only
process illustrated above that occurs also with high back vowels, as discussed above
(e.g. in Kayardild, Margany, Gunya, and Tamil). In section 6.3.3 below I will
propose formal accounts for the processes avoiding retroflex – front vowel
sequences with one example of each process.
4.3.4 Exception: The ruki-rule in Sanskrit
The morphophonemic process of retroflexing /s/ after the vowels /u/ and /i/ and after
the consonants /k/ and /r/ in Sanskrit (Whitney 1889: 61f.), spelled out in ruleformat in (42), is a recurrent topic in phonological descriptions.
(42) s → / r, u, k, i _
This process is often referred to as the ruki-rule and it involves four contexts which
seems to make up an unnatural class as they include both back and front vowels,
contradicting the rule for deretroflexion in a front vowel context given in (1c). Via
the ruki-rule the retroflex fricative [ ] was introduced into Sanskrit from the protoIndo-European alveolar fricative /s/.30 Other retroflex segments in Sanskrit
subsequently emerged from assimilation, see section 4.6 below. Examples of the
ruki rule are given in (43a) with the locative plural suffix /-su/ (Whitney 1889,
Flemming 1997). In other environments, this suffix occurs with an alveolar fricative,
see (43b).
30
The retroflex fricative entered Sanskrit also via a change from Indo-European *k before a /tU/. This
separate development will be ignored here since it is a process that does not occur cross-linguistically
and seems to lack a phonetic grounding. For a detailed discussion of this change see Hall (1997b:
213f.).
108
(43) (a)
Chapter 4
[svas? u]
[IatU u u]
[vak u]
[a%nUi u]
‘sister’
‘enemy’
‘voice’
‘fire’
(b)
[Vasu]
[apsu]
‘progeny’
‘water’
According to Whitney (1889: 61f.), a following [ ]31 prevents the retroflexion of /s/.
Thus forms like [us a] or [tUis as] do not surface as [u a] and [tUi as], as though they
satisfy the conditions of the ruki-rule.32 A dissimilation process like this occurs in
other languages, too, cf. section 4.6 below.
The [i] context seems unusual as trigger of the ruki-rule since retroflexes are
expected to avoid front vowels, recall section 4.3 and the articulatory explanation for
it in section 2.5. The other three segments triggering the ruki-rule are phonetically
motivated, however, as discussed below. Several explanations have been put
forward for the unusual set of contexts in which the change from an alveolar to a
retroflex fricative occurs. Whitney (1889: 61f.) himself proposes an articulatory
explanation for its four contexts: retroflexion after a retroflex rhotic is clearly an
assimilatory process, and the contexts /k/, /i/ and /u/ share a retracted tongue
position which causes the tip of the tongue “to reach the roof of the mouth more
easily at a point further back than the dental one” (ibid.). But as Vennemann (1974:
93) points out, it does not follow from the retracting influence of /i/, /u/, and /k/ that
the outcome should be retroflex instead of for instance palato-alveolar.
A widespread assumption concerning the development of the ruki-rule is that it
occurred historically in two stages, with the retroflex as final outcome and a
different segment as intermediate output. Misra (1967: 28ff.), Mayrhofer (1989) and
Hall (1997b) propose that Proto-Indo-European *s developed to a palato-alveolar
fricative [] in Indo-Iranian in the ruki context, and then, via a general rule, to the
retroflex in Sanskrit. The two stages of this diachronic process are given in (44).
(44) (a) *s (Indo-European)
(b) * (Indo-Iranian)
→
→
/ , u, k, i _ (Indo-Iranian)
(Sanskrit)
Hall proposes that evidence for such an intermediate stage can be found in the fact
that the same change (44a) occurred in Avestan, Old Persian and Baltic, where the
alveolar remains palato-alveolar, and in Slavonic, where it further changed into a
velar [x] (Allen 1951, 1954, Andersen 1968). The change in (44b) is context free,
and motivated by Hall as a change towards an unmarked sibilant inventory. The
output of (43a), //, became phonemic, which caused the two-way sibilant place
contrast /s, I/ in Indo-European to change to /s, I, /. Hall shows that this three-way
place contrast does not occur in any other language of the world because of the large
similarity between /I, /, and therefore a further change (43b) was triggered, see the
representation of the development in (45).
31
32
This transcription is mine, based on descriptions in Whitney.
Another exception, though not systematic, is the occurrence of a retroflex fricative somewhere after
the alveolar fricative, e.g. [sisak i] (Whitney 1889: 62). In such cases, the ruki-rule sometimes does
not apply.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
(45)
stage 1
/s, I/
→
stage 2
/s, I, /
→
109
stage 3
/s, I, /
Hall’s proposal is based on the assumption that the Sanskrit sound represented as ‘s’
and described as ‘post-alveolar laminal fricative’ is the alveolo-palatal [I]. Usually,
however, it is interpreted as the palato-alveolar [], as by Whitney (1889) and Allen
(1953). If the palato-alveolar is taken as the Sanskrit post-alveolar fricative, then the
outcome of the ruki-rule would collapse with this already existing class:
(46)
stage 1
/s, /
→
stage 2
/s, /
→
stage 3
? /s, I, /
From the second stage in this development there is no obvious way to derive the
inventory assumed for stage 3. This supports the assumption made by Hall that the
Sanskrit laminal post-alveolar is the alveolo-palatal [I], which will be followed here.
Hall’s proposal, however, does not provide an answer to the question why all four
contexts of the ruki rule caused exactly the same output, namely a palato-alveolar
[].
Another proposal, going back to Morgenstierne (1929) and applied by Allen
(1951: 941), Vennemann (1974), Gnanadesikan (1993: 47), and Flemming (1997),
suggests that /s/ assimilated to the four contexts, resulting in different assimilation
outputs at an intermediate stage, or several intermediate stages, as Morgenstierne
(1929: 2000) proposes. These outputs collapsed at a later stage to the retroflex
category, see the development depicted in (47).33
(47)
stage 1
/s/
stage 2
→ [s, s, us, ks, is]
stage 3
→ /s, /
The symbols s, us, ks, and is are used here to indicate the four allophones of the ruki
assimilation (in line with Vennemann’s transcription), and s as the allophone
occurring in other environments. Allen (1951: 941, 1954: 564) assumes that the
outputs of the second stage could have looked like [ , xw , x, N], respectively.
Gnanadesikan (1993: 47, footnote 22) proposes only three different outputs.
According to her, / / could have triggered a retroflex fricative, /u/ and /k/ a velar
one, and /i/ an alveolo-palatal one. Flemming (1997) suggests also three differing
outputs at stage 2, though they differ slightly from Gnanadesikan’s. Flemming’s
outputs are [ , sw, , ], respectively. The three proposals are summarized in table 4.2
on the next page.
The exact output of the distinct assimilation processes is obviously not clearly
predictable because of lack of supporting evidence. The preceding retroflex can be
assumed to have a retroflexing influence on the fricative; cross-linguistic evidence
plus a phonetic motivation for this kind of process were presented in section 4.1.
33
This representation neglects the additional posterior fricatives that occur in Indo-European and
Sanskrit given in (45) and (46), as they are irrelevant for the line of reasoning.
110
Chapter 4
Table 4.2 Comparison of different proposals for the output of /s/-assimilation in protoIndo-European.
_
u_
k_
i_
Allen
(1951, 1954)
Gnanadesikan
(1993)
Flemming
(1997)
xw
x
N
x
x
I
sw
The second context, the back vowel /u/, can also cause retroflexion, recall section
4.2 above, though Flemming’s assumption of an intermediate stage of a rounded
alveolar seems also likely, as it would involve only a change in secondary
articulation with a similar acoustic result. Rounding in a back vowel context is also
predicted by Allen. For the k and i context, however, a retroflex output is very
unlikely. Retraction of s in the context of k rather leads to a palato-alveolar [], or, if
place-assimilation takes place, to a velar [x]. The alveolo-palatal [I] is not a possible
output of this process, since it is already present in the Sanskrit sibilant inventory.
The high front vowel /i/ causes palatalization of a front coronal sibilant which can
result in a more posterior place of articulation as in the palato-alveolar [], see a
similar development of palatalization in Basque (Iverson & OnFderra 1985). A
summary of these predictions is given in table 4.3.
Table 4.3
Alternative proposal for the output of /s/-assimilation in proto-Indo-European.
input
_
u_
k_
i_
output
/sw
/x
Based on these assumptions, minimally two (for instance [ , ]) to maximally four
([ , sw, x, ]) different outputs of ruki-assimilation can be postulated.
All assimilatory outputs (those by Allen, Gnanadesikan, Flemming, and my
own) have in common that they are acoustically very similar to each other: they all
have lowered high frequencies, as pointed out already by Vennemann (1974: 93),34
though they are acoustically very distinct from /s/. The perceptual similarity of these
outputs led to a collapse of all two to four allophones into one category which
contrasts with the original category of the alveolar fricative. A merger of
acoustically similar allophones is a common diachronic process. In Norwegian, for
example, the palato-alveolar [], an assimilation product from historical /sj/ and
34
For a discussion on the acoustic similarity of retroflex, rounded fricatives, and palato-alveolar see e.g.
Hamann (2002b). Note also that the outputs of the ruki-rule assumed by Flemming are particularly
designed to be acoustically as close as possible.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
111
/skV/ (Kristoffersen 2000: 23), merged with the output of the retroflexion rule, [ ],
to one category [ ], so skje ‘spoon’ is realized nowadays as [ e].35
The merger of the four allophones in Sanskrit, however, does not explain why
the resulting category is retroflex rather than palato-alveolar, velar or alveolopalatal. The retroflex category seems arbitrary, as the same process of assimilation
in ruki context and merger of outputs lead to a different category in other IndoIranian languages: in Avestan, Old Persian, and Baltic it resulted in a palato-alveolar
fricative, and in Slavic in a velar one. Allen (1954: 564) proposes that the different
resulting categories in these languages might be due to already existing processes
and categories: in Sanskrit, retroflexion was an established process in connection
with liquids, thus the retroflex was a likely category to emerge. Avestan had no
retroflex segments or allophones, thus a retroflex is not expected to be the endresult
of this development. Allen proposes that the existing palatalization and
labiovelarization processes would favour either [N] or [xw]. For Slavonic, the already
existing opposition of palatalized and velarized articulations promoted the
emergence of [N]/[x] (Allen 1954: 565).
Another instance of fricative merger, which also seems to be motivated by
already existing categories occurred in Old High German, according to Vennemann
(1974: 94). Here, the allophones of /s/ after /r/ (which might have been a retroflex
allophone, analogous to the processes described in section 4.1), and before /w, b, p,
m/, i.e. both [rs, sw ], merged with the existing class [], for instance in the words
bars ‘bass’, swert ‘sword’, spil ‘game’, and smal ‘small’ (Penzl 1969: 80).
The ruki-rule was included in this chapter on phonetically-grounded processes
involving retroflexes despite the fact that at first glance this process seems to have
an unnatural context with retroflexion occuring after both back and front vowels. At
closer investigation it became clear that this diachronic process took place in several
stages, each of which with a natural context and a phonetic motivation.
4.4 Retroflexion of vowels
This section deals with the rule of vowel retroflexion illustrated in (1d). Ball &
Rahilly (1999: 125) point out that in many languages post-vocalic rhotics can be
realized by pronouncing part or all of the vowels with a tongue tip raising or
backwards bending instead of a rhotic gesture. The resulting vowels are called rcoloured, retroflexed, or rhotacized, recall the description of retroflex vowels in
section 2.2.4.7. Vowel retroflexion is not only caused by rhotics but can also occur
before a non-rhotic retroflex consonant, often in combination with a drop of the
retroflex. Section 4.4.1 will present examples of vowel retroflexion in retroflex
contexts, and section 4.4.2 gives a phonetic explanation for this process.
35
A further merger can be observed in present day Norwegian, where the phoneme classes [I] and [ ]
collapse in favour of the retroflex, cf. Papazian (1994) and Dommelen (2001).
112
Chapter 4
4.4.1 Examples
Several Dravidian languages show retroflexion and backing of vowels in preretroflex position (Zvelebil 1970: 38). Badaga (Emeneau 1939: 44) even has two
types of retroflex vowels, half and fully retroflexed. Examples of this are given in
(48) using Emeneau’s transcription.
(48) Plain vowel
kae
‘unripe fruit’
kombu ‘horn’
be·
‘mouth
half retroflexed
áé
‘tiger’s den’
kómbile
‘I did not have
(her) as wife’
bé·
‘bangle’
fully retroflexed
käë ‘weeds’
köë ‘carcass’
bë·
‘crops’
Zvelebil gives an example of Badaga vowel retroflexion, which was caused by a
retroflex rhotic that was later lost, as comparisons with other Dravidian languages
illustrate. Thus ‘ass’ is [ka?te] in Badaga, where it contrasts with [kate], ‘I learned’,
whereas in Kota ‘ass’ is realized as [ka?t]. The retroflexion of vowels in Badaga
seems to show some incompatibility with fronted tongue articulations, a process
typical of retroflexes as we saw already in section 4.3. Phonetically the half and
fully retroflexed /i/ and /e/ are backed. Emeneau (1939) mentions that in the fullyretroflexed phonemes “the elevation of the tongue to mid and high position [is] as
far back in the oral cavity as possible, in the half-retroflexed phonemes [it is]
advanced almost to the mixed position” (p. 44). The fully retroflexed short /i/ does
not occur in Emeneau’s data, which he took to be an accidental gap. This gap,
however, might be an indication that a fully retroflex short /i/ does not exist in
Badaga, due to the large articulatory distance between the retroflex and the front
vowel gesture as described above. The existence of a fully retroflexed long high
front vowel, e.g. kï·e ‘down’, is no counterevidence to this claim, as the long vowel
allows a transition from a front high tongue position to a backed one. Further work
on the Badaga vowel system has to be conducted to confirm or falsify this
hypothesis.
Kodagu is another South-Dravidian language in which vowels are retroflexed
·
by a following rhotic, which then gets deleted: the future base /tir-p-/ ‘I shall finish’
is realized as [t.?p], again with retraction of the vowel (Zvelebil 1970: 38). In
Tamil, all retroflex consonants trigger retroflexion of the vowels that precede them
(Christdas 1988: 181).
The dialects of British English spoken in West Somerset and North-east Devon
have an interesting process involving the retroflex rhotic, a segment typical for this
area, followed by a high front vowel [i] or [G]. Besides lowering and centralizing of
the vowels to [1], the rhotic is realized as retroflexion of the vowel. The word red,
for example, is pronounced as [1?&] in these dialects, and pretty as [p1?&T] (Wakelin
1972: 99).
Australian languages show many processes of vowel retroflexion before
retroflex consonants. In Bunuba, according to Rumsey (2000), all vowels followed
by /r/ “take on an r-coloration” (p. 44). In some Bunuba words with g or b followed
by /ir/, the /i/ merges completely with the following /r/, e.g. biray ‘come out’
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
113
surfaces as [b e]. The result sounds like a consonant cluster, according to Rumsey.
This process of high front vowel deletion can be taken as further evidence for the
dislike of retroflexes and high front vowels, recall section 4.3.36
Morphy (1983: 20) observes that in the Djapu dialect of Yolngu, another
Australian language spoken in Arnhem Land, all vowels have a slight degree of
retroflexion before retroflex consonants. The vowels in the Australian language
Yukulta show the same process, according to Keen (1983). In Margany and Gunya,
only the low vowel /a/ is retroflexed before a retroflex consonant (Breen 1981), see
/4an ba&/ [4an ba?&] ‘sweat’.37
While it is usually the preceding vowel so affected, in languages such as
Marrithiyel the following vowel is retroflexed. Pitta-Pitta (Australian) is a case
where vowels are retroflexed both when preceding and following a retroflex
consonant. The retroflexion is apparently weaker in the following vowels (Blake &
Breen 1971). And in Bengali, /i/ is said to be somewhat retroflex after //, /&/, /l&/,
and /l/ (Ferguson 1960).
4.4.2 Phonetic grounding
Vowel retroflexion can easily be accounted for as a gestural overlap of the vowel
gesture and the retroflexion of the tongue tip: the tongue tip is curled in already
when the vowel is still articulated. This overlap of gestures is possible since vowels
do not involve the tongue tip in their articulation. If the retroflex apical gesture is not
synchronized with the beginning or end of the vowel gesture, it influences the vowel
next to it. Especially retroflex approximants, which have no closure and thus no
definite point which the retroflex tongue tip gesture can be synchronized with, make
retroflexion of a neighbouring vowel very likely. The examples in 4.5.1 show that
high front vowels undergo lowering and/or retraction before they are retroflexed.
This is due to the dislike of the high tongue middle plus fronted tongue back with
the lowered tongue middle and the retracted tongue back of retroflexes.
The gestural overlap occurring in retroflexed vowels is depicted in figure 4.5
on the next page, where the left graphic illustrates non-overlap, and the right total
overlap (retroflexion of the vowel).
Vowel retroflexion adds a cue to facilitate the perception of the retroflex
consonant, without diminishing the vowel cues as the processes of vowel lowering
or retraction. The gestural overlap that occurs in vowel retroflexion can result in a
lengthening of the vowel gesture as observed in the Australian language Kayardild
(Hamilton 1996: 45, Evans 1985: 504), in order to prolong the cues for the vowel.
36
37
The Bunuban example illustrates the difficulty to distinguish between a retroflexed vowel and a
retroflex approximant in vowel context. A similar problem is observable in Mayali, a Gunwinggun
language spoken in Arnhem Land and Croker Island. According to Evans (1995: 740), Mayali has a
process of “syllable-retroflexion” manifested in different ways: the word for ‘death adder’, for
example, can be pronounced as [be?k], [b?ek], [be?ek], or [bek]. These realizations might be instances
of approximant insertion and/or vowel retroflexion, a point to be clarified in future studies.
Breen (1981: 289) uses a subscript dot under the /a/ to indicate the retroflexion of this vowel, just as
he uses the alveolar with subscript dot for the retroflex series.
114
Chapter 4
a
tip
neutral
middle
back
closed
retracted
time
Figure 4.5
raised
closed
lowered
lowered
neutral
a?
retracted
time
Low vowel – retroflex sequences with synchronized gestures (left) and with
gestural overlap (right). The three tiers indicate the three tongue parts tip,
middle, and back. The tongue blade is not included, since it is not relevant for
the depiction of this process. Neutral position of the tongue parts is depicted
with white boxes, a positive degree of constriction i.e. raising of the articulator
with a grey box (dark grey standing for full closure), and negative constriction
i.e. lowering of the articulator with a dotted box.
Retroflexion of a vowel sometimes leads to a deletion of the retroflex
consonant triggering it, as in the case of Badaga or the South-Western dialects of
Britain, which can be explained by a total overlap of vowel and retroflexion gesture.
This process occurs only for retroflex approximants, since approximants do not
require a full closure and thus the retroflexion gesture can totally blend with the
vowel gesture.
The descriptions above showed that vowel retroflexion often co-occurs with
vowel retraction (as described in 4.3.2.1), a further indication of the retroflex gesture
being articulated already during the vowel, as a lower tongue position facilitates the
retroflexion of the tongue tip.
The more frequent retroflexion of vowels in a position preceding retroflex
segments compared to those following retroflexes has also an articulatory
explanation: the tongue tip can curl inwards already during the articulation of the
vowel (anticipatory gesture), since vowel and retroflex are articulated with different
parts of the tongue. Thus an overlap of both gestures is possible. Gestural overlap is
also possible in the other direction, and results in retroflexion of a following vowel
(as was shown for Marrithiyel, Pitta-Pitta, and Bengali). But instead of an
anticipatory gesture which saves articulatory time, the retroflex gesture is held
longer than necessary in this case and thus influences the vowel. Retroflexion of
following vowels is probably perceptually motivated by an enhancement of the
retroflex cues only, whereas retroflexion of the preceding vowel is perceptually and
articulatorily motivated.
4.5 Phonotactics of retroflex segments
This section is concerned with restrictions on the phonotactics of retroflexes. The
occurrence of retroflexes within a syllable and a prosodic word is cross-
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
115
linguistically asymmetrical: word-finally and post-vocalically they occur more often
than in word-initial position and post-consonantally, see (1e). This asymmetry was
the topic of an extensive study by Steriade (1995, 2001a), whose phonetic
explanation will be largely followed here. Steriade’s analysis is described in detail in
section 5.3.3.
The following subsection 4.5.1 gives some examples of this asymmetrical
behaviour. Possible phonetic accounts are discussed in subsection 4.5.2.
4.5.1 Examples
This section discusses two language families: the Australian languages, which
mostly have two apicals and two laminals and show a number of interesting
restrictions on the occurrence of retroflexes, and the Indo-Aryan languages, with
smaller coronal inventories.
Most Australian languages have no retroflex segment word-initially, which
leads to a neutralization of the contrast between apical alveolar38 and retroflex in
word-initial position as described by researchers such as Evans (1995: 727),
Hamilton (1993: 134), and Gnanadesikan (1993: 35). Bunuba (Rumsey 2000) is
such a language. Word-medially, Bunuba contrasts both apicals, see (49a) (the
sounds in question are boldfaced), whereas word-initially only apical alveolars
occur, see (49b). If a subsequent syllable contains [&, , ], the word-initial apical is
realized as retroflex, see (49c), which is an instance of long-distance retroflexion, to
be discussed in section 4.6.2.39
(49) (a) bii
%a u
dDi i%i
(b) laWi
dumuru
(c) a&L
uu
‘thigh’
‘penis’
‘bird’ (gen.)
‘freshwater eel’
‘chest’
‘short’
‘heart’
widi%i
%alu
dDirin%in
‘stick insect’
‘road’
‘owlet nightjar’
Further Australian languages with the same restrictions on retroflexes word-initally
and word-finally are Andiljaugwa (Dixon 1970), Kalkatungu (Hamilton 1996), Kitja
(Dixon 1980), Ndjebbana (McKay 2000)40, Ngiyambaa (Donaldson 1980), Thargan
(Dixon 1980), Watjarri (Douglas 1981), and the closely-related Margany and Gunya
(Breen 1981). Only 3 of the 22 languages Dixon (1980) investigates show an apical
contrast in word-initial position.
A number of Australian languages is reported to have a retroflex but no apical
alveolar in word-initial position. That is, the apical contrast is neutralized in favour
38
39
40
As defined in chapter 1, ‘apical alveolar’ is used in this dissertation to denote any kind of front apical
articulation, i.e. it includes both alveolar and dental place of articulation.
The rhotic / / is the only retroflex segment that does not trigger the retroflexion of word-initial apicals.
Recall from the data in (19) that the high front vowels in these examples are retracted before a
retroflex.
The retroflex rhotic in Ndjebbana, however, does occur in word-initial position, where it contrasts
with the apical alveolar rhotic (McKay 2000: 177).
116
Chapter 4
of the retroflex place of articulation. Hamilton’s (1996) database of Australian
languages lists Bularnu, Djambarrpuyngu, Gaalpu, Kayardild, Mangarrayi, Marra,
Ngalakan, Ngandi, Pintupi, Pitta-Pitta, Ritharrngu, Wardaman, Wambaya, and
Walmatjari41 as such languages. Lardil is a further example of an Australian
language with a retroflex as the only apical in word-initial position (Dixon 1970,
Gnanadesikan 1994: 128). Sharpe (1972) describes that Alawa, a Maran Australian
language spoken in Arnhem land, has a retroflex in word-initial position following a
vowel and within a phonological phrase. In the same position following a phrase
boundary, the segment is alveolar. This indicates a dependency of the type of
articulation on the phrasal position.
For some Australian languages free variation between both apicals is reported.
According to Hamilton (1996: 133), Gooniyandi is such a language, where the two
word-initial apical series vary freely between alveolar and retroflex articulation
when not conditioned by a following apical (50a) (data based on McGregor 1990:
70f.). When the following consonant is apical, the initial apical segment harmonizes
to this place of articulation, see (50b). Laminal consonants that follow the retroflex
do not cause assimilation of the initial segment to a laminal articulation, see (50c).
(50) (a)
(b)
(c)
/duwu/
/di ipindi/
/dili/
/laW%ija/
[duwu ~ &uwu]
[&i ipindi]
[dili]
[laW%ija ~ aW%ija]
‘cave’
‘he entered’
‘flame; light’
‘midday’
Neutralization occurs also post-consonantally in Gooniyandi, whereas postvocalically, a contrast between retroflex and apical alveolar is given, see (51).
(51) /kilii/
/wa&%uluna/
[kilii]
[wa&%uluna]
‘grass’
‘I bring them’
McKay (2000: 177) mentions that the Ndjebbana neutralized initial apical
sometimes appears to be retroflex, thus providing another example of variation.
Butcher’s (1992) phonetic study shows that free variation occurs in the neutralized
position of several Australian languages.
Besides in word-initial position, retroflexes also have the tendency not to occur
post-consonantally, see Gooniyandi. A language showing neutralization after
consonants but not in initial position is Nunggubuyu (Steriade 1995: 18).
Languages that have an apical contrast only intervocalically and in V_C
position are numerous on the Australian continent. The Djapu dialect of Dhuwala41
Walmatjari has been subject of several studies with differing interpretations. According to
Gnanadesikan (1993: 49) this language has retroflexes only utterance-initially, but alveolars
morpheme-initially and word-initially. Gnanadesikan (1994: 128f.) states that there is free variation
between alveolars and retroflexes in syllable-initial positions in Walmatjari, except after /u/, /a/ and
other retroflex consonants, where they are solely retroflex. This interpretation is based on Hudson &
Richards (1969) who claim that the alveolar/retroflex contrast is “neutralized”, and who use the
symbols t , d, etc. for their transcription.
· ·
Gnanadesikan further proposes that one might interpret the Walmatjari initial apicals as being
articulated midway between an alveolar and a retroflex, see discussion in section 4.5.2 below.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
117
Dhuwal (Morphy 1983, Hamilton 1993: 131), for example, has both apicals and
laminals post-vocalically in coda position with a following non-coronal.
Homorganic coronal clusters are also allowed, for instance [mi.&u4] ‘snail’. These
tendencies in Australian phonotactics lead Dixon (1980: 155) to posit restrictions on
the occurrence of segments in words of the general shape C1VC2C3VC4: the apical
contrast only occurs at C2 and C4, i.e. those slots that follow a vowel. Apical
contrasts do not generally occur in C3-position in a heterorganic cluster.
A number of Dravidian and Indo-Aryan languages also disallow retroflexion
word-initially. Proto-Dravidian has neither retroflex nor alveolar consonants in
word-initial position, i.e. no word begins with [t, l, r, , , , ] (Zvelebil 1970: 77).
This behaviour holds for many modern Dravidian languages, such as Irula (Diffloth
·
1975).42 Tamil (Christdas 1988), Kod5agu (Ebert 1996), and Toda (Shalev, Ladefoged
& Bhaskararao 1993: 101). In the Indo-Aryan language Punjabi, all phonemes are
allowed in word-initial position apart from the retroflex liquids [, ] and the nasal
[]. Punjabi, however, allows [] and [ ] as final members in word-internal threeconsonantal clusters such as [lomb i] ‘fox’ and word-final clusters such as [k, n&,
l, r&] (Bhatia 1993: 340).
Interestingly, some Indo-Aryan languages with a twofold apical series but no
retroflexes in word-initial position allow both apicals word-initially in loanwords. In
Kodagu (Ebert 1996), for example, retroflex consonants do not occur initially in
·
native words. In loanwords, however, word-initial retroflex consonants are quite
frequent: English /t/, for instance, is rendered in Kodagu as []: teacher is [iter.]
·
(Ebert 1996: 6).43 Punjabi (Bhatia 1993) also only allows retroflexes in loanwords,
e.g. [ram] ‘tram’. The Dravidian Tamil allows the retroflex fricative [ß] initially
exceptionally in some proper nouns, all of them recent borrowings from Hindi or
Sanskrit.
4.5.2 Phonetic grounding
As we saw in section 4.5.1 above, several Australian languages suspend the contrast
between the two apicals in word-initial position and in post-consonantal position.
The realization of the apical in these positions varies; some languages choose an
apical realization, others a retroflex one, and some allow variation between the two.
From an articulatory point of view, one would expect the apical contrast to
neutralize towards the apical alveolar, as the apical alveolar involves a less complex
articulation than the retroflex since no displacement of the tongue tip is involved
42
43
The segment // can occur in a number of items in some Dravidian languages word-initially. Zvelebil
(1970: 102) explains the occurrence of // word-initially by four processes: assimilation towards
following (but not immediately adjacent) retroflex nasals (to be discussed below in section 4.6.2),
metathesis, onomatopoetic forms, and borrowings from non-Dravidian languages.
The retroflex phonemes might be chosen as equivalent to English alveolars because the English
interdentals /X/ and /Y/ are represented in Kod5agu by the dentals [tU] and [dU], respectively. In order to
retain a distinction between the two English coronal series, these phoneme classes are transferred to
the two perceptually closest native coronal phonemes (dental and retroflex). A similar shift in coronal
categories for the adaptation of English words can be observed in Kannada (Schiffman 1983: 11ff.):
town [aZnu] or end [G&u] versus thing [tU(>)T4] or thanks [tU(>)e4ksu].
118
Chapter 4
(recall the universal articulatory markedness hierarchy under (2) in chapter 1).
Steriade (1995) points out that languages with a contrast between apical dental and
apical retroflex have two articulations with displaced tongue tip, as the dental
involves a fronting of the tongue. In these languages an articulatory neutralization
towards the apical alveolar should be expected, according to Steriade. This
occurrence of a third articulatory position in neutralization is possible, but is not
reported very often. Due to the tendency of language transcribers to classify
occurring segments into the phonemically existing apical categories of a language
(either dental or retroflex), this deviation may pass unnoticed. It seems unlikely,
however, that languages should employ a new categorical articulation instead of one
of the already existing ones because of the general tendency to re-use already
learned gestures. Further phonetic studies investigating the actual articulation of
apical segments in sites of neutralization are necessary to clarify this point.
The articulatory variation and neutralization observed in the phonotactic
patterns of retroflexes is only acceptable if the resulting output shows no large
perceptual deviation from the input. It was shown in section 3.5 that retroflexes have
strong VC transitions, and that their CV transitions are weaker and more similar to
those of apical alveolars. Steriade (1993b) used this fact to explain why the
neutralization of apical contrast mostly takes place in word-initial and postconsonantal position: the cues distinguishing between apicals are insufficient in
these positions that have no VC transitions, recall the markedness hierarchy under
(4) in chapter 1. The less distinct CV cues of apicals can also explain why a
language such as Proto-Dravidian has no apical in initial position at all.
Furthermore, the lesser saliency implies that a language should not employ a
contrast between retroflex and apical alveolar in postconsonantal position without
having this contrast in postvocalic position. This was attested by the data above: no
language could be found that contrasts alveolar and retroflex apicals in initial or
postconsonantal position only.
Flemming (2002) argues that the CV transitional cues and the VOT cues of
retroflexes are more contrastive with those of laminals than the apical alveolar or
dental cues are. Following from this, Flemming assumes the neutralized apical is
retroflex, in order to preserve the contrast with the laminal series. This assumption
sounds reasonable but is not confirmed by the data: the examples given in 4.5.1
show no preference for retroflexion in neutralized positions.
In sum, the asymmetrical behaviour of retroflex cues can account for the
phonotactic behaviour of this class. The realization of the neutralized category,
however, seems to be language-specific. Constraint rankings in an OT framework,
which can account for the cross-linguistic phonotactic differences are proposed in
section 6.3.6.
4.6 Assimilation of retroflexion
Languages with retroflex segments commonly show assimilation processes
involving this specific articulatory class, see (1f). Two kinds of assimilation can be
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
119
observed, one of adjacent segments and one of segments which are separated by
intervening material, see rule (1f), repeated here as (52).
(52) (a)
(b)
t →
Vt →
V
or
or
t
→
tV →
V
Examples of local assimilation processes like (52a) are presented in section 4.6.1,
examples for long-distance retroflexion (52b) in 4.6.2. A phonetic account for both
is proposed in 4.6.3.
Assimilation is topically related to dissimilation processes. Dissimilation of
retroflexes occurred for example with Proto-Dravidian consonant sequences such
*//, which occur as [n & ] in several modern Dravidian languages.44 Another
example for dissimilation is the infinitive marker –/a/ of Punjabi which has a
variant [na] after retroflex segments, cf. /ja+a/ ‘to know’ [jana] (Bhatia
1993). Dissimilation processes are probably due to the need for increased perceptual
distinction. These processes are not further treated in this dissertation.
4.6.1 Local assimilation
Let us first look at examples in which retroflexes affect following segments.
Retroflex segments in Swedish and Norwegian assimilate following dentals into
retroflexes. Examples from Swedish (from Eliasson 1986: 280) are given in (53).45
(53)
/has/
/Gst/
/væ&sli%/
/kvassekel/
[ha ]
[æ ]
[væ& i%]
[kva ek1l]
harts
Ernst
världslig
kvartssekel
‘resin’
(name)
‘worldly’
‘quarter-century’
This assimilation is iterative, that is, dentals following the assimilated retroflex are
also retroflexed, as all but the first example in (53) illustrate.
Norwegian assimilation of retroflexes has an equivalent in Swedish. An
interesting phenomenon concerning Norwegian retroflex assimilation is the
formation of the patronymic form, by the addition of the suffix -sen /-sn[/ to a name.
According to Kristoffersen (2000: 318), three different conditions for this process
have to be distinguished. If the name ends in an /r/, then the two coronals in the
suffix are retroflexed and the r is deleted: Persen [pe:. []. If the name ends in a
retroflex, Gjert [jæ ], then the initial segment of the suffix will be realized as
retroflex. The nasal, however, remains dental: [jæ. n[], not *[jæ. []. If the name
ends in an assimilated retroflex, Morten /mon/ [m-.[],46 where the retroflexion of
44
45
46
According to Zvelebil (1970: 169), these languages are Kolami, Naikr5i, Parji, Gondi, Konda, Pengo,
Kurukh, Malto, Brahui, and Kuvi.
Retroflexion in Swedish does not assimilate from lateral to non-lateral, compare pärltråd [pætrod]
‘string of pearls’ to pärllist [pæist] ‘pearl molding’ (Eliasson 1986: 280). No explanation for this
exception seems to be available.
Kristoffersen (ibid.) represents the surface structure of Morten with a geminate retroflex stop, in order
to encode the shortness of the vowel. A different way of encoding vowel length is to assume an
underlying length specification of Norwegian vowels (see discussion in Fretheim 1983), which makes
Kristoffersen’s move unnecessary.
120
Chapter 4
the final segment originates from the previous retroflex, further spreading is
blocked: the output form is [m-.[.sn[], not *[m-.[. n[] or *[m-.[. []. A way of
summarizing these three conditions is to assume a restriction on the number of
retroflex consonants for this process: no more than two retroflex segments are
allowed on the surface.
In both Norwegian and Swedish, assimilation of a dental to a retroflex is
progressive. A similar rule can be found in some Dravidian and Indo-Aryan
languages. In Kannada (or Kanarese), a Dravidian language spoken in the Karnataka
state of South India, sequences of a retroflex (lateral, stop or nasal) and a nonretroflex coronal usually show progressive assimilation of the non-retroflex
(Schiffman 1983: 8, 16). The examples in (54) illustrate this point.47
(54)
/he-al-ila/
/ko-d-e/
/t-ilu/
/ka-d-e/
[heTla]
[ko&K]
[t-u]
[ka&K]
tell-inf-neg
obtain-past-1s
see-past-1s
‘didn’t say’
‘I obtained’
‘cradle’
‘I saw’
In rapid speech, Kannada also shows progressive assimilation of retroflexion with
other segments (Sridhar 1990: 303). Furthermore, in colloquial Kannada a voiced
retroflex stop is sometimes inserted between a coronal lateral or nasal and [r], see
the examples in (55) (Schiffman 1983).
(55) (a)
(b)
/ellaru/
/ero/
/kaaru/
[Gl&ru] ~ [Gldru] ‘all people’
[e&ro]
‘get up!’
[ka&ru]
‘thieves’
Sridhar (1990: 311) claims that if a dental precedes the inserted stop, this stop can be
either dental or retroflex, see (55a). If the preceding coronal is retroflex, however, as
in the case of (55b), the inserted stop is always retroflex. This follows the general
tendency of progressive assimilation of retroflexion. Further assimilation of the /r/ is
prevented as Kannada has no retroflex rhotic.
In many Dravidian languages an alternation between a retroflex lateral and a
retroflex stop can be observed.48 According to Zvelebil (1070: 102), this alternation
originates historically in a progressive assimilation of // + /t/ > // and a subsequent
deletion of the lateral. Furthermore, the geminate [&] in Telugu and Kannada
probably arose from the progressive assimilation of a voiced dental suffix –d to a
preceding root-final //, i.e. // + /dU/ > /&&/. Examples are Telugu [a&u] and Kannada
[a&i] ‘to obstruct’ from Proto-Dravidian *a- (Zvelebil 1970: 104).
47
48
The retroflex and the non-retroflex in Kannada are often only adjacent after deletion of an
intermediate vowel. This process of vowel deletion and another one of vowel reduction, also
observable in the data in (55) are not discussed here.
In South-Dravidian, there is a widespread alternation between [] and [], as Zvelebil (p. 101f.)
describes, with [] probably as the original sound: Literary Tamil /amaviam/ ‘with mother’ is
realized as [amaviam] in Madurai Tamil. There is also an alternation with [] in certain items: Tamil
[ta] ‘coolness, cool’ vs. [tai] ‘coolness’, Malayalam [ta] ‘cold’, Kannada [ta] ‘coldness, cold’.
Again, [] is presumably the underlying phoneme, and Zvelebil (p. 102) proposes the development *//
> [] / [] / [].
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
121
Diachronically retroflex stops were introduced into Sanskrit by progressive
assimilation of dentals to the already existing retroflex fricatives (recall section 4.3.4
on the diachronic development of the fricative).49 Thus, the following development
occurs: * t > and *Bd > B& (Bhat 1973: 33). Examples are given in (56) (from
Misra 1967: 68f.); they show that the triggering retroflex fricative was deleted at a
later stage, when the retroflex stops had become phonemic.
(56)
Sanskrit
nUi&a
mi&ha
vo&hum
le&hi
<
<
<
<
pre-Sanskrit
*nUi da
*mi dUha
*va dUhum
*la dUhi
gloss
‘nest’
‘reward’
‘to carry’
‘licks’
Another example of the assimilation of retroflexion comes from south-western
British dialects that have a retroflex [?] (recall description in section 4.1). In these
dialects, an assimilation of the following alveolar to this retroflex segment occurs, so
‘readers’ is realized as [?id1?B] (Ball & Rahilly 1999: 56). According to Wakelin
(1972: 99), the rhotic is deleted in such cases and surfaces as vowel retroflexion (cf.
section 4.4): tears (verb) [tG?B] and shirt [1 ?]. Besides progressive assimilation
these dialects also have regressive assimilation, e.g. tree [?i] or straw [ ?-] (ibid.).
Let us look at examples of processes where retroflexes cause assimilation of
the preceding segments. Modern Telugu has regressive assimilation after vowel
deletion, see the examples in (57) (based on Gilbert 1992).
/patUa+ebilu/
/adUi+&abba/
(57)
[paebilu ]
[a&&abba]
‘old table’
‘that is a can’
In Sanskrit, dentals also undergo regressive assimilation to retroflexes, see
(58a) (Whitney 1889: 66f., Allen 1962: 83ff.).
(58) (a)
(b)
/tUatU+&aukatUe/
/tUanU+&imb\anU/
/patUasU+alatUi/
/tUanU+VanUanU/
/etUatU+tI>atUtUram/
/tUatUasU+tIa/
[tUa&aukatUe]
[tUa&imb\anU]
[patUa alatUi]
[tUaWVanUanU]
[etUattI>atUtUram]
[tUatUaItIa]
‘it approaches’
‘those infants’
‘the foot is disturbed’
‘those people’
‘this umbrella’
‘and then’
The regressive assimilation process in Sanskrit applies also to palatals, see (58b). A
dental, however, does not trigger assimilation of a preceding coronal, e.g. / asu/.50
49
50
Another source for retroflex segments in Sanskrit were borrowings from Dravidian, see Burrow
(1955) and Masica (1991) for discussion and examples. Masica (1991: 157f.) further mentions the
introduction of non-sibilant retroflexes via the retroflex rhotic and its syllabic counterpart, which leads
to the question of how these segments were introduced into the language. For a possible answer, see
section 4.1 above, where it was proposed that rhotic approximants can emerge easily as alternants of
non-retroflex coronal rhotics.
On the contrary, one case of progressive assimilation of dental to following retroflex occurs, namely
when the retroflex is the fricative [ ] (Whitney 1889: 67f.), e.g. /dvi tas/ [dvi as] or /dve tum/
[dve um].
122
Chapter 4
Regressive assimilation is also present in Vietnamese, where retroflex // and
// occur after a vowel (word-final) only if the following word has an initial
retroflex affricate (Bhat 1973: 45).
Indo-Aryan Punjabi (Bhatia 1993: 347) shows the reverse process of the
usually observed one: a retroflex is assimilated to the following nasal dental. Punjabi
regressive assimilation of a stem-final retroflex to the following dental nasal of the
present-I suffix -/na/ is exemplified in (59).
(59)
ma
dDa
‘to agree’ + na
‘to know’ + na
[mana]
[dDana]
Besides assimilation to retroflex segments, one can also find assimilation of
retroflexes towards other places of articulation. Colloquial Tamil (Zvelebil 1970:
103), for example, has a process whereby retroflexes are assimilated to following
non-coronals, namely palatals, velars, and labials. Examples of this process are
given in (60).
(60) Literary Tamil
[ukarnten]
[kaci]
[kepen]
Colloquial Tamil
[okkanden]
[kacci]
[keppen]
gloss
‘I sat’
‘party’
‘I shall hear’
This process is not restricted to retroflexes, other coronals such as alveolar /t/ for
example assimilate as well.
4.6.2 Non-local assimilation
In a small number of languages retroflex sounds can cause retroflexion of nonadjacent coronal segments. Examples of such long-distance retroflexion occur for
instance in Sanskrit, South-Dravidian, and Australian, indicating that it is not a
feature specific to one language family or to areal contact.
In Sanskrit n is retroflexed when it follows a retroflex continuant [ ], or any
rhotic. Examples for this so-called nati rule are given in (61) with the middle
participle –anUa– (Whitney 1889: 65).51
(61)
[puraa]
[tIak aa]
[k ub>aa]
[kr[paa]
‘fill’
+
‘see’
+
‘quake’ +
‘lament’ +
middle participle
middle participle
middle participle
middle participle
However, the nati rule is blocked by intervening non-retroflex coronal consonants:
[k vedana], ‘hum’ + middle participle (ibid.).
In Dravidian, long-distance retroflexion occurs as a diachronic process. The
development of initial retroflex stops in some Dravidian languages is due to the
spread of retroflexion from a retroflex nasal occurring after but not adjacent to the
51
N-retroflexion occurs only when the nasal is followed by a vowel, another nasal or a glide, hence the
alternation [brahman] vs. [brahmaa] ‘Brahman’ (Whitney 1889: 65).
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
123
initial segment in the same word, and a deletion of the retroflex nasal at a later stage.
Thus, Proto-Dravidian *tak- ‘to be obtained/ to remain’ is [tauka] in Malayalam
or [taku] in Tamil, but [&aku] in Telugu (Zvelebil 1970: 102). Presumably this
process was blocked by intervening coronal consonants, though this hypothesis
requires further testing.
In the Australian language Mpakwithi the retroflex continuant [?]52 causes
retroflexion of the vowel which occurs in the preceding syllable, even if consonants
occur between these two segments: /%wap@a/ ‘is eating’ is realized as [4%wa?f?a]
(Evans 1995: 739). In Ritharngu or Ritharungo (Heath 1980), spoken in Eastern
Arnhem Land, a retroflex causes retroflexion of a preceding vowel even across a
glottal stop: /aEa/ ‘metal axe’ is realized as [a?Ea] (Heath p. 11).
4.6.3 Phonetic grounding
Assimilation processes are articulatorily motivated by a reduction of the different
places of articulation of two (adjacent) segments to one place. This reduction is
usually the case if the gestures are made with the same articulator, for instance if
both are apical. If the articulators differ, as in sequences of labial and apical, for
example, one finds overlap of gestures (see the studies of Browman & Goldstein
1989, and many others). The overlap results in a loss of perceptual cues for one
gesture and subsequent reduction of this gesture.
The direction of assimilation is usually regressive, i.e. assimilation of a
segment to the following occurs. This direction is motivated by the anticipation of
the following gesture during the articulation of the present segment. Apicals,
however, show a preference for progressive assimilation, as observed by Steriade
(1995, 2001). Applied to retroflexes, this means that they cause a change of the
following segment into a retroflex, or that a retroflex itself assimilates to a preceding
segment. Steriade accounts for this behaviour by the perceptual strong VC cues of
retroflexion, which override the less strong CV cues of other segments: spreading
proceeds from the segment possessing more salient place cues to the segment with
less salient cues. Thus, a retroflex followed by an apical alveolar or dental has strong
VC cues, which override the CV cues of the following apical consonant. A retroflex
preceded by an apical alveolar, on the other hand, is itself assimilated to the
preceding segment, as the VC cues of the apical alveolar are stronger than the CV
transitional cues of the retroflex, as Steriade argues.
The implications of this explanation are, however, not always met. In a
sequence of retroflex plus non-retroflex apical, the retroflex segment does not
generally determine the output of the assimilation. There are cross-linguistically a
number of cases where the retroflex is assimilated to the following apical, as in the
examples from Punjabi in (59), where the retroflex nasal assimilated to a following
dental nasal. Furthermore, retroflexes can cause regressive assimilation of preceding
dentals, as testified by the examples in Sanskrit, British English, and Vietnamese.
52
This segment is transcribed as [@] by Evans, but is referred to as retroflex continuant at a later point (p.
740).
124
Chapter 4
Sanskrit even has a process of regressive assimilation of a retroflex to a following
dental, which is exactly the opposite of Steriade’s prediction.
In sum, there are perceptual reasons why retroflexion should spread preferably
towards the following segment, but these motives are not as strong in every
language as to result in a universal pattern of progressive assimilation for
retroflexes. Local assimilation of retroflexion is in principle not restricted to
coronals, but whereas an example could be given for the assimilation of a retroflex
to a following velar, palatal or labial in colloquial Tamil, I could find no example of
a retroflex segment causing an adjacent non-coronal to turn into a retroflex. A
phonetic explanation for this gap cannot be provided here.
For non-local assimilations a different account than for local assimilation has
to be put forward, since the segments are not adjacent and thus the process can be
blocked by intervening segments. An articulatory explanation for the nati-rule in
Sanskrit was proposed by Whitney (1889: 65), who assumes that once the tip of the
tongue is in the retroflex position, it stays there to make the next nasal coronal
contact, unless a segment interferes that demands a different tongue front position,
such as another coronal. The same assumption of holding the tongue tip gesture is
made by Evans (1995) for the long distance retroflexion in the Australian language
Mpakwithi: the retroflexion of the tongue tip, which is independent of the rest of the
tongue, can be slower than the movements of the rest of the tongue. This explanation
is valid only if the intervening segment is non-coronal. Evans’ examples do not
contain intervening coronals.
Ritharngu as described by Heath has vowel retroflexion across the glottal stop
only, where the tongue tip can be held in retroflexed position during the intervening
segment. The preservation of the tongue tip gesture is thus a reasonable explanation
that can account for the data of the Australian, Indo-Aryan, and Dravidian examples.
This articulatory explanation can also account for why coronal segments block the
effect: they force the tongue tip to assume a different position and thus inhibit a
continuation of the assumed gesture. Furthermore, this accounts for the fact that
long-distance retroflexion can only apply to coronal consonants or to vowels which
can be articulated with an additional tongue retroflexion (recall section 4.4), not to
labials or velars.
No restriction on the direction of influence could be observed for long-distance
retroflexion: in Sanskrit the retroflex changed segments that occurred after it, in
Telugu segments before it.
Long-distance assimilation is observed very infrequently in languages with
retroflexes. This could be explained by the fact that long-distance assimilation, if it
indeed involves a retroflex gesture that is held over several segments, affects
intervening segments, thus the vowels between trigger and target are retroflexed.
This is probably not tolerable in most languages.
An analysis for both local and non-local retroflex assimilation is given in
section 6.3.6.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
125
4.7 Excursion: Retroflex fricatives in Toda
In section 2.2.6 the large coronal fricative system of Toda was introduced. Toda has
voiced and voiceless fricatives. The following discussion, however, is restricted to
the voiceless series. Furthermore, only the apical post-alveolar fricative, transcribed
here as a retracted alveolar [s] (following Sakthivel 1976, 1977), and the subapical
palatal fricative, transcribed as [ ], are of interest. It was illustrated in section 2.2.6
that both segments can be considered retroflex, since the apical post-alveolar is
similar to retroflex fricatives in other languages, and the subapical palatal is
retroflex under every possible definition of retroflexion. Applying the four retroflex
properties as postulated in section 2.3, the subapical palatal satisfies all four. The
apical post-alveolar also satisfies all four if we follow Ladefoged & Maddieson’s
(1986) description of this segmental class. Ladefoged (2001: 153) describes this
sound as apical alveolar with secondary velarization. According to this description,
this sound class would neither satisfy the property of posteriority nor that of
sublingual cavity, and thus not be considered a retroflex, see the discussion in
section 2.4.3. In the present section the phonological behaviour of both segmental
classes is investigated to see whether both show typical retroflex behaviour as given
in (1) of this chapter, or whether the apical post-alveolar does not. The latter case
would provide evidence for a phonetically retroflex (assuming Ladefoged &
Maddieson’s (1986) definition) but phonologically non-retroflex class.
The first indication to look at is front vowel incompatibility, because this
occurs very often with retroflex classes (cf. section 4.3). Both Toda fricatives /s^/ and
/ / occur after the short and the long high front vowel, see (62a) and (62b),
respectively, with short vowels in the left column and long vowels in the right
column (Sakthivel 1976: 69ff. and 176ff.).53
(62) (a)
(b)
kTs
i Xs
‘to crow’
‘nighttime’
kis
ki
‘handle (of spoon etc.)’
‘Mund of the Pï:r clan’
·
The occurrence of the two apical fricatives before the high front vowels seems to be
allophonic: the apical post-alveolar /s^/ occurs only before the short vowel, see (63a),
and / / only before the long vowel, see (63b) (ibid.).54
(63) (a)
(b)
nesTkj
k- i
‘rice’
‘name of a buffalo’
The examples in (62) and (63) illustrate no particularly retroflex behaviour of the
two classes with respect to front vowel incompatability.
The next process that could yield information on the phonologically retroflex
status of the apical post-alveolar fricative in Toda is palatalization. Sakthivel (1976,
1977) consistently uses the separate symbol of the palatal glide [j] to transcribe both
53
54
The first example in (62a) shows vowel lowering of the short /i/ to [T] interconsonantally (Sakthivel
1976: 50), see also the first example under (63a).
The sequence retroflex plus long high front vowel occurs in Toda only word-finally (Sakthivel 1976:
75f.).
126
Chapter 4
the phoneme as well as what others (for instance Emeneau 1984) transcribe as
secondary palatalization. I will follow Sakthivel’s transcription because I find an
incompatibility of retroflexes and secondary palatalization well-supported (as
elaborated in 2.5 and 4.3.2.5). The palatal glide occurs only after the apical postalveolar /s^/, see the examples in (64) (Sakthivel 1976: 216 and 219), not after the
subapical palatal.
(64)
.sjam
isj
‘exactly’
‘rat’
Interestingly palatalization occurs after all other subapical palatals in Toda apart
from the fricative, see the examples in (65).
(65)
pTjk
oj
nT j
kTj
‘in vain’
‘nail’
‘Mund of the Niry clan’
·
‘parrot’
As shown in section 2.2.6, the Toda fricatives are the largest coronal series in this
language, the subapical stop contrasts only with a dental and an alveolar, and the
nasal, rhotic, and lateral only with an alveolar. The occurrence of palatalization with
all very retroflex consonants except the very retroflex fricative may indicate that the
stop, nasal, trill, and lateral have a lesser degree of retroflexion, and are presumably
articulated more like the apical post-alveolar fricatives, which might be due to the
smaller stop, nasal, trill, and lateral inventories (cf. section 2.2.6). This suggestion
has to be further investigated.
The palatalization processes therefore make a distinction between the palatal
subapical fricative, which behaves in this respect like retroflexes in other languages,
and the apical post-alveolar fricative, which does not. The sequence palatal glide
plus fricative, however, unites the two classes again: the glide occurs neither before
the subapical palatal nor before the apical post-alveolar fricative (but it occurs
before other coronal fricatives). This is illustrated e.g. by the fact that the genitive
marker –s changes to a [] if added to a word ending with a palatal glide, see (66)
(Sakthivel 1977: 44f.).
input
(66) &j – s
poj – s
mud p-_j – s
output
&j
poj
mud p-_j
gloss
‘in the pot’
‘in the mouth’
‘in three dairies’
Unfortunately, there is no morpheme with an initial – that could illustrate a
corresponding change of the fricative. The argumentation of an incompatibility of
tongue gestures for retroflex and palatal that was applied to account for the nonoccurrence of palatalized retroflexes can be used here to account for the nonoccurrence of glide-retroflex sequences.
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
127
The phonotactics of the two fricative classes show no difference, but some
behaviour in line with retroflexes (see (1e)): neither the apico-post-alveolar nor the
subapical palatal fricative occurs in word-initial position (Sakthivel 1976: 56ff.).
Summing up these observations, the apical post-alveolars behave in some
respects phonologically like retroflexes (for instance in its non-occurrence after the
palatal glide), in others not (for instance it occurs with a following glide, and in high
front vowel context). This result can interpreted in two ways, namely that it is
possible to have (i) phonetically and phonologically two retroflex classes, or (ii) a
phonetically retroflex, but phonologically non-retroflex class. In the first case, the
two retroflex segmental classes nevertheless have to be phonologically
distinguished, which is unproblematic if one departs from the traditional feature
description of retroflexes as [coronal, –anterior, –distributed], as argued in section
5.2.3 below.
4.8 Conclusion
In this chapter it was illustrated that retroflex segments show cross-linguistically and
diachronically homogeneous behaviour with respect to processes such as a
preference of back vowel context, incompatibility of front vowel context, and so on.
Furthermore, it was shown that this behaviour is phonetically motivated. and that
this phonetic grounding can account for the similarity in synchronic rules and
diachronic emergence of retroflexes.
Several of these processes have a preference for a specific order: rhotics
causing retroflexion usually occur in front of the apical to be retroflexed, and front
vowels that are changed in retroflex context are also pre-consonantal. Generalizing
away from single processes, retroflexes preferably occur post-vocalically and affect
preceding segments. This observation can be accounted for in several ways. First of
all, the asymmetrical spread of cues as described in 3.5, with VC cues being stronger
than the CV cues of retroflexes, is a cause for a restriction on the occurrence and the
direction of influence. Furthermore, studies such as Krakow (1999) show a
difference in gestural overlap between a consonant and a preceding vowel compared
to a consonant following a vowel: the gestures are much better synchronized in CV
gestures, but overlap for VC gestures. The gesture of retroflexion hence influences
the preceding vowels, not the following ones.
With respect to the manner of the retroflexes involved in the different
processes, some further generalizations can be made. If a single manner class and
not the whole class of retroflexes is the trigger or the target of a process, then it is
often a retroflex approximant that is changed due to its articulatory instability.
Retroflex fricatives, on the other hand, often cause adjacent segments to change to a
retroflex, because of their strong internal cues.
Noteworthy in the descriptions in this chapter is the similarity between
retroflex segments and rhotics, see the restricted distribution of vowels in front of
128
Chapter 4
both (section 4.3),55 and the retroflexion of both vowels (section 4.4) and consonants
(section 4.1) in rhotic context. These processes are not restricted to apical rhotics,
but can partly also be triggered by uvular rhotics, which indicates that it is not a
common articulatory property between retroflexes and coronal rhotics (such as
tongue tip articulation) but rather a common perceptual characteristic, namely the
low F3, that accounts for the common restrictions.
The illustration of a cross-linguistically similar behaviour of the class of
retroflexes is not taken as evidence for a universally valid category of retroflexes, let
alone, for the innateness of features determining such a category. The rules and
restrictions observed in this chapter fall out of the definition of the retroflex class in
the preceding chapters 2 and 3. Due to the articulatory criteria of posteriority,
apicality, retraction, and sublingual cavity, the so-defined class shares the same
articulatory restrictions. Avoidance of front vowels and occurrence in back vowel
context, for example, is due to apicality and retraction of the segments (as attested
by the fact that other, non-anterior apicals share this property, cf. Bhat 1973). Thus,
any segment that satisfies the articulatory criteria proposed in chapter 2 is bound to
show this articulatory behaviour. Likewise, the retroflex class was defined as
sharing the acoustic features of high/middle F2 and low F3, which account for their
perceptual similarity with rhotics and back vowels.
The apical post-alveolar and the subapical palatal in Toda were both shown in
section 4.7 to meet the articulatory properties of retroflexion and also to behave
partly as retroflexes. If one universal category were assumed, then a language that
has two segmental classes both belonging to one universal category would be
problematic and impossible to account for.
Several points that were described in the present chapter are worth future
research. For the process of retroflexion after rhotics (section 4.1), the question
arises whether non-retroflex, non-coronal rhotics can really trigger retroflexion, too.
This could be checked by a detailed investigation of the Southern Swedish dialect of
Svantesson (2001) to decide whether its retracted alveolar segments fulfil the four
articulatory properties of retroflexes developed here in chapter 2. If these segments
turn out to be retroflex, this dialect will be an example language for retroflexion in
non-coronal rhotic context.
Another point for further study is the diachronic development of retroflex
segments from Classical Tibetan to Modern Sino-Tibetan languages (also section
4.1) and the question whether assimilation of velars and labials to coronals took
place before retroflexion via rhotics occurred, or whether the velars and labials were
retroflexed directly.
Concerning the affinity with back vowels illustrated in section 4.2 and the
vowel-lowering processes from section 4.3, more diachronic and synchronic
evidence for the articulatory similarity between retroflexes and mid back vowels
rather than high back vowels is hoped to be found in the future.
55
An exception to this generalization is Scottish English, which can have all vowels before a rhotic
(Harris 1994: 255).
Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding
129
The retroflexed vowels of Badaga, which illustrated vowel retroflexion in
section 4.4, have to be investigated articulatorily to determine whether they include
high front retroflexed vowels or whether the gap in Emeneau’s (1939) data was not
accidental.
Quite a number of retroflexes showed a dependence on segmental length or
prosodic position, a point that was not the focus of the present study but which
might yield interesting insights for prosodic theories.
Lastly, I hope that confirming evidence for the processes of retroflexion via
velarization and rounding that were left undiscussed here, will be found as well.
5
Phonological representations of retroflexes
This chapter is concerned with formal phonological descriptions of retroflexes and
the notion of phonological features. In the structure of the thesis, it is situated as
follows: Chapter 4 illustrated cross-linguistic, phonological processes of retroflexion
and de-retroflexion, and their phonetic grounding. The topic of the present chapter is
a featural representation of retroflex phonemes that can account for these processes.
A formal representation of retroflexion and de-retroflexion processes by means of
these features is then presented in chapter 6.
In the present chapter I will argue that neither the traditional features
[+coronal, –anterior, –distributed], nor their alternative within the framework of
Feature Geometry (usually [CORONAL, –anterior, –distributed, +back]) suffice to account for the perceptually and articulatorily motivated processes described in chapter 4.
More phonetically oriented approaches in the framework of Optimality Theory, which separate articulatory and auditory representations and features, are invoked, and it is demonstrated that this separation is actually required to handle all of
the data given in chapter 4. It will be claimed that the major shortcomings of almost
all of the previous approaches is the requirement that features express natural
classes. Following Pierrehumbert, Beckman & Ladd (1996) and Boersma (1998,
1999), it will be proposed that features are only universal in as far as they are
grounded in the human vocal tract anatomy, the hearing system, and the mental capacity to categorize. Thus, I will assume no universally valid system of phonological
features. Furthermore, I will argue that the attempt to differentiate phonological
features and phonetic cues is often futile, and that phonetic cues are directly relevant
for phonological processes and should therefore be able to function as features. A
third point argued for here is that defining ‘natural classes’ that hold crosslinguistically is not a function of features, but a descriptive device for linguists that
facilitates the comparison of language-specific processes across different languages.
The structure of this chapter is as follows. In section 5.1 the function of distinctive features in earlier approaches and in the present approach is defined. Section
5.2 presents the representation of retroflexes in the featural systems of Jakobson,
Fant and Halle (1952), Chomsky and Halle (1968), and the theory of Feature Geometry. More phonetically oriented approaches to distinctive features are discussed
in section 5.3. In section 5.4, I propose my own approach including perceptual features and restrictions on their articulatory realizations, based largely on Boersma’s
(1998) Functional Phonology. Section 5.5 concludes the chapter. No complete discussion and overview of the traditional and the phonetically oriented approaches is
aimed at, instead the main concepts of these theories are presented with a focus on
the featural representations proposed for retroflexes (even in this respect no attempt
at completeness is made).
132
Chapter 5
5.1 The functions of features
Before discussing the features (or, alternatively, cues) that are used for the representation of retroflexes in former approaches and in more recent ones, we have to
clarify the function of phonological features. It is generally accepted that features
are necessary to represent phonological processes in a language. I will term this the
representational function of features. Furthermore, features have the classificatory
function of encoding contrasts between segments in natural languages, which results
in natural classes: segments that share a feature belong to a natural class. Evidence
for such classes comes from phoneme inventories in natural languages and also from
phonological rules referring to the feature and hence to these classes. An example
illustrating these two functions of features is the feature [voice] which classifies obstruents as either voiced or voiceless. A number of languages such as Dutch and
German show a process of final devoicing by which the voiced sonorants are realized as voiceless in the coda-position. This rule is evidence for the existence of the
feature [voice] since only this feature is changed in the process of final devoicing,
whereas all others are maintained.
Many authors (such as Chomsky & Halle 1968, and Ladefoged 1971, 1975)
claim that a further function of phonological features is to ensure phonetic interpretability of the segments. Our example feature [voice] can be phonetically interpreted
as vibration of the vocal folds for [+voice] segments among other phonetic correlates. The three functions are summarized in (1), together with the domain in which
they apply.
(1)
i. representation
ii. classification
iii. phonetic interpretability
language-specific
cross-linguistic
language-specific
Lahiri & Blumstein (1984:133) state that “features provide the interface between the physical attributes of the speech sounds and their functional attributes in
the phonological systems of natural language”. From this position, we can derive a
structure as in figure 5.1, see Lindau & Ladefoged (1986) and Nearey (1995: 29) on
similar representations.
segments
S1
phonological features
(and feature values)
αF1
phonetic gestures/cues
[c1, c2,
αF2
S2
αF3
c3, c6]
αF1
αF2
αF3
[c3, c4, c5, c6, c7]
Figure 5.1 Schematic representation of relation segment – features – gestures/cues in traditional view.
According to this view, segments are made up of features, which in turn are correlated with acoustic cues and/or articulatory gestures, that is, with their physical attributes. Nearey (1995) calls the relation between the phonetic properties and the
Phonological representations of retroflexes
133
phonological features represented in his figure 3.2, which is similar to my figure 5.1,
many-to-many, since several attributes can be associated with one feature and several features with one attribute, opposed to a one-to-one relation represented by Stevens & Blumstein (1981), where one feature is associated with one physical attribute. The latter view, which is very restricted, will not be further dealt with in this
dissertation.
The representation of both the articulatory gestures and perceptual cues on one
level in figure 5.1 is a simplification. In standard featural theories, gestures are assumed to be the primary correlates of the phonological features, from which perceptual cues are derived.
Furthermore, in standard featural approaches phonological features are universal and innate, that is, part of the Universal Grammar, and language learners use
those features from the restricted innate set that are active in their language. All
segments in all natural languages can be described with the same set of features. As
Halle (1992: 207) puts it, “features, rather than speech sounds, are the ultimate
building blocks of language”. The phonetic realization of these features, however,
i.e. the correlation between features and cues, is language-specific according to
standard belief because languages differ in the way they phonetically realize featural
contrasts.
Phonological features and their phonetic realizations differ not only in their
claim to universality. Phonetic cues have a temporal structure within the segment, a
stop burst or instance is situated after the stop closure and before the CV transitions,
and cues do not necessarily hold for the whole duration of a segment. Some cues can
even span two segments, consider the case of vowel transitions. Features, on the
other hand, generally apply to the whole segment, although since the advent of
Autosegmental Phonology (Goldsmith 1976) features are assumed to project their
own tier, and thus can be assigned to a segment, a syllable, a phonological word, and
so on. Units smaller than the segment (for instance single cues such as the silence of
the stop closure)1 are not accessible for autosegmental features.2
The search for universal features has been one of the main aims in
phonological theory in the last half century. Cues, however, have only recently become a focus of interest for phonologists, for example in the rise of laboratory phonology (Kingston & Beckman 1990, Docherty & Ladd 1992, Connell & Arvaniti
1995, and others). Some of these more recent approaches suggest that the distinction
between cues or gestures and features (or phonetic and phonological features in Steriade 2000) is superfluous, and that the two are the same. If so, a correlation as in
figure 5.2 follows.
1
2
The stop closure could be a useful autosegment in the description of stop gemination processes, since
only the silence of the stop closure is stretched/doubled in geminates, while all other cues such as the
stop release burst or vowel transitions are not affected by this process.
The mora is a counterexample to this claim since it is a unit sometimes smaller than the segment that
can be an autosegment. A mora, however, is an abstract descriptive unit without concrete physical
correlates.
134
Chapter 5
segments
cues/gestures = features
S1
S2
[c1, c2, c3, c6]
[c3, c4, c5, c6, c7]
Figure 5.2 Schematic representation of relation segment – cues in phonetic approaches.
Phonologists such as Steriade (1995, 1997, 2001), Flemming (1995/2002, 1997),
Boersma (1998, 2001), and others have shown in their work that phonological patterns can be explained by directly referring to the “details of their physical implementation” (Steriade 2000: 314). Their proposals will be discussed in sections 5.3.3
and 5.3.4. I will argue below that figure 5.2 is correct and that there is no need for
traditional universal features as represented in figure 5.1.
5.2 Featural representations
The concept of features distinguishing the sounds of a language was initially developed by Jakobson and Trubetzkoy. According to Anderson (1985: 119), Trubetzkoy’s Grundzüge der Phonologie (1939) can be described as the first attempt to provide “a universal framework of the features that are exploited for phonological purposes in the languages of the world as opposed to purely descriptive frameworks.”
The aim of Trubetzkoy’s features is, however, to distinguish segments within a language, not to develop a universal feature system. The first full-fledged system of
features was proposed by Jakobson, Fant and Halle (1952), and is outlined in section
5.2.1 below. The next influential feature system was developed by Chomsky and
Halle’s (1968), discussed in section 5.2.2. Both systems share the idea of the segment as an unordered bundle of features, which was replaced in later theories by hierarchical relations between features (to be discussed in section 5.2.3).
5.2.1 Jakobson, Fant, & Halle (1952)
Jakobson, Fant & Halle (1952) and Jakobson & Halle (1956) introduced a universal
system of twelve distinctive features that were defined both acoustically and articulatorily. Most of these features refer to sounds which do not form a class in articulatory terms. The feature [grave], for example, specifies labial and velar consonants
as well as back vowels. [grave] sounds show a low frequency concentration in the
acoustic spectrum and are formed with a relatively large, undivided oral resonant
cavity. The Jakobsonian features are unary, but as most of them have a complement
feature that comprises all segments not included by that feature, for example [acute]
being the complement of [grave], they can be reformulated as binary features (e.g.
[±grave], where [−grave] stands for [acute]). The feature [flat] is an example of a
feature that has more than one counterpart: non-flat sounds can be either [sharp] or
[plain]. Sharp sounds show an upward shift in the upper frequencies of the spectrum
and are articulated with a broad pharynx. The feature [sharp] is restricted to secondarily palatalized consonants, but does not include palatal consonants. [plain] refers
Phonological representations of retroflexes
135
to sounds that have a relatively closed pharynx and no shift in the upper frequencies.
It comprises non-sharp and non-flat sounds.
Jakobson et al.’s work was written in the structuralist framework, and the main
idea of this feature system is to specify phonemic contrast between segments. In this
system, coronals are represented as [acute], which describes sounds that involve a
division of the oral cavity into two smaller cavities, resulting in a prominent high
frequency region in the acoustic spectrum. According to this definition, retroflex
segments belong to the acute sounds as their post-alveolar constriction results in two
resonators in the oral cavity (recall also the short description of [acute], [diffuse],
and [flat] presented in section 3.1.7). The acute-grave distinction is cross-classified
by the features [diffuse] and [compact]. Retroflex consonants, like all apical sounds,
are specified by the feature [diffuse], which describes a constriction in the front of
the oral cavity. Diffuse sounds do not have a spectral concentration of energy. Its
opposite is [compact], defined as referring to sounds with a constriction in the back
of the oral cavity and with an energy concentration in the central region of the spectrum.
As demonstrated in chapter 3 on the acoustics of retroflexes, retroflexion results in a downward shift of the upper frequencies in the spectrum. In Jakobson et
al.’s terms, this is specified by the feature [flat]. The same acoustic effect of ‘flatness’ can be achieved by lip-rounding, velarization or pharyngealization, which illustrates that the feature [flat] is only an additional specification and cannot contrast
a retroflex and a rounded segment with otherwise identical features. In sum, retroflex segments are [acute, diffuse] in Jakobson et al.’s terminology, and an additional
specification by the feature [flat] is possible if necessary for further distinction. This
is, for example, the case for those Australian languages with two laminals and two
apicals such as Pitta-Pitta (Dixon 1980: 134). A feature specification for such a language according to Jakobson et al. is depicted in table 5.1.
Table 5.1
Jakobson, Fant & Halle’s (1952) features applied to a language with a four-way
coronal distinction.
[compact]
[diffuse]
[acute]
[grave]
[flat]
p
t
t
+
+
+
+
+
+
+
+
+
t()
k
+
+
+
+
+
The feature system of Jakobson et al. is insufficient for describing and distinguishing all four coronals plus labial and velar place of articulation: as table 5.1 illustrates, the features do not distinguish between dentals and alveolars, which have
identical feature specifications. The retroflex class can only be distinguished from
the dentals and alveolars by the feature [flat], which is therefore a necessary specification in languages with large inventories.
136
Chapter 5
Jassem (1979) uses Jakobson et al.’s features successfully for the characterization of Polish fricatives. Shalev et al. (1993), however, show that this feature system
is not sufficient for the description of the large fricative inventory of Toda (recall
this inventory from (5) in section 2.2.4.2). For another application of the Jakobson et
al. feature system see the discussion of Hamilton (1996) in section 5.3.2 below.
5.2.2 Chomsky & Halle (1968)
In The Sound Pattern of English (henceforth SPE), Chomsky & Halle (1968) introduce a system of features which is based exclusively on articulation,3 and which includes reformulations of Jakobson et al.’s features in articulatory terms. The former
classification of grave-acute sounds, for example, is carried out by the feature [±coronal] in SPE, where the definition of [+coronal] does not refer to the acoustic property of prominent high frequencies but to the articulation with the tongue tip or
blade. This formulation results in slightly different natural classes as [−coronal] includes palatals which were not included in the feature [grave] (see table 5.1).4 As in
Jakobson et al.’s proposal, the more than 20 features in SPE are proposed for classificatory purposes. But in contrast to previous approaches, SPE features also have a
direct phonetic function: they are specifically designed to describe ‘the speech producing capabilities of the human vocal apparatus’ (SPE: 297). Thus, SPE aimed at a
phonetic grounding of universal features in the articulatory system.
Retroflexes are classified as [+coronal] in the SPE system (SPE: 304). Coronal
and non-coronal sounds are supplemented by the feature [anterior]: every place of
articulation from the lips up to the alveolar region is [+anterior]. Hence retroflexes
are [−anterior]. For a further distinction of [+coronal] sounds in the post-alveolar region, as for instance needed for Australian languages, the feature [distributed] is
used: “distributed sounds are produced with a constriction that extends for a considerable distance along the direction of the air flow” (SPE: 312). Retroflexes, which
are articulated with a short length of constriction, are [−distributed]. The full specification with SPE features for a language with four coronals is given in table 5.2.
Table 5.2
SPE features applied to a language with a four-way coronal distinction
[coronal]
[anterior]
[distributed]
3
4
p
t
t
t
k
−
+
+
+
+
+
+
+
−
+
−
−
+
−
+
−
−
+
The only exception seems to be the feature [strident], defined as sounds that are marked acoustically
by greater noisiness than their nonstrident counterparts (SPE: 329).
Furthermore, uvulars and pharyngeals are unspecified for [grave] in Jakobson et al. but are [-coronal]
in SPE. Keating (1988a: 4), however, points out that, according to the definitions in Jakobson et al.,
uvulars and pharyngeals are plausibly [+grave].
For a discussion of the advantage of retaining [grave] instead of [coronal] see Hyman (1973) and Odden (1978).
Phonological representations of retroflexes
137
In contrast to the terms referring to the articulator, the feature ‘distributed’ is not restricted to consonants articulated with the tongue tip only, but is also used to distinguish between labials [+distributed] and labiodentals [−distributed], and between
velars and uvulars. As Keating (1988a) points out, this results from the tendency in
SPE to use one feature to distinguish as many sounds as possible.5
Chomsky & Halle discuss Ladefoged (1964), who notes a difference between
the retroflex segments of Ewe and the retroflex sounds found in the languages of the
Indian subcontinent. SPE comments on the consequences of these inventories for the
phonological representation:
If this difference were systematic, it would clearly have to be reflected in the grammars
of these languages. It is, however, quite sufficient to note that the point of contact between the tongue and the roof of the mouth is somewhat more advanced in one language than the other. This fact would presumably be reflected in low-level phonetic
rules that assign numerical values to the different features. The existence of a systematic phonetic difference does not, therefore, in itself constitute a necessary and sufficient condition for postulating an additional point of articulation. (SPE: 313)
The standard view at that time of distinguishing universal phonological features
from language-specific phonetic interpretation rules becomes obvious in this quotation. Only if a language were found to phonologically contrast both retroflex articulations, would a universal feature to distinguish the two articulations be introduced.
The reduction of features to just those which are phonologically relevant, and
the optimal use of features for several distinctions at the same time (as in the case of
[distributed]) often conflicts with the aim of including in the feature system the phonetic restrictions of the vocal apparatus. Some of the resulting shortcomings are illustrated in the following section on Feature Geometry.
5.2.3 Feature Geometry
The theory of Feature Geometry (henceforth FG, early key literature is: Clements
1985, Sagey 1986, McCarthy 1988) will be discussed in relatively great detail as it
offers several possible accounts of the representation of retroflexes. It will be shown
that none of them can capture all three functions of features as outlined in (1), especially the claim of universal applicability is problematic.
This section is subdivided as follows: In 5.2.3.1, the basic notions of FG are
given, section 5.2.3.2 illustrates the different proposals on how to deal with vowel
interactions of retroflexes, and section 5.2.3.3 briefly describes why FG fails to fulfil
its aims in the areas under investigation.
5.2.3.1 Basic notions
FG basically introduces a hierarchical structure to Chomsky and Halle’s features.
Whereas SPE uses the binary features [±coronal] and [±anterior] to classify all
5
One shortcoming of such an approach is that the features that are maximally used do not necessarily
reflect natural classes, as is for instance the case for the class of [±anterior] sounds, see section 5.2.3.2
below.
138
Chapter 5
sounds according to their place of articulation, FG introduces the privative place
nodes LABIAL, CORONAL, and DORSAL (under a common P LACE node)6, which correspond to three articulators. These three articulatory features dominate other features,
as in figure 5.3 below. The reason for positing privative features is that the classes
made up of the negative value of the SPE features ‘coronal’ and ‘anterior’ have been
argued not to form a natural class. The class of [−coronal] sounds, including labials,
palatals and velars, is now split up into the LABIAL node (formerly specified as
[–coronal +anterior]) and the DORSAL node (formerly [–coronal –anterior]). According to Hume (1994: 21), this description with (privative) nodes “expresses more
accurately natural phenomena such as assimilation and the behaviour of complex
segments”. Place assimilation for example can be expressed in FG as the spreading
of the PLACE node. Furthermore, the SPE features ‘distributed’ and ‘anterior’ are
now restricted to the coronal node and therefore receive a different interpretation.
The new value [−distributed] now stands for an apical articulation, [+distributed] for
a laminal, which led some phonologists to propose the replacement of [±distributed]
by the privatives ‘apical’ and ‘laminal’ (see Gnanadesikan 1994, Hamilton 1993,
1996, Walsh Dickey 1997).
Figure 5.3 from Clements (1991: 28f.) illustrates the hierarchically structured
features within his FG version.
consonantal
sonorant
[nasal]
[voiced]
LARYNGEAL
ORAL CAVITY
[spread glottis] PLACE
LABIAL
CORONAL
[continuant]
DORSAL
[rounded] [anterior] [distributed] [back] [high]
Figure 5.3
Hierarchical ordering of features according to FG in Clements (1991).
Besides the hierarchical ordering, FG also incorporates the idea of non-linearity of
features developed in autosegmental phonology (Goldsmith 1976). Features are arranged on their own tier and can apply if necessary to a sequence of sounds, a whole
syllable or a phonological word instead of just the segment.
In FG retroflexes are usually specified as [CORONAL , −anterior, −distributed]
(as in Clements 1991). The values for the features [anterior] and [distributed] for
coronal consonants are not different from those in SPE terms, compare table 5.3 to
table 5.2 above, but receive a different interpretation, as elaborated above.
6
is often assumed to be an additional node under PLACE, see for instance Pulleyblank (1988),
who proposes it for the specification of segments that are articulated with the tongue root, such as [
]. Clements (1991) includes the feature [low] under the DORSAL node as well, but as that is not
common it is left out here.
RADICAL
Phonological representations of retroflexes
Table 5.3
139
FG representation of a language with a four way coronal distinction
[CORONAL]
[anterior]
[distributed]
t
t
t
√
+
+
√
+
−
√
−
−
√
−
+
In table 5.3 and below ‘√’ denotes the presence of a privative feature.
Within FG, several proposals were put forward especially for the representation of coronals and their interaction with vowels, particularly in literature such as
Pulleyblank (1989) and Hume (1992). The following subsection describes these
proposals and their consequences for the representation of retroflexes.
5.2.3.2 Proposals for retroflexes patterning with vowels
Chapter 4 illustrated that retroflexes show an affinity to back vowels and a dispreference for front vowels. To account for these correlations, many authors propose one
feature with the same value for back vowels and retroflexes and an opposite value
for front vowels. The proposed feature is usually [back], although some phonologists (such as Pulleyblank 1989) propose [front]. These features are either under a
separate dorsal node or they replace one of the coronal features [distributed] or [anterior], see (2) for a list of the proposals.
(2)
(a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
(e)
additional DORSAL feature (Sagey 1986, Pulleyblank 1989)
retroflexes as language-dependently [±back] (Hall 1997)
replacement of [distributed] by [back] (Lin 1989, Rubach 1984)
replacement of [distributed] by [front] (Pulleyblank 1989)
replacement of [anterior] by [back] (Gnanadesikan 1993)
An additional DORSAL feature [back] for coronal segments, see (2a), is used by
Sagey (1986) and others to indicate secondary articulations: [+back] coronals are
velarized, and [−back] coronals are palatalized. According to this definition, retroflexes should be specified as [+back], because they have a retracted tongue body, i.e.
they are velarized. This representation is, however, not obligatory, as pointed out by
Hall (1997: 49f.). Hall therefore proposes to distinguish non-velarized from velarized retroflex segments by representing the latter with an additional feature [+back],
see (2b). The two different representations are given in (3).
(3)
[] = [CORONAL, -anterior, -distributed]
[ ] = [CORONAL, -anterior, -distributed, +back]
According to Hall, the choice between these two segment classes is languagespecific. Such a representation implies that only velarized retroflexes show an affinity with back vowels. The representation proposed by Hall is based on the assumption that languages such as Lardil have non-velarized retroflexes. But in chapter 2 I
illustrated that retroflexes are generally retracted. The two possible counterexamples
of Lardil and Badaga were shown in section 2.3.4 actually not to have non-velarized
retroflexes. Thus, I argue here that the distinction proposed by Hall is invalid.
140
Chapter 5
Rubach (1984), Lin (1989: 61), and others propose the replacement of [distributed] by [back], see (2c), which has the consequence that retroflexes are CORONAL
[+back] instead of [−distributed]. The resulting natural classes are exactly the same
as in the classical FG approach, see table 5.4 vs. 5.3.
Table 5.4
Featural representation with [back] instead of [distributed].
[CORONAL]
[anterior]
[back]
t
t
t
i
u
√
+
−
√
+
+
√
−
+
√
−
−
√
√
−
+
Front vowels can now be represented by the coronal feature [−back], and back vowels by [+back], ensuring the correlation with retroflexes. But whereas [distributed]
was defined by the length of the constriction, [+back] is defined as a raised tongue
back, i.e. it has the phonetic correlate of velarization. The feature replacement thus
implies that all apical sounds have a raised tongue body, an assumption that is an
overgeneralization as apical alveolars can be non-velarized, recall the discussion of
apicals and retraction in section 2.3.4.
The same classes that are predicted by the coronal feature [back] also result
from Pulleyblank’s (1989) proposal to replace [distributed] with [front], see (2d). In
addition, Pulleyblank does not have the feature [anterior], but instead introduces the
DORSAL feature [high] to further distinguish coronal sounds, as illustrated in table
5.5.
Table 5.5
Featural representation according to Pulleyblank (1989).
[CORONAL]
[front]
[DORSAL]
[high]
t
t
t
i
u
√
+
√
√
−
√
√
−
√
+
√
+
√
+
√
+
√
−
The feature [front] has the opposite values of the feature [back], but as the values
plus and minus do not have any implication for the resulting classes, this fact can be
ignored. As front vowels are [+front] and back vowels and retroflexes [–front], the
interactions of vowels and retroflexes can be captured in this system. Phonetically,
[+front] is realized as a tongue front raising, which is not present in retroflexion,
thus a specification for retroflexes as [–front] is appropriate. The DORSAL feature
[+high] is not further defined by Pulleyblank. Its restriction within the coronals to
retroflexes and palato-alveolars is unmotivated.7 Phonetically, these two sound
classes do not share a shape of the tongue body distinguishing them from the other
7
Pulleyblank’s restriction of [+high] to retroflexes and palato-alveolars implies that these classes are
structurally more marked than alveolars and dentals, which is supported by Pulleyblank’s remarks on
the allophonic behaviour of coronals in Australian languages (p. 385).
Phonological representations of retroflexes
141
two coronals, they have only a post-alveolar place of articulation in common. Phonologically, there is no evidence for this class, either. Both the proposal by Pulleyblank and those by Rubach and Lin described in the paragraph above run into a
problem with the representation of the nati rule in Sanskrit (see section 4.6.2): with
traditional features, the retroflexion of an apical nasal after a (non-adjacent) retroflex
fricative can be described as a spreading of [–anterior] from one [–distributed] segment, the retroflex, to the other, the nasal. Intervening [+anterior] segments block
the process. If, however, apicals and retroflexes were specified by [back] or [front]
instead of [–distributed], then the long-distance assimilation of the nati rule would
be blocked by other segments that are specified by these features such as velars.
Another proposal to formalize the interaction of coronals with vowels is to replace [anterior] by [back], see (2e) (Gnanadesikan 1993). This proposal hinges on
the claim that the feature [anterior] does not describe a natural class. For a discussion of the (non-)naturalness of the classes yielded by [+anterior] and [–anterior] see
McCarthy (1988), Pulleyblank (1989), and Hall (1997: 45f.). A feature specification
for coronals resulting from the replacement of [anterior] by [back] looks like the
following, where dentals and alveolars are unspecified for the feature [back].
Table 5.6
Featural representation without [anterior] (Gnanadesikan 1993).
[CORONAL]
[distributed]
[back]
t
t
t
i
u
√
+
√
−
√
−
+
√
+
−
√
√
−
+
In this representation, front vowels could be specified as [−back] and back vowels as
[+back] to formalize the interaction with the retroflex class. Like Pulleyblank’s approach, a representation such as that in table 5.6 also presupposes that laminal dentals and apical alveolars are structurally less marked than retroflexes and laminal
palato-alveolars (see footnote 7). A universal assumption such as this is, however,
problematic: recall for instance that retroflexes did not show phonotactic markedness in all languages, see section 4.5.2.
In sum, none of the proposals discussed above satisfies all three functions of
features. Some proposals are phonetically non-interpretable, others predict nonoccurring classes. Pulleyblank’s feature value [+high] for retroflexes and palatoalveolars is an example of both: the phonetic equivalent of [+high] is not clear, plus
the two sound classes do not share a tongue gesture that could be referred to by this
feature. The majority of proposals fails to be universally valid, such as the DORSAL
specifications of retroflexes as proposed by Pulleyblank, Sagey, and others, or the
representation of apical alveolars as universally velarized, as in Lin and Rubach.
5.2.3.3 Why does FG fail?
As shown in the previous section, there is no consensus on how to represent retroflex segments in a feature geometric approach. Different proposals are made depending on the language or language group under scrutiny. This discrepancy in the
142
Chapter 5
presentation hinges on four major flaws of FG, all stemming from the three-fold
function assumed for features: the assumption that there is one universal hierarchy
(classificatory function), the irreconcilability of this universality with languagespecific processes (conflict between classificatory and representational function),
and the restriction to articulatory features and over-simplification of the dependency
relations in the vocal apparatus (both miscomprehensions of the function of phonetic
interpretability).
Let us look at the universality of features first. The fact that no single geometrical arrangement of features seems to hold for all languages was pointed out by
Mester (1986): “some properties of Feature Geometry, in particular dependency relations between features, vary from one language to another”. This problem can be
solved in two possible ways. The first one is to assume a universal order of features,
without all features being employed or activated in all languages. This approach is
put forward by Clements (1999, 2001) who posits tiers that are not relevant in every
language, but are inactively present. The activation of relevant tiers is done via the
“Prominence Criterion”: in any language, all and only prominent features and nodes
are projected onto separate autosegmental tiers. This idea is compatible with the notion of parameters, which decide on the presence or absence of features and their
combination (compare Cho, 1991 who introduces the parameter [COR] or [−cor]).
Even this alternation runs into problems with languages such as Toda with a large
coronal fricative system that contains two retroflex classes, see sections 2.2.6 and
3.3.2.3. Since the features [distributed] and [anterior] and their articulatory definitions yield no way of distinguishing between an apical post-alveolar and an subapical palatal articulation (both classes are [–anterior, –distributed]), the Toda classes
cannot be distinguished by these traditional features.
A second solution to the problems following from universal features is to assume that phonological features are not innate, but that they show cross-linguistic
similarity due to the restrictions of the vocal and hearing apparatus, and the classificatory abilities of the human. This functional approach is represented by Boersma
(1998), illustrated in section 5.3.4 below. According to this functional view, the articulatory and perceptual systems are universal, but a phonological use of them is
language-dependent: hence a phonological theory built on universal features is
bound to fail.
A further point of criticism is that the features employed in FG are almost only
based on articulation, as illustrated by Flemming (1995). This articulatory bias is
problematic. Some phonological processes are triggered by perceptual similarity,
which is not captured in an articulatory feature system; this was pointed out for instance by Ohala (1989, 1993) in a description of perceptual cues playing a role in
sound change. The problem of the general restriction to articulatory features becomes obvious with the re-introduction of Jakobson et al.’s perceptual feature
[grave], which combines velars and labials as sounds with an energy concentration
in the low frequency area, as the articulatorily defined feature [peripheral] under the
PLACE node (Dogil 1988, Avery & Rice 1989, Rice 1994, Hall 1997). Articulatory
and perceptual features are both necessary in an account of phonological processes,
Phonological representations of retroflexes
143
as will be illustrated for the class of retroflexes in chapter 6. Furthermore, the two
sets of features have to be distinguished in representations. Proposals along this line
and their advantages are discussed in section 5.3 below.
In addition to being restricted to articulatory features, FG shows an oversimplification of the vocal tract anatomy and introduces some pseudo-articulatory nodes
which are not motivated from an articulatory point of view. Ohala (1992: 178f.) is
one of those who pointed out that the interaction of the glottal and the supraglottal
domain is far more complex from an aerodynamic view than is presented in FG.
Furthermore, the division of the tongue into CORONAL and DORSAL (and sometimes
RADICAL) nodes has to be questioned. Coronal and dorsal articulations are treated as
independent from each other, and so are anterior and non-anterior coronals. It has
been illustrated in chapter 2 for retroflexes that this class shows a very complex interaction of the front, middle and back of the tongue: the raising of the tongue tip
causes a lowering of the middle and a retraction of the back of the tongue, which accounts for some of the phonological behaviour of retroflexes (as illustrated in chapter 4). This interaction has to be represented in FG by referring to two different articulator nodes. In addition, the border between the coronal and dorsal classes is not
well motivated. A long-standing debate is, for instance, the status of palatal consonants and whether they are coronal or not (see Hall 1997 for an overview of this discussion). Phonetic and phonological evidence was also brought forward for a complex articulation of palato-alveolars with both coronal area and dorsum involved, see
for example Recasens et al. (1995) and Gussenhoven & Jacobs (1998: 199f.). A possible account of such interrelations of the tongue movements is proposed in 5.4.2
below.
A further point of criticism concerning the oversimplification of FG is that the
articulators are all dependents of the place node. The only need for the place node is
to explain assimilation processes, where one place of articulation is replaced by another, depicted as spreading and deletion of the place node. Boersma (1998: 442)
points out that the three articulators grouped under the place node are articulatorily
mainly independent of each other, and that the LABIAL, CORONAL , and DORSAL
groupings arise because they happen to share the same influence on perceived nasality. Thus there is no reason why they should be subsumed under one node indicating one class. I agree on the needlessness of the place node. For the representation of retroflex assimilation processes without a place node see section 6.3.7.
As illustrated in section 5.1 above, it is traditionally assumed that phonological
features should describe natural classes, yield a tool to describe the phonological
processes in a given language, and ensure phonetic interpretability. These three
functions of features, which are inherently assumed in FG, are conflicting. A description of natural classes occurring in the languages of the world calls for a minimal set of features in order not to overpredict any classes. The features for such a
purpose do not have to be phonetically interpretable; on the contrary, a very abstract
system that is free of phonetic substance is open for phonetic interpretation in single
languages and is thus more useful for this classification task. The functions of phonetic interpretability and describing phonological processes, on the other hand, ask
144
Chapter 5
for different features. As observed by Kingston & Diehl (1994) and others, features
differ in their phonetic content from language to language, depending on the number
and ways of contrasts the respective language employs. The combination of features
associated with single segments differs accordingly (Pierrehumbert, et al.1996: 142),
thus a universal abstract set seems inappropriate. Furthermore, phonological processes in specific languages often refer to phonetic detail (illustrated in section 5.3
below), hence their representation needs to be fine-grained, with phonetically based
features. An alternative view solving this problem is proposed in section 5.4 below.
5.3 Alternative approaches
This section discusses recent modifications of or alternatives to the previously presented feature theories, which improve the representations of retroflexes and solve
some of the problems encountered by FG. All of these proposals were developed
within the framework of Optimality Theory (henceforth: OT; Prince & Smolensky
1993) as this theory provides the possibility of formalizing interactions of articulatory and perceptual motivations for phonological processes.
In subsection 5.3.1, Flemming’s approach is illustrated, in section 5.3.2 that of
Hamilton, in 5.3.3 the proposal made by Steriade, and in section 5.3.4 the theory of
Boersma. These four are chosen here for two reasons. Hamilton, Flemming, and Steriade deal with perceptual motivations for processes, specifically those involving
coronals. Flemming and Hamilton propose perceptual representations to account for
these processes, whereas Steriade proposes the device she dubs a ‘perception map’
to integrate information on perception into phonological accounts. Both Flemming
and Steriade are concerned with processes involving the class of retroflex segments.
Boersma is included although he does not explicitly treat retroflexes or coronals.
However, his work provides a full-fledged model of perceptual features and their
functioning in a perception and articulation grammar, and a slightly modified version of Boersma’s proposal will serve as the framework for my own analysis in
chapter 6, see section 5.4 below.
5.3.1 Flemming: Dispersion Theory of Contrast
Flemming’s (1995) dissertation “Auditory Presentations in Phonology” (revised in
2002)8 is of importance for the present work as it incorporates features based on perception (which Flemming calls auditory features) into phonological representations.
Furthermore, it illustrates a formal analysis for the auditory motivation of rounding
in retroflex contexts.
Flemming argues for the necessity of parallel articulatory and auditory feature
representations, and assumes that the relationship between the two is determined by
articulatory-to-acoustic mappings (2002: 4). Within an OT framework, the two representations are subject to two distinct constraint families, where constraints on the
distinctiveness of segments refer to the auditory representations, and constraints on
8
The differences between the two versions will be indicated where appropriate.
Phonological representations of retroflexes
145
the effort of gestures to the articulatory representation. The main idea is that a segment class is not inherently marked, but can only be marked with respect to its contrastiveness to other segment classes in the same inventory. The analysis of the
maximal perceptual distance between segments within one language is what Flemming terms the “Dispersion theory of contrast”, after Lindblom’s (1986) “Theory of
Adaptive Dispersion.” According to this theory, selection of contrast is based on the
three following principles:9
(4)
i. Maximize the number of contrasts
ii. Maximize the distinctiveness of contrasts
iii. Minimize articulatory effort
These principles are implemented in the Dispersion theory as markedness constraints on output forms.
Contrary to standard optimality-theoretic analyses, the Dispersion theory does
not include constraints that require faithfulness to underlying forms. Flemming argues that faithfulness constraints are not necessary in his theory, they are even incompatible with it (2002: 33) since the aim of Dispersion theory is to account for inventories in general, independent of language-specific inputs.
To compare the contrastiveness of segments, Flemming assigns segments values for auditory features on what he calls ‘dimension scales’, as illustrated in (5a)
for the F2 frequency (p. 94), and (5b) for the noise frequency (p. 55).10
(5)
1
i
(a) F2 dimension
2
y
ø
3
4
(b) Noise frequency (NF)
5
u
1
2
w
3
w
w
4
5
sw
6
s
On the F2 scale in (5a) the value 1 stands for high F2, 5 for low F2, and on the noise
frequency scale in (5b) 1 stands for low noise and 6 for high noise. Independently of
the values used, Flemming’s scales represent some ordering of segments with respect to a single feature and therefore allow a direct comparison of the auditory distance between sounds. The term ‘scale’ is problematic, as it implies a scalar measurement, i.e. equal distance between the items, which is actually not present in these
orderings. The distance in noise frequency between the fricatives [] and [w ], for
example, cannot be assumed to be the same as the distance between [w] and [].
Flemming’s evidence for auditory representations in phonology comes from
three types of phonological phenomena: enhancement, assimilation, and neutralization. All of these involve auditory contrasts between segment classes. Examples
9
10
Functional principles such as those in (4) are not new to phonological theory, see for example literature as early as Passy (1891), although Flemming fails to mention this.
In his earlier (1995) version, Flemming uses binary auditory features instead of scalar ones. The scale
for F2 is subdivided into the features [highest F2, high F2, low F2, lowest F2]. This change is neither
mentioned nor explained in the later version (2002).
146
Chapter 5
with retroflex segments are given for all three processes: enhancement is shown by
the rounding of retroflexes, see (6),11,12 assimilation by the rounding of front vowels
in retroflex context, see (7), and neutralization by coronal neutralization, see (8).
(6)
(7)
(8)
Enhancement
(a) retroflex fricatives are rounded
→ w
(b) retroflex approximants are rounded
→ w
Assimilation
rounding of vowels adjacent to retroflexes i → y
Neutralization
only retroflexes occur in initial and
postconsonantal position
Ct ~ C → C
in Polish
in English
in Wembawemba
and Wergaia
in Gooniyandi
and Walmatjari
Flemming motivates the rounding of the retroflex fricative in Polish13 in (6a) by the
presence of two other coronal fricatives in Polish, namely [] and [s] (p. 56). The
rounding occurs in order to make the retroflex auditorily more distinct from the alveolo-palatal [] on the noise frequency scale. As we can see on the scale given
above in (5b), the pair [ - ] has a difference of one scale-unit of noise frequency,
whereas [w - ] has a difference of two, thus the contrast in the Polish fricative inventory is enhanced by rounding. Mandarin, not discussed in Flemming’s dissertation, has the same sibilant inventory as Polish (Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996:
150f.), although the retroflex is not rounded. Mandarin thus does not enhance the
auditory distinction between the retroflex and the alveolo-palatal sibilant. In Flemming’s theory, the difference between Mandarin and Polish cannot be accounted for
by refering to different underlying input forms since the Dispersion theory does not
take underlying representations into account. Presumably, these language-specific
differences can be explained by a constraint militating against an additional articulation of rounding being ranked higher in Mandarin than in Polish.
With respect to the rounding of the English approximant in (6b), Flemming
explains that lip rounding serves to further lower the F3 of the retroflex, thus “enhancing contrasts on this dimension” (p. 56). Which contrast has to be enhanced is
not further specified by Flemming, but we can assume that it is the one between the
approximant and the lateral [l] in English.
Examples of the process of vowel rounding adjacent to retroflexes in Wembawemba and Wergaia, see (7), were presented in section 4.3.2.4, where it was also
explained that this process is articulatorily motivated by the incompatibility of the
high front vowel gesture with the retraction of the tongue body necessary for retroflexion. Flemming also assumes an incompatibility of retroflexion and high front
11
12
13
Flemming transcribes the retroflex approximant with the symbol [].
In Flemming’s account the process of rounding is not restricted to retroflex fricatives but is also
shown to occur with [–anterior] segments, and he exemplifies rounding of laminal post-alveolars with
the process → w in English and French (p. 55).
According to Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996) the Polish retroflex fricative shows lip-protrusion, not
rounding. Lip protrusion causes the vocal tract to be lengthened, which results in lower formant frequencies. Lip rounding is a closing off of the vocal tract on one side, which has a lowering effect on
F3, so both lip movements have similar acoustic results.
Phonological representations of retroflexes
147
vowel gesture, and points out that the rounding of the vowel serves to enhance the
retroflex perception, as the retroflex gesture is reduced or even lost in this context
(in his analysis Flemming presumes the complete loss of retroflexion). One question
arises here: if the retroflexion of the consonant is supposed to be enhanced, why is it
the vowel that is rounded, and not the retroflex itself, as is the case with the Polish
fricative and the English approximant in (6)? The two possibilities are illustrated in
(9).
(9) (a) i → yt
(b) i → it
The rounding of the vowel as in (9a) jeopardizes the distinctiveness of the vowel as
well as that of the consonant: the vowel is rounded and hence is closer to the back
rounded /u/ in both languages, and the retroflex is changed to a non-retroflex coronal, making it indistinguishable from the front coronal. If maximal contrastiveness
is assumed, as in Flemming’s Dispersion theory, one would expect a development as
in (9b), where the original retroflex is rounded to enhance its contrast with the nonretroflex coronal. However, the segments undergoing de-retroflexion in Wembawemba and Wergaia are plosives, and secondary rounding of plosives can be perceived in the release phase only. Retroflexion of the vowel, on the other hand, is
perceivable throughout the vowel segment. This factor of cue perceptebility obviously influences the choice of the segment that undergoes rounding, i.e., this is a
case of cue enhancement.
Flemming also discusses processes where mid front vowels are lowered in a
retroflex context (pp. 90ff.). He distinguishes vowel rounding and vowel lowering,
assuming that the first occurs only for high front vowels which make a retroflex
gesture impossible, whereas the latter occurs with mid front vowels, where slight
retroflexion is possible if the vowel is lowered. Thus, the rounding of the vowel is
necessary only when a retroflexion gesture is articulatorily impossible.
Apical positional neutralization in (8) was already illustrated in section 4.5.
Flemming (2002: 43f.) follows Steriade’s (1995, 2002) analysis of this process (see
section 5.3.3 below), and argues that this neutralization is caused by a lack of contrast between the apical alveolar and the retroflex in initial position. In this position,
the VC transitions are not available since no vowel precedes, and the available CV
cues are very similar for retroflexes and front apicals. The distance between the F3
in the release is only one unit on Flemming’s F3 scale, see (10).
(10)
F3
approach
2
release
4
t, t, t
approach release
5
5
Flemming also explains that despite their more complex articulation, retroflexes can
occur in the site of neutralization. This is due to the fact that their F3 releases are
distinct from those of all other coronals, as can be seen in (10) (other cues distinguishing the four coronals are not known and therefore not specified in this analysis).
148
Chapter 5
In general, the need for auditory representations is well-motivated in Flemming’s dissertation by various processes that cannot be accounted for with articulatory features. Flemming’s Dispersion theory, however, has a big drawback. It is a
model to evaluate inventories only, which is done by comparing output representations. Input representations are irrelevant for this theory. As pointed out by Boersma
(1998: 361), the Dispersion theory is therefore not a model of a production grammar, i.e., cannot account for the mapping of an underlying form to a surface form.
The articulatory representations employed by Flemming are given in the traditional features in a FG hierarchy, although the insufficiency of these features becomes obvious when the tongue body position has to be specified (which is done
with the features [high] and [front]), or different degrees of retroflexion are needed
(there is only the binary feature [anterior] which specifies either full retroflexion or
none). The traditional featural representations for these are modified in Flemming’s
analysis of retroflexion in (high) front vowel context, by a scalar representation of
the feature [anterior] as [−1ant] for full retroflexion, [0ant] for partial retroflexion,
and [+1ant] for anterior coronals. Furthermore, a backness dimension is introduced,
which is divided into [−2back] for a front tongue body, [0back] for central, and
[−1back] for an intermediate position. In brackets Flemming adds that positive values would represent a back tongue body position. Flemming’s changes of these features support the point made in section 5.2 above that FG features are insufficient
for the representation of phonological processes involving retroflexes and other
segments. Furthermore, Flemming’s work illustrates the need for phonetically rich
phonological representations.
5.3.2 Hamilton: Gestural and Perceptual Theory of Markedness
Hamilton’s (1996) dissertation deals with the phonotactic restrictions on coronals in
Australian languages and argues that these are phonetically grounded.14 The actual
data and their explanation were already discussed in section 4.5. The present section
is only concerned with Hamilton’s theoretical assumptions and their formalization.
According to Hamilton, unmarked clusters are gesturally and/or perceptually simple,
whereas marked structures are gesturally and/or perceptually complex. These basic
ideas are couched in what he calls the “gestural and perceptual theory of markedness”, given here in (11) and (12) (p.9ff.):
(11) Gestural Theory of Markedness (GTM):
Simple gestures are less marked than complex gestures.
(12) Perceptual Theory of Markedness (PTM):
Perceptually simple speech sounds are less marked than perceptually complex
speech sounds.
A ‘simple gesture’ is defined as one involving articulatorily less effort than a complex one, and a ‘perceptually simple speech sound’ is defined as one that has robust
14
Hamilton’s analysis is restricted to morpheme-internal phonotactics because “the set of consonant
clusters which occur at morpheme junctures on the surface are typically less restrictive than what is
attested morpheme internally” (p. 18).
Phonological representations of retroflexes
149
spectral cues. The resulting two markedness hierarchies are assumed to be languageindependent, and could be formalized as fixed rankings of markedness constraints in
an OT framework, which is a s step that Hamilton suggests but does not take himself. According to Hamilton, the ranking between GTM and PTM constraints has to
be language-specific.
Reference to perceptual simplicity and perceptual cues implies that Hamilton
includes perceptual features15 in his phonological representations. The perceptual
features he proposes are based on the feature system of Jakobson et al. (1952) and
Jakobson & Halle (1956) (see section 5.2.1). Hamilton replaces the unary features
[acute] and [grave], by [±grave], and [compact] and [diffuse] by [±compact], thus
reducing the original system. Furthermore, he uses the feature [sharp] for distinguishing primary places of articulation, and not to refer to secondary palatalization
as originally defined: laminal palato-alveolars are [+sharp] because of their high F3,
all other coronals are [−sharp].
Table 5.7
Hamilton’s (1996) acoustic features (an empty field indicates no specification for
this feature).
labial
grave
sharp
flat
compact
+
−
laminal
dental
−
−
−
apical
alveolar
−
−
−
laminal
pal.alv.
−
+
−
retroflex
velar
−
−
+
+
+
The non-distinctiveness between laminal dentals and apical alveolars reflects their
similar formant transitions (recall the discussion in 3.3). Hamilton provides no solution on how to distinguish between laminal dentals and apical alveolars in languages
that contrast these two classes.
In addition to the acoustic features in table 5.7, Hamilton introduces a set of
articulatory features which differs slightly from the standard ones of FG. Hamilton
(1996) proposes the unary features [apical] and [laminal] instead of the feature
[±distributed], based on the observation that Australian languages show longdistance harmony patterns between laminals to which apicals are transparent, and
vice versa. This point is not illustrated by Hamilton who states that “these patterns
are at present poorly understood” (p. 42). The further distinction of laminals and
apicals is undertaken by the four unary features [dental], [alveolar], [post-alveolar],
and [alveolo-palatal] as Hamilton considers the use of the feature [±anterior] to be
an overgeneralization (recall also the criticism on this feature in section 5.2.3.1). The
complete specification with Hamilton’s articulatory features (including additional
features to distinguish labials and velars and for the tongue body shape) is given in
(13) (from Hamilton 1996: 45).
15
Hamilton refers to them as ‘acoustic’ features.
150
Chapter 5
(13) labials
lamino-dentals
apico-alveolars
apico-post-alveolars
lamino-alveolo-palatals
dorso-velars
[labial]
[coronal], [laminal], [dental]
[coronal], [apical], [alveolar]
[coronal], [apical], [post-alveolar]
[coronal], [laminal], [alveolo-palatal], [high]
[dorsal], [velar], [high]
The reduplication problem occurring if both acoustic (see table 5.7) and articulatory features (see (13)) are assumed is solved by Hamilton by posing cooccurrence restrictions on the features. The acoustic feature [+flat], for example, is
linked to the retroflex gestures, i.e. [coronal, apical, post-alveolar], because retroflexion is the only articulation which produces a [+flat] acoustic effect (p. 53). The
co-occurrence restrictions are to be understood as phonetically grounded constraints
(universally unviolated) to rule out impossible feature combinations such as [−flat]
retroflexes or [+flat] non-retroflex coronals.
In general, Hamilton’s dissertation convinces primarily because of its distinction between articulatory and perceptual representations and the proposal of complete feature systems to formalize both. Basing the articulatory features more on the
actual dependencies of the articulators than was done before in FG (for instance, by
treating apical and laminal classes as independent of each other, and by introducing
separate place features for their dependents) is an important step forward towards
more phonetically oriented representations. However, the work lacks an actual application of these features: no formal account of the phonotactic restrictions is given
to illustrate the interaction of the features and the necessity of assuming these features.
5.3.3 Steriade: Licensing by Cue
A large part of Steriade’s work (1995, 2001a, 2001b) deals with the two
phonological processes of neutralization and assimilation and their phonetic motivation. The outputs of both processes are claimed to be determined by the distribution
of the perceptual cues of input and output segment, not by their prosodic position.
Her approach is termed ‘licensing by cue’, in contrast to the ‘prosodic licensing’ approach (Jun 1995, Beckman 1998, Lombardi 1999). Although Steriade applies her
analysis to a large variety of processes (such as laryngeal neutralization, metathesis,
and loan adaptation), the present discussion is restricted to the treatment of retroflexes for obvious reasons.
Neutralization of the contrast between apical alveolar and retroflex segments
(as exemplified in section 4.5) has the same positional restrictions as the assimilation of retroflexes to apical alveolars (recall the examples in section 4.6): both processes take place mainly in post-consonantal and word-initial position. This similarity
is explained by Steriade as being based on the non-available VC transitional cues in
these positions. Let us take the cue distribution for retroflex stops as an example,
which looks like the following, according to Steriade (1995: 16).
Phonological representations of retroflexes
Table 5.8
Steriade’s proposal for cue distribution in retroflex stops. This table combines the
last two contexts into one column, whereas Steriade lists them separately, although they have the same cues. For possible differences, see text below.
contexts
cues
151
V_V
lowering of F3 + F4
burst spectrum
burst amplitude
VOT
closure duration
V_C / V_#
lowering of F3 + F4
#_V / C_V
burst spectrum
burst amplitude
VOT
This table shows that the intervocalic context (first column) provides the largest
number of cues for retroflex stops and is therefore the context that provides most saliency. Although in the post-vocalic context (second column in table 5.9) there are
fewer cues than in the pre-vocalic context (last column), these cues of lowered F3
and F4 are more salient than the prevocalic ones. Hence, retroflex segments can be
distinguished best from other apicals in intervocalic position, quite well in postvocalic position (a point already illustrated in 3.3), and least in post-consonant or postpausal context. Besides context differences, the cue distribution also depends on the
manner of segments (recall discussion in 3.3): fricatives, which have strong internal
cues of frication noise, are less prone to assimilation and neutralization than stops.
Nasals have weaker cues than both stops and fricatives, and often show assimilation
or neutralization when the other two classes do not.
According to Steriade (2001a, 2001b), the speaker is aware of these distributions: in order to be able to assimilate or neutralize a retroflex segment, the speaker
judges whether the modified output sequence would be perceptually very different
from the expected output sequence. If this is the case, then assimilation or neutralization is prohibited. Only perceptually very similar pairs can undergo the processes.
This awareness of cue distribution does not emerge from traditional phonological
features. Steriade (2001a, 2001b) proposes a perceptibility map, or P-map, as a supplement to distinctive feature theory to provide this information. The P-map is defined as a “repository of speakers’ knowledge […] that certain contrasts are more
discriminable than others, and that the same contrast is more salient in some positions than in others” (2001a: 236). ‘Contrast’ in this definition refers to the perceived difference between two strings, the lexical norm and its potential modification, independent of its phonemic status. Sources for the knowledge represented by
the P-map might be observations from confusion rates or similarity computations.
An example of a P-map is given in table 5.9 on the next page, where the font
size reflects the similarity assumed between segments: the larger the font, the less
similar the members of a pair.
Steriade, who works in an OT framework, links such a P-map to the grammar
by correspondence constraints referring to the information given in the P-map. Thus,
if a possible contrast x-y is perceptually more similar than the contrast w-z, then the
correspondence constraint militating against a replacement of w by z is ranked
higher than the one militating against a replacement of x by y.
152
Chapter 5
Table 5.9
s/
t/
n/
Hypothetical P-map fragment for apical pairs (Steriade 2001a: 237).
V_V
V_#
V_C
#_V
C_V
C_C
s/
t/
n/
s/
t/
n/
s/
t/
n/
s/
s/
s/
t/
t/
t/
n/
n/
n/
The contrast between apical alveolar and retroflex stop in a V_C context, for example, is more salient than the same contrast in a C_V context. Steriade formalizes this
information as correspondence constraints which require identity of the feature [anterior], and their following ranking (Steriade 2001a: 240):
(14) IDENT (anterior) /V [_apical, stop] C >> IDENT (anterior) /C [_apical, stop] V
This ranking is assumed to be fixed (i.e., universal) and thus invariably predicts progressive assimilation for a sequence of retroflex–apical or apical–retroflex stops.
Steriade (2001a: 241) points out that these constraints are highly specific, as they refer to specific values (anteriority) in specific segment types (apical stops) and specific contexts (V_C versus C_V).
Like Flemming’s and Hamilton’s accounts, Steriade’s shows that phonological
processes are sensitive to perceptual cues, and that it is necessary to incorporate
these cues into phonological accounts. In contrast to the other authors discussed in
this section, Steriade does not assume a perceptual specification of segments and
therefore a separate set of perceptual features or cues. In her view, it is sufficient to
introduce correspondence constraints that refer to the cue information stored somewhere else.
This latter point is exactly where Steriade’s approach appears to be vulnerable
to criticism. Two different devices are introduced to explain the influence of perceptual knowledge on phonological processes: the P-map and the correspondence
constraints incorporating the information of the P-map into the grammar. Does the
speaker really need a separate P-map, where the information is stored, when the
same information could be integrated in the constraints and their rankings? The necessity for this duplication most likely emerges from the fact that the constraints
used by Steriade refer to discrete phonological features, not to cues (no direct incorporation of perceptual knowledge). Looking at Steriade’s constraints without the additional knowledge of the P-map, it does not become clear why exactly this ranking
should be assumed as fixed, a point that she explicitly makes to justify the existence
of the P-map.16 However, direct reference to cue information and a strict division of
16
Steriade (2001b: 2) writes: “The general rationale for the P-map proposal is that attested phonological
systems display less diversity than predicted by versions of Optimality Theory (OT) in which correspondence and phonotactic constraints interact freely.” Thus, the P-map is necessary to restrict constraint rankings and their possible outputs in Steriade’s view. On the same page she comments that the
non-restrictiveness of possible outcomes and repair strategies is not a problem of OT alone, and that
other approaches also fail. This observation indicates that it might not be the framework that causes
these problems, but the tools used within all these frameworks, namely the traditional, nonphonetically oriented features.
Phonological representations of retroflexes
153
the constraints according to context could provide inherently ranked constraints and
make an additional P-map superfluous. Instead of the feature [anterior], it is the
transitional cue of F3 (and the burst noise) that differs between alveolar and retroflex apical stops and should therefore be made the argument of the IDENT constraints
in (14). This supposes an underlying specification of segments by perceptual cues,
which Steriade avoids. An alternative analysis of neutralization and assimilation of
retroflex segments tackling this point is given in sections 6.3.5 and 6.3.6, respectively.
Another point of criticism of Steriade’s work is that it exclusively focuses on
some cues, while others are ignored. For example the distribution of cues depending
on context in table 5.8 does not include any F3 (and F4) transitions of the retroflex
into the following vowel. Steriade (2001a: 230) even claims that the second consonant in apical clusters “lacks all transitional cues to place distinguishing it from another apical.” Section 3.5, however, illustrated that the CV transitions do differ for
retroflex and apical alveolar. The burst spectrum in pre-vocalic position that is listed
by Steriade (see last column in table 5.8), supplies another additional cue differentiating alveolars and retroflexes, although it is not put to use in Steriade’s descriptions
and analyses following her list.
A minor point on the notion of cue licensing has to be added. This term implies that any prosodic position is irrelevant for the distribution of cues, and thus the
only relevant information for neutralization or assimilation of retroflexes is whether
they occur in post-vocalic or post-consonantal position. Nevertheless, table 5.8 includes word boundary contexts. Furthermore, Steriade (1995: 17) states that the nontransitional cues (i.e. burst spectrum, amplitude and VOT) of the retroflex are
stronger in initial position but she does not describe the consequences hereof for cue
licensing. In later work, Steriade (2001a) mentions non-neutralization of word-initial
retroflexes and calls it the ‘initial deviation’ (p. 226) as observable for instance in
Djinang, which has an apical contrast word-initially but not post-consonantally
(word-internal). In the same work, Steriade mentions that for neutralization processes a ‘final deviation’ occurs, where word-final apical segments are not assimilated to following word-initial apical segments but instead regressive assimilation
applies (p. 227), for instance in Sanskrit and Punjabi. Steriade provides no explanation for these two deviations, and the different terminology used obscures the fact
that both types of deviating behaviour save the cues of the word-initial position.
5.3.4 Boersma: Functional Phonology
In his theory of ‘Functional Phonology (henceforth: FP), Boersma (1998, 1999) emphasizes the functional principles of efficient speech production, given here in (15).
(15) i. minimization of articulatory effort
ii. minimization of perceptual confusion
These two principles are already familiar to the reader from the description of
Flemming’s approach. Speech perception according to Boersma follows different
functional principles, given in (16).
154
Chapter 5
(16) i. maximization of recognition
ii. minimization of categorization
According to FP, the two different principles of speech production necessitate a distinction between articulatory and perceptual representations in phonology: articulatory effort, see (16i), refers to articulatory representations, and perceptual confusion,
see (16ii), (plus the two perception principles in (16)) to perceptual ones. Furthermore, separate grammars are postulated to model production, perception, and recognition. Contrary to traditional views and Hamilton’s and Flemming’s approach,
Boersma assumes the perceptual representation to be underlying. The perceptual UR
is transformed into an articulatory output via the production grammar, see the first
step in (17). This articulatory output is categorized by the listener (and the speaker,
too) in the perception grammar into a perceptual form, see the second step in (17) (in
Boersma’s notation).
→
(discrete)
production
perceptual specification
(17) |underlying form|
[phonetic form]
→
/phonological form/
(continuous)
perception
(discrete)
articulatory output
perceptual output
The production system of the speaker is modelled in FP as an OT production grammar as in (18) (1998: 146).
(18) FP production grammar
|spec|
[art1] → /perc1/
[art2] → /perc2/
A
*!
B
*
In this tableau, spec stands for the perceptual specification, artx for candidates of the
articulatory output, and percx for corresponding perceptual outputs. Constraints applying in the production grammar are constraints for faithfulness of the perceptual
output to the underlying perceptual specification (e.g. *REPLACE) and markedness
constraints militating against complex/marked articulations (e.g. *GESTURE). “Lowranked *GESTURE constraints determine the finite set of allowed articulatory features and feature combinations in one language” (p. 163).
A listener’s categorization system can be similarly modelled by a perception
grammar in an OT framework, see (19) (1998: 147).
(19) FP perception grammar
[ac]
/cat1/
/cat2/
A
*!
B
*
In the perception grammar model, ac denotes the acoustic input, and catx the candidate perceptual categories. The constraints relevant for the perception grammar are
Phonological representations of retroflexes
155
faithfulness constraints which militate against the warping of the existing perceptual
categories in a language (*WARP) and markedness constraints against the perception
of any category (*CATEG). As in the production grammar, low-ranked markedness
constraints, here *CATEG, determine the finite set of allowed perceptual feature values. Features in this definition (and the above definition for the production grammar) are values on a continuous scale or combinations thereof. Feature values are
given in concrete numbers, such as [260] (Hz) for a F1 frequency. According to
faithfulness, listeners do not rank the acoustically realized feature values directly
along continuous scales but categorize them into perceptual feature values (p.176).
Perceptual features are understood to be relatively independent from each
other in FP, with hardly any hierarchical dependencies. An overview of these perceptual features and their dependencies is given in figure 5.4 (based on Boersma
1998: 23), where conjunction of features is shown by solid lines and disjunctions by
dashed lines.
perceived
heard
seen
static
dynamic
silence sounding
lips
duration transition release
loudness
size
shape
burst nasal lateral open close round spread
strong weak
voiced
spectrum
pitch murmur sonorant
noise
aspirated mellow strident sibilant
H M L
F1
F2
nasal
min mid max max min
(high)
(low) (front) (back)
Figure 5.4
lateral
place
bilab labd. dent. alv. pal. vel. uvul. phar. epigl. gl.
Perceptual features of FP and their dependencies.
The articulatory gestures used for the realization of the UR are not arranged in a hierarchy; Boersma argues that the articulators are nearly independent of each other. A
representation of them is given in figure 5.5 on the next page (based on Boersma
1998: 23, figure 1.8).
156
Chapter 5
lip
place
blade
stricture
bilab labiodent closed critic approx narrow open wide
velum
raised lowered
glottis
constricted adducted spread
place
distributed stricture
+
−
vocal folds
tense
lax
Figure 5.5 Articulatory gestures in FP.
Many of these gestural features have values along a continuous range, for instance
the place feature for the tongue blade.
The notion of universality is interpreted in FP as the fact that languages tend to
have similar phenomena because the communicative functions are similar in most
languages. FP thus has no need for innateness. All features and representations can
be learned without any previous knowledge since languages make use of the perceptual and articulatory possibilities.
To summarize, the theory of FP makes some assumptions that depart radically
from traditional featural theories. First of all, both articulatory and perceptual features are necessary, with perceptual ones as underlying specifications. Secondly, the
features are not innate, and thus the feature values chosen from articulatory and perceptual space are not universally valid but are language-specific. Thirdly, features
are not used to define cross-linguistic categories. These points tackle exactly the
problems with FG and its modifications as pointed out above.
A point not fully developed in Boersma’s FP is the system of articulatory gestures as depicted in figure 5.5. For articulations with the tongue, only ‘blade’ is explicitly given and further specified with ‘place’, ‘distributed’, and ‘stricture’. The
use of the subspecification ‘distributed’ is not defined, but can be assumed to follow
traditional definitions, where the tip and the blade are subsumed under one node and
are distinguished by [±distributed]. Thus, the tip and the blade of the tongue are
treated as being depend on each other in Boersma’s model. Besides giving the exact
articulator, a gestural model also needs to take into account the restrictions on the
co-occurrence of tongue gestures, a point that is not included in FP. The interaction
of tongue gestures will be further illustrated in section 5.4.2.
Boersma does not explicitly deal with any phonological processes involving
retroflexes. The application of FP to retroflex processes with modifications newly
introduced here will be described in section 5.4 below and demonstrated in greater
detail in chapter 6.
Phonological representations of retroflexes
157
5.4 Featural representation of retroflexes: Present approach
Following the proposals made by Flemming, Hamilton, and Boersma described
above, my own proposal also involves perceptual features in addition to articulatory
ones in order to capture all phonological processes of retroflexes discussed in chapter 4. Both perceptual cues and articulatory gestures are necessary for phonological
representations because of their different functions (in line with Flemming 1995 and
Boersma 1998): perceptual features are responsible for acquisition (see Boersma
2000),17 articulatory ones for assimilation processes (see the traditional featural accounts and Browman & Goldstein 1989, 1992). This poses the problem of duplicating information by having both articulatory and perceptual features. In contrast to
Flemming, I assume that the perceptual features do not emerge from underlying articulatory representations that are mapped onto auditory representations. Hamilton’s
proposal to restrict perceptual features by co-occurrence constraints is also not followed here. Instead, I adopt Boersma’s idea of underlying perceptual representations, which are mapped onto articulatory gestures via a production grammar.
As illustrated in section 5.1 in general and in section 5.2.3.3 for FG specifically, traditional feature approaches assume three functions of phonological features:
describing natural classes, yielding a tool to describe the phonological processes in a
given language, and ensuring phonetic interpretability. These three functions were
shown to be in conflict, since the description of natural classes requires a minimal,
abstract set of features, whereas phonetic interpretability and the description of
phonological processes in specific languages requires fine-grained, phonetically
based features. These different tasks should be performed by different sets of features, as illustrated in (20).
(20)
function
(a) representation &
phonetic interpretability
(b) classification
feature set needed
language-specific sets of phonetically
based features
abstract, universally valid class features
Returning to the example of the feature [voice] from section 5.1, this feature is a
very abstract means for cross-linguistic classification, see (20b). Single languages
such as Dutch or German instead use the phonetically interpretable features [vocal
fold vibration] and [aspiration], respectively, see (20a). Phonological descriptions of
single languages thus require language-specific sets of features, which are based on
the phonetic realizations of the segment classes in the languages. My primary goal
here will be to explicate my views on the former set of features (20a) since I consider this closer to the central aim of phonology, slightly neglecting the latter set
(20b) in the following discussions.
A feature set fulfilling the functions under (20a) has to be very close to phonetics, i.e., has to be directly phonetically interpretable. This can be realized by taking phonetic cues as the prime elements of phonology instead of whole segments (as
17
Acquisition processes include sound change. See Ohala (1989, 1993) for an illustration of the relevance of perceptual cues in sound change.
158
Chapter 5
in a phonemics or segmentalism approach) or so-called universal features which
cannot be directly interpreted (as in traditional feature geometry). The approach
taken here is as illustrated in figure 5.2 above, repeated here in figure 5.6 for convenience.
segments
cues/gestures = features
S1
S2
[ c1 , c2 , c3 , c6 ]
[c3 , c4 , c5 , c6 , c7]
Figure 5.6 Schematic representation of relation segment – cues in phonetic approaches.
According to this view, the phonetic cues, i.e. the features, are directly phonetically
interpretable. Figure 5.6 is slightly simplified as it implies that only single cues can
be features, a point that is certainly not valid: it is often not only one cue but cue
clusters which are the trigger or the target of specific phonological processes. Furthermore, not all cues of a segment class are used as phonologically distinctive cues
in a language. Thus, an intermediate level in our model of the connection between
segments and phonetic correlates can be posited in which only those cues and cue
clusters are stated that are relevant to the phonology of the language in question.
This yields the following model:
segments
S1
features
/c1/
cues
[c1, c2, c3, c6]
Figure 5.7
/c3, c6/
S2
/c4/ /c3 , c6/ /c7/
[c4, c3, c5, c6, c7]
Schematic representation of phonetic features.
It might be asked where the difference lies between traditional features as depicted
in figure 5.1 and the phonetically-based features postulated here, see figure 5.7, because the former are also correlated with phonetic cues and are an intermediate level
between segments and these cues. But whereas traditional phonological features are
mainly binary, are derived from cross-linguistic observations and are not directly
phonetically interpretable, the present phonological features can be scalar, directly
interpreted, and are language-specific. The phonological representations assumed
here are therefore rich.
As phonetic cues and gestures are based on the articulatory and perceptual
systems of humans, they can be assumed to be universally restricted, in as far as no
gesture or cue occurs in any language that is articulatorily impossible or not perceivable. In this sense, phonetic cues and gestures make up a universal set from which
languages make use. A difference between important and non-important features or
primary and enhancing features as proposed for instance by Stevens et al. (1986)
therefore cannot be stated as universal since the possible set of enhancing features
totally depends on the features each language choses as primary.
In figure 5.7, I deliberately refer only to cues, that is, perceptual cues, in the
lowest level of representation since I assume that perceptual cues are the primary
Phonological representations of retroflexes
159
correlates and the basis for phonological features from which articulatory gestures
are derived. Underlying representations are therefore assumed to be perceptual, following Boersma (1998, 2001). The transformation of this underlying perceptual representation into an articulatory representation is performed in the production grammar of the speaker (again following Boersma); only the perceptual representation is
stored in the mental lexicon. An illustration of the assumed model is given in figure
5.8 (based on Boersma 1998: 143), see also (17) above.
/underlying perceptual form/
via production grammar
[articulatory output]art
[acoustic output]ac
via perception grammar
[perceptual output]perc
Figure 5.8 Correlation between underlying perceptual representation and articulatory representation according to Boersma’s model for a speaker, with separate production
and perception grammar.
The details of the perceptual and articulatory representations in my account are explained in the following subsections.
5.4.1 Perceptual representations
The perceptual representation proposed here does not consist of a complete set of
features. Instead, only those features are illustrated that are relevant and sufficient
for the analysis of retroflexes in chapter 6. Suggestions for other features are given
where possible. Cues for voicing, implosion, and ejective articulations are ignored
here. The representations are labelled “perceptual”, although as illustrated in chapter
3, they are actually acoustic characteristics. The label perceptual is used to indicate
that it is the perception of the information by the listener which is relevant, not the
way we technically measure and describe this information.
The perceptual features assumed here are in line with Boersma’s proposal (see
figure 5.4): they project their own tiers, and are hierarchically independent of each
other. In contrast to Boersma, the perceptual representations applied here are phonetically rich. Instead of actual values for the different features, I will use a stylized
ordering representation, similar to that of Flemming (2002): formant frequencies are
represented by 5 to 9 values. This step of abstraction is taken because I am dealing
with data from several languages and several speakers, where the actual values are
expected to differ from language to language. The abstraction with several values is
made for ease of comparison. In contrast to Flemming, who uses the values 1 to 5, I
term the lowest formant value maximally [–4], the middle value [0], and the highest
formant value maximally [+4], to facilitate an interpretation as low, middle, and
160
Chapter 5
high formants.18 The zero value should not be mistaken as an indication that no formant is present. The absence of a feature is not specified in my approach. The absence of formant frequencies and frication noise in the case of stop closures, however, is specified with the cue [silence] since this is a relevant cue for the identification of the stop manner. Furthermore, the duration of a stop closure is an additional
feature to specific places of articulation (recall the difference between very short
stop closures in retroflexes and rather long stop closures in laminals in section
3.3.3), and it indicates the difference between geminate and singleton segment. Possible values for this feature are [0] for an average closure length as in laterals, [–1]
for short closures as found in retroflexes, [+1] for long closures as in laminals, and
[+2] and higher for geminate segments.19
Segment classes with inherent formants (e.g. nasals, laterals, and rhotics) can
be presented with values for the features F2 and F3 (as explained in section 3.1, F1
is not used here for differentiating consonants). The same features will be used for
the representation of stops, since they have formant transitions leading into and out
of the stop closure. Furthermore, I will describe fricatives by F3 values, which stand
for the lowest peak in the friction noise (see section 3.3.2). By this rather abstract
representation, I yield the same features for retroflex articulations across all manners.
Based on the formant values as described in section 3.3.1, the following values
are assigned for consonants:
Table 5.10 Values for the formant features [F2] and [F3] for consonants.
labial
[F2]
[F3]
0
−1
laminal
dental
+1
0
apical
alveolar
+1
0
laminal
pal.alv.
+2
+2
retroflex
velar
+1
−2
−2
−1
The class of coronals is roughly captured by high [F2] values, and a retroflex class
with the feature specifications [+1F2] and [−2F3]. Not all retroflexes have a subapical post-alveolar articulation, as we saw in chapter 2. Retroflex segments with an
apical post-alveolar constriction instead of a subapical one, which was explained to
result in a higher F3 than in the average retroflex in chapter 3, are represented with
the feature value [−1F3] (as for instance for the segments in the West-African language Ewe).
Vowels are represented by the features [F1] and [F2] as illustrated in table 5.11
and 5.12. The [F2] features are conditional on [F1], e.g., [+4] refers to a different
18
19
In an earlier account (Hamann 2002b), I used binary features (comparable to those used by Flemming
1995). This approach is not followed here, as it necessitates describing a linear scale of three and more
values with several binary features.
Certainly the length of a geminate depends on the length of its singleton counterparts and also on the
length of the preceding vowels; thus, a more fine-grained value system is necessary to account for this
variation. As my analysis in chapter 6 involves no geminates, a further refinement is not needed here.
Phonological representations of retroflexes
161
actual F2 value for [i] than for [e]. Rounding makes a difference of 2 values for
[F2], and place a difference of 3 values.
Table 5.11 Feature [F1] with its values for low to high vowels.
[F1]
+3
+2
+1
0
–1
–2
–3
a
æ
$, %
&
e, o, ø, (, )
*, +
i, u, ,, -, , y
Table 5.12 Feature [F2] with its values for front to back vowels (including the influence of
rounding).
[F2]
+4
unrounded
rounded
i, e, $
+3
+2
+1
,, (
y, ø
0
–1
–2
-
, )
–3
–4
o, %, u
In addition, rounded vowels are specified by a low F3, i.e. [–2F3] (according to Stevens 1998: 292ff., rounding results in a prominent, low F2-F3 peak for vowels).
Besides the formant structures, consonants are also specified by mannerspecific features such as friction noise, burst noise, and so on. These features and
their values do not have to be specified for the analyses in chapter 6, but could be
formalized easily: friction noise, for instance, can be represented as in Flemming’s
noise frequency scale (recall (9b)).
Reference to these perceptual features in the FP version of OT used here is
made by specific constraints, as will be elaborated in section 6.1.
5.4.2 Articulatory representations
The independent articulatory gestures of the Boersma model as illustrated in figure
5.5 are adopted here as articulatory representations. The interaction of these gestures
can be modelled as in Browman & Goldstein’s (1989) Articulatory Phonology, in
which each gesture is assigned a single tier, and where the duration and overlap of
gestures is represented by overlap of the gesture tiers on a temporal axis. Some minor modifications are introduced here to Boersma’s gestures. In addition to the
specification of non-tongue articulations, the present model includes four separate
representations for the tongue parts, namely tip, blade, middle, and back. All four of
them can be further specified by the stricture type and the exact place of articulation.
Boersma’s specification ‘distributed’ for the feature blade is not taken over here.The
resulting representations for the tongue parts is illustrated in figure 5.9 on the next
page.
162
Chapter 5
tip
stricture
place
closed critical approx. narrow open wide
blade
stricture
middle
place
closed…wide dental …palatal
Figure 5.9
dental alveolar post-alveolar palatal
stricture
back
place
closed…wide post-alv. …velar
stricture
place
closed…wide velar…pharyn.
Articulatory gestures of the tongue.
The proposed tongue parts and their possible movements are depicted in figure 5.10,
in which only the tongue surface is shown.
tip – blade – middle – back
Figure 5.10 Abstract assumption on tongue parts and their movements on the vertical axis.
This figure is a simplification of the possible tongue tip movements in the vertical,
i.e. the height, dimension. The subdivision proposed here departs from the FG classification of the tongue into coronal and dorsal features in as far as it gives independent status to apical and laminal articulation (they are not united under a node
such as CORONAL), and provides a further subdivision of the dorsal tongue part into
tongue middle and back, again both not dependent of one node. Evidence for this division is the observation that these four articulators are independently involved in
the major articulations of the tongue. In section 5.2.3.2 on the attempts within FG to
describe the tongue dorsum movements, we saw that two kinds of movements have
to be distinguished, namely a raising or lowering of the tongue middle and a retraction or fronting of the tongue back, usually represented by the DORSAL features
[high] and [back]. The problems with this representation and proposed alternatives
were described in the same section.
Phonological representations of retroflexes
163
The presentation proposed here avoids a further problem encountered within
FG: the drawing of a borderline between coronal and dorsal articulations. The postalveolar, alveolo-palatal, and palatal articulations, which were all described as phonetically and phonologically involving both coronal and dorsal articulations (recall
the discussion in section 5.2.3.3), now belong to the articulation with the tongue
middle (part of the former DORSAL ), which are shown below to trigger coarticulation of the tongue blade (part of the former CORONAL).
The four tongue articulators can move either upwards or downwards from their
rest-position (as illustrated in figure 5.10), yielding the following articulations:
(21) tip
blade
middle
back
up
down
up
down
up
down
up
down
apical alveolar articulations
withdrawn tongue tip (e.g. for laminal post-alveolars)
laminal post-alveolar articulations
lowered and stretched tongue blade
(e.g. for apical post-alveolar articulations)
palatal articulations
lowered tongue middle (e.g. for velar articulations)
velar articulations
tongue fronting (e.g. for palatal articulations)
This list does not include movements in the horizontal axis. Every tongue part can
move backwards and forwards if it is up, i.e., fronting or backing can occur. These
possibilities are elaborated in (22). The downward movements have only limited
options of displacement, which are therefore omitted here.20
(22) tip
fronted
backed
blade fronted
backed
middle fronted
backed
back
fronted
backed
apical dental articulations
apical post-alveolar to palatal articulations
laminal dental to alveolar articulations
laminal post-alveolar to palatal articulations
alveolo-palatal articulations
velar articulations
velar articulations
pharyngeal articulations
This list of the fronting and backing movements of the tongue parts and that of the
raising and lowering movements in (21) indicate that the single tongue articulations
are not independent of each other: gestures involving the movement of one part of
the tongue always affect adjacent parts. Apical articulations cause the laminal area
to move, laminal articulations raise the apex and the tongue middle, and movements
of the tongue back cause a lowering of the tongue middle. Furthermore, certain
chain reactions can occur. The displacement of the tongue tip to the post-alveolar
region or further back (i.e. retroflexion) causes the tongue middle to lower and the
20
Both the backing of the tongue middle and fronting of the tongue back are assumed to result in a velar
articulation. This is due to the fact that horizontal displacements of the tongue body are rather restricted and do not seem to result in several articulatory classes.
164
Chapter 5
tongue back to rise. A graphic illustration of this is given in figure 5.11. The underlying grey line shows the tongue parts at position of rest, as in figure 5.10.
tip – blade – middle – back
Figure 5.11 Retroflexion of the tongue tip and concomitant movements of the whole tongue.
The restrictions on the co-occurrence of the tongue movements as given in (21) and
(22) have to be formalized explicitly; they cannot be expressed by grouping the respective tongue parts under a common node as assumed in FG. My formalization of
these interactions as articulatory implications together with constraints on the articulatory gestures introduced here is given in section 6.2 below.
5.5 Conclusion
It was shown in this chapter that the traditionally assumed three functions of
phonological features are incongruous: distinguishing natural classes is at crosspurposes with attempts to account for phonological processes and securing phonetic
interpretability. These functions have to be distinguished and realized by two distinct devices, one that is adaptable to language-specific phonetics and phonology,
and another, more abstract, one that can be used for the paralinguistic task of classification.
Connected with the functional restriction of features, it was proposed in the
present model that features are not innate. The main evidence for innateness of features is the existence of identical phonological processes and segment classes in unrelated languages that are accounted for in traditional approaches by ‘natural’
classes sharing the same feature. Thus it is traditionally assumed that retroflexes are
a natural class due to their innate, shared features [C ORONAL , –anterior,
–distributed]. This class is argued here to emerge, however, via similar (not necessarily identical) ways of articulation as described in chapter 2, and via their perceptual similarity, as described in chapter 3. The similar phonological behaviour of
these segments as illustrated in chapter 4 is therefore grounded in their similar phonetic properties (articulatory gestures and perceptual cues), and is not a result of
their sharing the same universal features.
Lastly, it was argued that phonological processes involve both articulatory and
perceptual information, and therefore both articulatory and perceptual representations are necessary in phonology. Instead of the traditionally assumed underlying
Phonological representations of retroflexes
165
articulatory specification, the present approach assumes an underlying perceptual
presentation from which the articulation is derived, as illustrated in figure 5.12.
+1 F2
−2 F3
underlying
perc. representation
[tongue tip up, tongue back retracted]
(⇒ […])
production
grammar
articulatory
realization
Figure 5.12 Representation of the perceptual UR of retroflexes and their realization in the articulation grammar by specific gestures (which imply other gestures, as illustrated in section 6.2 below), according to the model of FP applied here.
The perceptual features given on the left of figure 5.12 are only a very reduced set,
and are not meant to be a full specification, as already discussed in chapter 3. Furthermore, they are generalized across the languages discussed, in order to yield a
tool to describe all retroflexes in these languages similarly. However, the representations actually needed for the phonological description of a single language may
look different according to the inventory and processes found in this language. For
the case of the two retroflex fricatives in Toda, for instance, the apical post-alveolar
may be specified as [+1F2, –1F3], and the subapical palatal as [+1F2, –2F3]. Thus
the non-innate perceptual UR proposed here can avoid the problem of distinguishing
these two classes encountered in FG.
The articulatory representation on the righthand side of figure 5.12 and its implication of other gestures will be further specified in chapter 6.2. Both the underlying perceptual representation of retroflexes, and the articulatory gestures associated
with it, are put to use in the following chapter.
6
Analysis of retroflex processes
The aim of the present chapter is to provide analyses in an OT framework (Prince &
Smolensky 1993) for the diachronic and synchronic segmental changes described in
chapter 4, which are caused by a retroflex context or which result in retroflexion or
de-retroflexion. One exception to this general aim exists: this chapter does not
account for processes that were attributed to misparsing errors of listeners only.
Since no articulatory gestures are saved via such a misparsing process, no
articulatory optimisation strategies apply, and therefore misparsing has no functional
motivation in production. An analysis of a misparsing error in a functionally based
OT production grammar is thus impossible. In chapter 4 a reassociation of cues was
proposed as an alternative explanation for, for instance, a change from alveolar to
retroflex in a back vowel context or a change from a high front vowel to a rounded
high front vowel in a retroflex context. There I also illustrated the acoustic
similarities between the original forms and the forms the listener derived from
reassociation of cues. An OT formalization of these processes needs to be modelled
in a perception grammar but this would be outside the scope of this book. Only one
process below will include a formalization of a perception grammar, namely
secondary palatalization of retroflexes in section 6.3.3.5. In that section, both
production and perception grammar are modelled to illustrate that the gestural
sequence of a retroflex and a palatal glide can be perceived as a secondarily
palatalized retroflex.
This chapter is structured in the following way. Section 6.1 introduces the
constraints on the perceptual specifications used here, section 6.2 deals with the
constraints on the articulation, section 6.3 provides analyses for the processes
discussed in chapter 4, and section 6.4 concludes. Together, these sections provide
phonological accounts for the processes illustrated in chapter 4. Furthermore, crosslinguistic variation that could be found for some processes will be accounted for,
either by phonetically motivated universal rankings of articulatory markedness
constraints and language-specific rankings of faithfulness constraints, as in the case
of vowel retraction and lowering (section 6.3.3.2), or by faithfulness constraints that
are universally ranked plus language-dependent rankings of articulatory markedness
constraints, as in the case of context-sensitive assimilation processes (section 6.3.6).
6.1 Constraints on perceptual representations
For the analysis of processes involving retroflexes, I assume underlying perceptual
specifications as illustrated in section 5.4.1 above. Departing from Boersma (1998,
1999, 2003a, b), the perceptual specifications are represented as underlying
representations or inputs in a traditional slashed line notation, such as /i/ for a high
front vowel. The perceptual URs are associated with articulatory gestures that
168
Chapter 6
implement these specifications. Every output candidate to be evaluated is thus a pair
of perceptual features correlated with articulatory gestures (Boersma 1998: 146):
[art1] /perc1/. Boersma’s notation does not list all the articulatory gestures and
perceptual features present in a segment, but gives shorthands using the easily
interpretable IPA symbols. The present exposition proceeds similarly. Furthermore,
in the present analysis the output pairs [art1] /perc1/ are further abbreviated to one
form, the traditionally used IPA transcription in square brackets, such as [i]. It has to
be kept in mind, though, that this stands for a candidate output form with both
articulatory and perceptual specifications.
Due to my assumption that underlying specifications are perceptual,
faithfulness constraints (henceforth: faith constraints) only hold for the perceptual
representations of items. Being faithful to an articulation is hence not possible,
contrary to what is assumed in standard versions of OT (McCarthy & Prince 1993,
1995; Prince & Smolensky 1993). Three constraint families ensuring the faithfulness
to features or segment content are distinguished here: *DELETE, *INSERT, and
*REPLACE, following Boersma (1998). A further constraint family preventing the
change in timing of two adjacent features or segments is used, namely the PATH
constraints (Boersma 1998: 193f.).
*DELETE constraints militate against the deletion of underlying perceptual
features, and are similar to McCarthy & Prince’s (1995) IDENT. The *DELETE
constraint family can prohibit deletion of single features, see for example (1a) and
(1b).
(1)
(a) *DELETE (+1F2): “Don’t delete the underlyingly specified feature [+1F2].”
(b) *DELETE (–2F3): “Don’t delete the underlyingly specified feature [–2F3].”
Faithfulness to the combinations of features in a segment can be described by
constraint conjunction. If a language has retroflex segments, for example, the two
features [+1F2] and [–2F3] cooccur to form the class of ‘retroflex’. Deletion of a
retroflex segment violates a conjoint constraint as defined in (2a). In (2b) – (2d)
constraints are formulated militating against feature combinations that determine
specific manners of articulation (in a shorthand notation instead of listing all the
cues for stops, fricatives, approximants, and so on, in detail). (2e) illustrates a
constraint preserving combinations of features for manner and place.
(2)
(a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
(e)
*DELETE (+1F2 & –2F3): “Don’t delete both underlyingly specified
features [+1F2] and [–2F3] (i.e. a retroflex).”
*DELETE (stop): “Don’t delete the underlyingly specified stop features
(silence, burst, etc.).”
*DELETE (rhotic): “Don’t delete the underlyingly specified rhotic
features (formant weakening, etc.).”
*DELETE (fricative): “Don’t delete the underlyingly specified fricative
features (friction noise, intensity, etc.).”
*DELETE ( ): “Don’t delete the underlyingly specified retroflex stop
features.”
Analysis of retroflex processes
169
Naturally, we can assume that faith constraints applying to one feature only are
violated far more often than those for feature combinations, since the deletion of one
feature is perceptually more tolerable than the deletion of several features.
We can see from the examples in (1) and (2) that the faith constraints can
target three different types of arguments: single features as in (1a) and (1b); feature
combinations for describing natural classes, which are usually given with shorthand
notations and not with all features listed, such as (2b) – (2d); or thirdly feature
combinations that describe specific segment classes, e.g. (2e) which targets the
group of retroflex plosives. The last type of argument is notated with IPA symbols.
If only one feature is deleted, inserted, or changed, Boersma’s constraints
correspond to McCarthy & Prince’s (1995) IDENT constraint, which militates against
changes of single features,1 If feature combinations making up a whole segment are
deleted, inserted, or changed, the corresponding constraints in McCarthy & Prince’s
framework are Max-IO against deletion, DEP-IO against insertion, and both against
segmental changes.
The second type of faith constraints used here are *INSERT constraints, which
prohibit the insertion of surface features or feature clusters. *INSERT constraints are
defined analogously to the *DELETE constraints. Examples are given where needed
in the analysis below.
The third type of faith constraint used for the present analysis is the *REPLACE
constraint family, which accounts for changes of one feature value into another
value on the same perceptual tier. *REPLACE constraints depart from McCarthy &
Prince’s approach, where a process of feature change is formalized as violation of an
IDENT constraint. What IDENT fails to capture is the possibility of gradual changes in
feature-values, and the fact that a small change is better accepted (i.e. causes fewer
constraint violations) than the deletion of a whole feature and the insertion of a new
one. A definition of *REPLACE is given in (3), from Boersma (1998: 176f.).
(3)
*REPLACE (feature: value1, value2 / condition / left-env_right-env):
“Do not replace a specified value (value1) on a perceptual tier (feature) with a
different value (value2), under a certain condition and in the environment leftenv and right-env.”
Examples of universally ranked, gradient *REPLACE constraints that are sensitive to
the context will be given in section 6.3.6 below.
Faithfulness to the underlying simultaneity or non-simultaneity of features will
be expressed in terms of PATH constraints (Boersma 1998: 193f., based on
Archangeli & Pulleyblank 1994). *INSERTPATH as defined in (4) militates against
the insertion of a path between two features that was not specified in the underlying
representation, see the definition in (4) (from Boersma 1998: 194).
(4)
1
*INSERTPATH (f × g): “A combined feature on the tiers f and g that is heard in
the surface form, also occurs in the specification.”
Zoll (1996) subdivides McCarthy & Prince’s (1995) IDENT constraint into MAX [F] and DEP [F],
analogous to MAX-IO and DEP-IO.
170
Chapter 6
Outputs violating *INSERTPATH constraints create a simultaneity of two features that
was not present in the underlying representation.
*DELETEPATH constraints as defined in (5) (also Boersma 1998: 194) militate
against the deletion of a path, which results in non-simultaneity of the underlyingly
simultaneous features involved.
(5)
*DELETEPATH (f × g): “A specified combined feature on the tiers f and g
appears (is heard) in the surface form.”
Specific PATH constraints needed for the present analysis will be defined below
when necessary.
6.2 Constraints on articulatory representations
Before defining the necessary constraints on the articulatory representations, we
have to go back to the model of independent tongue parts introduced in 5.4.2,
repeated here in figure 6.1, because it has consequences for possible articulations
and therefore restricts our articulatory representations of segments.
tip – blade – middle – back
Figure 6.1
Abstract assumption on tongue parts and their movements on the vertical axis.
This model divides the tongue into four parts, the tip, blade, middle, and back
(including the root), with borders as defined in chapter 2. Contrary to distinctions of
the tongue in other models, for instance the coronal – dorsal distinction in FG as
described in chapter 5, the present model does not assume that the separate tongue
parts can act independently of each other. The four parts can separately be the active
articulator in producing the restriction relevant for specific articulatory classes, but
the movement of one part triggers certain movements of adjacent and even nonadjacent parts. This was illustrated in chapter 5. The raising of the tongue blade, for
example, was shown to cause the tongue middle to rise as well, though not to the
same extent as an active constriction of the tongue middle would. Restrictions on
adjacent tongue parts can be expressed formally as in (6), which is not a complete
list.
(6)
(a)
(b)
*[tongue blade up, tongue middle down]
*[tongue blade down, tongue middle up]
Analysis of retroflex processes
(c)
(d)
171
*[tongue middle down, tongue back down]
*[tongue middle up, tongue back up]
These restrictions are not active cooccurrence constraints (such as proposed for
example by Calabrese 1988 or Hamilton 1996), as they need not be present in the
phonology of the speaker. Instead, these statements express physical restrictions on
the vocal apparatus, and formalize what is impossible to articulate. They restrict the
number of possible output candidates (thus are part of Gen in traditional OT terms),
as outputs can only be producible articulations (Boersma 1998: 278).
For complex articulations such as retroflex segments, the articulatory
restrictions can be understood as some chain implication, as formalized in (7a).
(7)
(a)
(b)
[tongue tip up & back] → [tongue blade front & displaced],
[tongue middle down], [tongue back up]
[tongue blade up]
→ [tongue middle up], [tongue back down]
The interaction of the tongue parts for retroflex articulation as given in (7a) can
account for several phonological restrictions of retroflexes as illustrated in sections
6.3.1 until 6.3.6.
It is important to note that the division of the tongue into parts and their
interplay as assumed here is not exclusively designed for modelling retroflex
articulations. (7b) gives the interactions of tongue parts for a laminal articulation:
here the raising of the laminal area causes a concomitant raising of the tongue
middle. This correlation can be found in phonetic accounts of laminals, which are
often described as slightly palatalized. Furthermore, it can account for the fact that
laminal but not apical stops show additional affrication: due to the raising of the
tongue middle, laminals have a long area of constriction behind the closure, which
promotes affrication in stop release.2
After clarifying these articulatory correlations and their restrictions on the
vocal apparatus, we can now move on to the articulatory constraints necessary for
our representations. Restrictions on articulation hold only for the articulatory form,
and are therefore formulated as markedness constraints on the articulatory part of
output. General restrictions on the effort of producing an articulation (described for
instance by LAZY by Kirchner 1997) are further distinguished here into *GESTURE,
*DISTANCE, *SYNCHRONIZE and *PRECISION constraints.
The first type of articulatory constraint described here is the *GESTURE
constraint family as developed by Boersma (1998: 152). For our purpose, we need
the following *GESTURE constraints:
(8)
2
(a)
*GESTURE (tongue tip: neutral to post-alveolar) = *GESTURE (retroflex):
“do not articulate a retroflex segment.”
Kehrein (2002: 60f.) describes the correlation of laminality and affrication and ascribes it to the
displacement and slowness of laminal articulations. Laminal articulations, however, are not by default
displaced: laminal post-alveolars for example involve only a raising of the tongue blade but no
displacement (recall section 5.4.2). Furthermore, it is not clear how the slowness of laminal gestures
can account for the occurring affrication. The explanation proposed here with cooccurring tongue
middle raising thus seems superior to the one given by Kehrein.
172
Chapter 6
(b)
(c)
*GESTURE (tongue tip: neutral to alveolar) = *GESTURE (apical alveolar):
“do not articulate an apical dental or alveolar segment.”
*GESTURE (tongue back: neutral to retracted) = *GESTURE (retraction):
“do not articulate a segment with a retracted tongue back.”
The constraints in (8a–c) militate against specific articulatory classes, namely
retroflexes, apical alveolar/dentals, and secondary articulations such as retraction for
non-retroflex segments. If a language has retroflex segments, the constraint (8a) is
very low ranked. If a language has an apical but no retroflex series, the constraints
are ranked as *GESTURE (retroflex) >> *GESTURE (apical alveolar).
In addition to the constraints against the articulation of specific classes in (8),
further constraints on articulation are needed that can account for the reluctance of
the articulators to change their position from one gesture to another. Boersma’s
(1998: 150) *DISTANCE constraint family seems to meet this purpose, see (9), where
| stands for an articulatory contour, i.e. a change in position (or tension) of the
articulator.
(9)
*DISTANCE (articulator: a | b): “An articulator does not move from location
a to b, away from the neutral position.”
For our purposes, we need specific *DISTANCE constraints as defined in (10).
(10) (a)
(b)
(c)
*DISTANCE (tongue tip: retroflex | alveolar) = *DISTANCE (tip):
“The tongue tip does not move from location post-alveolar to alveolar
and vice versa.”
*DISTANCE (tongue back: retracted | fronted) = *DISTANCE (back):
“The tongue back does not move from location retracted to fronted and
vice versa.”
*DISTANCE (tongue tip: manner1 | manner2) = *DISTANCE (manner):
“The tongue tip does not move from location manner1 to manner2 (e.g.
from approximant to full closure).”
(10a) and (10b) militate against changes in place of articulation, whereas (10c)
avoids a change in apical manner. Constraint (10b), which disallows a change in
tongue back position, also militates against a change from flat tongue middle to high
tongue middle by the articulatory restrictions on tongue movements elaborated
above. It applies to all retroflex articulations due to the correlation stated in (7a)
above.
A universal ranking of a specific *DISTANCE constraint family and its
relevance for language-specific variation will be illustrated in section 6.3.3.2 below
on vowel retraction and vowel lowering.
Besides *GESTURE and *DISTANCE , a third type of articulatory constraint
family, namely *SYNC, is required for our analysis. *SYNC constraints describe the
unwillingness of two gestures on different tiers to exactly finish the first gesture
when the second starts. A general definition is given in (11) (Boersma 1998: 154).
Analysis of retroflex processes
173
(11) *SYNC (articulator1: from1 | t o1; articulator2: from2 | to2): “the movement of
articulator1 from from 1 to t o1 is not synchronous with the movement of
articulator2 from from2 to to2.”
The articulatory result of this constraint might be either an overlap or a delay
between two gestures. For describing sequences of front vowels and retroflex
consonants and the gestural overlap to be observed there, we need the following
*SYNC constraint:
(12) *SYNC (tongue middle: raised | lowered; tongue tip: neutral | closed) = *SYNC
(vowel| retroflex): “the lowering of the tongue middle (for the vowel) is not
synchronized with the closure of the tongue tip (for the retroflex).”
A synchronization of the articulatory gestures is not specified in the underlying,
perceptual representation. It can only be achieved by being faithful to the
synchronization of the perceptual features, as stated in the PATH constraints in (4)
and (5). Non-synchronization of gestures is therefore accepted as long as it does not
result in a shift of the perceptual features.
A last articulatory constraint family that will be needed for the present analysis
is *PRECISION as formulated in (13) (Boersma 1998: 155).
(13) *PRECISION (articulator: position | environment): “In a certain environment, a
certain articulator does not work up the precision to put itself in a certain
position.”
This constraint family can account for the fact that some manner classes have to be
articulated more precise than others. A trill, for instance, requires a more precise
gesture than a flap, thus the following ranking holds:
(14) *PRECISION (trill) >> *PRECISION (flap)
6.3 Analysis of phonological processes of retroflexes
With the constraints restricting perceptual and articulatory specification in place, we
can now formalize the patterning of the phenomena of retroflexion as illustrated in
chapter 4, repeated here in (15) for convenience.
(15)
input
/rt/
/ut/
/i/
/ /
(d) /V/
(e) //
(f) /t/
/Vt/
(a)
(b)
(c)
output
[]
[u]
[it] or []
[t ] or []
[V ]
(ω[t], [Ct]
[]
[V]
process
retroflexion in rhotic context
retroflexion in back vowel context
deretroflexion or vowel retraction
deretroflexion or depalatalization
retroflexion of adjacent vowel
phonotactic restrictions on retroflexes
local assimilation of retroflexes
non-local assimilation of retroflexes
174
Chapter 6
The following subsections 6.3.1 – 6.3.6 will analyse the processes (15a) – (15f), in
that order.
6.3.1 Retroflexion via rhotics
Section 4.1.2 proposed two possible explanations for the process of retroflexion in
rhotic context in (15a), repeated here in (16). Processes with a reversed order of
rhotic and retroflex are not discussed here, as they can be treated identically.
(16) (a)
(b)
rt > t > >
rt >
staged process (diachronically only)
The process in (16a) involves several stages of the historical development, whereas
(16b) consists of only one stage. Let us start with formalizing (16b). This process
involves the collapse of two segmental categories with different manners but similar
place into one (with different place), which can be expressed via the articulatory
markedness constraint *DISTANCE (manner) defined above in (10c). Furthermore,
the collapse involves a change in surface representation compared to the underlying
perceptual representation. Part of the perceptual representations of the two input
segments /r/ and /t/, the occurring output //, and another possible output / / is given
in table 6.1.
Table 6.1
Partial perceptual representations of /r/, /t/, // and / /, with features specifying
place of articulation and for manner.
place
manner
r
t
–2F3
2trill
0F3
2stop
–2F3
2stop
–2F3
1trill
1stop
The trill /r/ in the first column and the flap / / in the last column are both assigned
the perceptual manner feature [trill], since a trill with a single contact sounds like a
flap (Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996: 237). They differ in feature value, however: the
trill has the value [2trill] and the flap the value [1trill], since the latter segment
sounds less trill than the former. Both stop and flap are specified by the feature
[stop], because flaps very often behave phonologically like stops. Again, the two
segments differ in the feature values: stops are [2stop] and flaps [1stop].
The output in (16b) is unfaithful to the underlying form in two ways: it violates
*DELETE (0F3) since the underlying sequence [-2F3, 0F3] is realized as [-2F3], and
it violates *DELETE (trill), since the underlying sequence trill – stop is realized as
stop. This is formalized in the tableau in (17) on the next page, which models the
retroflexion rule in Norwegian.
The faith constraints *DELETE (–2F3) and *DELETE (stop) secure the features
[–2F3] (of a rhotic or a retroflex) and [stop], respectively. The winning candidate
does not violate any of these, as it contains both features in the output.
xxxxxxxxxxxx
Analysis of retroflex processes
/rt/
(17)
*DELETE
(–2F3)
*DELETE
*REPLACE
(stop)
(2stop, 1stop)
[rt]
175
*DISTANCE
(manner)
*DELETE
(trill)
*!
*
[]
*!
[r]
[t]
*!
*
*!
[]
The constraint *REPLACE (2stop, 1stop) is necessary to exclude the last candidate, the
flap, which does not violate *DELETE (stop) since it contains stop cues. *REPLACE
(2stop, 1stop) has to be ranked below *DELETE (stop) because it is worse to delete the
whole [2stop] feature than to replace its value by [1stop]. *DELETE (stop) must be
higher ranked than *DELETE (trill) in Norwegian, as the winning candidate is faithful
only to the underlying stop specification, not to the trill specification.
In section 4.1 we saw an example of a language where the winning candidate
is faithful to the trill specification: in Yidgha, the sequence /rt/ developed to the
retroflex / /. This development can be accounted for by a reverse ranking of the
*DELETE (trill) and *DELETE (stop) constraints than in (17) (the ranking of
*R EPLACE (2stop, 1stop) below *DELETE (stop) remains fixed, of course). A
complication in the Yidgha development is the manner change from a trill to a flap.
The flap emerges as the winning candidate because it secures both manner features
of the underlying specification.
/rt/
(18)
*DELETE
(–2F3)
*DELETE
(trill)
[rt]
*DELETE
(stop)
*REPLACE
(2stop, 1stop)
*!
*!
[]
*!
[r]
[t]
*DISTANCE
(manner)
*!
*
*
[]
To account for the order of the input segments, i.e. for the fact that the rhotic
has to occur to the left of the target segment, we have to assume either contextsensitive faith constraints or context-sensitive markedness constraints. These
context-sensitive faith constraints and their rankings would look like (19a) for
Norwegian and (19b) for Yidgha, and the context-sensitive markedness constraints
and their rankings like (20a) for Norwegian and (20b) for Yidgha, where A stands
for all apical alveolars, and r for a trill.
(19) (a)
(b)
(20) (a)
(b)
*DELETE (trill / A_)
*DELETE (stop / _r)
*DISTANCE (trill, non-trill)
*DISTANCE (trill, stop)
>> *DELETE (trill / _A)
>> *DELETE (stop / r_)
>> *DISTANCE (non-trill, trill)
>> *DISTANCE (stop, trill)
176
Chapter 6
The second explanation for retroflexion in a rhotic context with subsequent
drop of the rhotic, (16a), has to be modelled in three diachronic stages, given in (21).
(21) (a)
(b)
(c)
r >
t >
>
Consider the process at stage one, shown in (21a), which describes the change of an
apical rhotic into a retroflex one, caused by articulatory variation. As we already
saw in the development in Yidgha, the apical trill and the retroflex flap show little
perceptual difference. The trill, however, requires a more precise articulation than a
flap, which is expressed by a *PRECISION (trill) constraint being higher ranked than
a *PRECISION (flap) constraint, see (14) above. The only constraint preventing a
development as in (21a) is the faith constraint *REPLACE (2trill, 1trill), which tries to
make sure that the trill feature is realized as a trill and not as a flap. If both
constraints are unranked with respect to each other, variation will emerge, as in (22):
(22)
/r/
*DELETE
(trill)
*PRECISION
(trill)
*REPLACE
(2trill, 1trill)
*
[r]
*
[]
[t]
*!
This tableau is interpreted as the front apical rhotic winning in approximately 50
percent of the cases, and the retroflex rhotic in the other 50 percent of the cases (see
Anttila 1995, Bermúdez-Otero 1996, and Löhken 1997 for OT models with such
free variation). Let us assume that this variation eventually ceases and that the
retroflex flap emerges as the established pronunciation of the rhotic in such a
language (this might be caused by *PRECISION (trill) eventually being higher ranked
than *REPLACE (2trill, 1trill)).
The next stage in the development of a retroflex series is the assimilation of a
following alveolar to the retroflex rhotic, see (21b). Here, only a change in the place
of articulation takes place, which is perceivable as a change from a sequence [–2F3,
0F3] to a continuous [–2F3]. This development is illustrated in tableau (23).
(23)
/ t/
*DELETE
(–2F3)
*DELETE
(trill)
*DELETE
(2stop)
[ t]
*!
[]
*!
*!
*
*
*!
[]
[rt]
*DELETE
(0F3)
*
[ ]
[t]
*DISTANCE
(tip)
*!
*
Analysis of retroflex processes
177
In tableau (23) the constraint *DISTANCE (tip) militates against the change from one
apical place of articulation to another, recall (10a). The faith constraints securing the
trill and stop manner cues, as well as those for retroflex place, are ranked higher
than the one for the alveolar place (*DELETE (0F3)), thus candidates three, four, and
five, which delete either one manner or the retroflex, are worse than candidates one
and two, which preserve both manner and retroflexion. The first candidate, which is
most faithful to the input, loses due to the effort saving constraint *DISTANCE (tip)
being ranked higher than the faithfulness constraint for the alveolar.
The third and last stage of the development of a retroflex series, (21c), is the
reduction of the sequence rhotic retroflex plus retroflex of another manner to the
non-rhotic retroflex. This process obviously involves ranking the faith constraint for
the rhotic feature lower than in previous stages, and ranking the constraint
*DISTANCE (manner) higher.
| |
(24)
*DELETE
(2stop)
[ ]
*DISTANCE
(manner)
*!
*DELETE
(1trill)
*
[]
[]
*!
6.3.2 Retroflexion via back vowels
As illustrated in section 4.2, front apicals can retroflex in a back vowel context. In
Margany, for instance, the underlying sequence /un/ is produced as [u]. The
perceptual representations of these segments plus that of /i/ are given in table 6.2
(based on the specifications in table 5.11 and 5.12).
Table 6.2
Perceptual representations of /n/, //, /u/ and /i/.
[F1]
[F2]
[F3]
manner-specific
n
+1
0
nasal
+1
–2
nasal
u
i
–3
–4
–2
–3
+4
The affinity of back vowels and retroflexes was described in 4.2 as articulatorily as
well as perceptually motivated. Perceptually, a re-association of the [–2F3] of [u] as
belonging to the formant structure of the consonant can occur. Articulatorily, a
reduction of gestures occurs: the input sequence involves a tongue back gesture from
the retracted /u/ to the non-retracted front coronal. In the output sequence, both [u]
and retroflex are retracted. The reduced number of gestures can be captured by the
*DISTANCE (back) constraint as defined in (10b), which militates against a change in
tongue back position. The gestural change violates *DELETE (0F3) since the
underlying sequence [–2F3, 0F3] is realized as a continuous [–2F3].
178
(25)
Chapter 6
/un/
*REPLACE
(–4F2, +4F2)
[un]
*DISTANCE
(back)
*!
*
[u]
[in]
*DELETE
(0F3)
*!
The ranking of the perceptual faith constraint *REPLACE (–4F2, +4F2) above the
faith constraint *DELETE (0F3) prevents a change from [u] to [i] in the output;
instead, the consonant assimilates to the retracted tongue back of the vowel.
In section 4.2.2 it was observed that primarily nasals, laterals, and rhotics
undergo such a change towards a retroflex place of articulation. Stops mainly
change only if the other manner classes changed, too. This asymmetrical behaviour
is due to the fact that nasals, laterals, and rhotics have weak place cues, which can
hardly be distinguished from each other. In our OT analysis such a difference in
manner can be expressed by faith constraints for stops being higher ranked than
those for nasals, laterals, and rhotics.3 The fact that in Margany apical nasals are
retroflexed in back vowel context, but stops are not, (see section 4.2.1), can now be
formalized with the two manner-specific faith constraints in (26).
(26) *DELETE (0F3 / plosives) >> * DELETE (0F3 / nasals)
Tableaux with this ranking and the two underlying forms /un/ and /ut/ are given in
(27a) and (27b). In this tableau it becomes obvious that the two constraints
*REPLACE (–4F2, +4F2) and *DISTANCE (back) have to be ranked with respect to
each other in Margany (*REPLACE (–4F2; +4F2) has to be ranked above *DISTANCE
(back)), otherwise [it] and [ut] would both be possible outputs of /ut/, which is not
the case.
(27)
/un/
*DELETE
(0F3 / stops)
*REPLACE
(–4F2, +4F2)
[un]
*DISTANCE
(back)
*!
*
[u]
*!
[in]
/ut/
*DELETE
(0F3 / stops)
*REPLACE
(–4F2, +4F2)
[ut]
[u]
[it]
3
*DELETE
(0F3 / nasals)
*DISTANCE
(back)
*
*DELETE
(0F3 / nasals)
*!
*!
See Boersma (1998: 217ff.) for an elaboration of the asymmetry in place assimilations and their
formalization as implicational universals.
Analysis of retroflex processes
179
6.3.3 Non-occurrence of retroflexes with front vowels or palatalization
Section 4.3 gave examples of retroflex segments avoiding a front vowel context.
This avoidance is realized in two ways, either by a change of the retroflex into an
alveolar, or by a change of the front vowel. These avoidance processes are
summarized in (28) and (29). Again, only one order of vowel and retroflex sequence
is illustrated here, the reverse order follows from the same analysis (with contextsensitive constraints accounting for the direction of the process, as illustrated for
retroflexion in rhotic context in (19) and (20)).
(28)
(29) (a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
input
/i/
/i/
//
/i/
/i/
output
[it]
[] or []
[æ]
[i]
[y]
process
de-retroflexion
retraction
lowering
diphthongization (schwa insertion)
rounding
Both strategies were also observed for secondary palatalization of retroflexes, see
(27a) and (27b). Furthermore, secondary palatalization can also be realized as a
separate palatal glide, see (27c).
(30) (a)
(b)
(c)
input
/j/
/j/
/j/
output
[tj]
[]
[j]
process
de-retroflexion
de-palatalization
separate palatal
Let us look at front vowel – retroflex sequences first. As in the case of retroflexion
in a back vowel context, accounted for in the previous section, the cause of
avoidance of the front vowel context is that retroflexes have a flat tongue middle and
a retracted tongue back. Front vowels, on the other hand, have a raised and fronted
tongue middle. The change in tongue position from front vowels to retroflexes or
vice versa requires a large gestural movement within a short time. This articulatory
difficulty can be expressed by the markedness constraint *DISTANCE (back).
Furthermore, the non-synchronicity of the gesture of tongue middle lowering (at the
end of the vowel) with the gesture of tongue tip raising and closure (at the beginning
of the retroflex) is accounted for with the *SYNC (vowel | retroflex) constraint as
defined in (12). With these constraints in hand, we can now formalize the different
avoidance strategies illustrated in (28) and (29).
6.3.3.1 De-retroflexion
Processes like (28), in which a segment is de-retroflexed before or after a front
vowel, can be accounted for in a similar way as the retroflexion in back vowel
context depicted in (25): the faith constraint securing the unchanged vowel and the
*DISTANCE (back) constraint that militates against a tongue back movement are
ranked above the faith constraint for the retroflex, see tableau (31) on the next page.
180
(31)
Chapter 6
/i/
*REPLACE
(+4F2, –4F2)
[i]
[u]
*DISTANCE
(back)
*!
*DELETE
(–2F3)
*!
[it]
*
The most faithful, first output candidate violates the constraint against a change in
the tongue back position from retracted to fronted. The second candidate, with a
back vowel instead of the underlying front vowel, violates the very high ranked
*REPLACE (+4F2, –4F2) constraint that secures the vowel cues.
6.3.3.2 Vowel retraction and lowering
Let us now look at the cases with a change in the vowel quality, see (29). These
processes occur in languages in which some gestural markedness constraints such as
*DISTANCE (back) and *SYNC (vowel | retroflex) are higher ranked than the faith
constraints for the vowels. Vowel retraction and vowel lowering can be modelled by
high-ranked *DISTANCE (back) constraints, which have to be further refined for this
purpose. Vowel retraction is caused by the large gestural distance between /i/ and
the retroflex, stated as *DISTANCE (i-R) in (32).
(32) *DISTANCE (i-R): “The tongue does not move from raised middle and fronted
back position for the high front vowel [i] to lowered middle and retracted
back position for a retroflex, or vice versa.”
The articulation of a distance from front high vowel to retroflex can be avoided by
retracting the vowel to [] (as in Ponapean, Bunuba, Russian, or Polish). But even
with this retracted tongue position, there is a distance to the retroflex gesture,
because [] still involves a raising of the tongue middle (though no fronting of the
tongue back). Articulating this distance between [] and a retroflex militates against
the constraint *DISTANCE (-R). Some languages (such as Toda) have a further
retraction of the high front vowel to [], where the tongue gesture is very close to
that of the retroflex consonant. This distance is captured by the constraint
*DISTANCE (-R). The decreasing gestural distances of the three vowel – retroflex
combinations can be represented by ranking the three *DISTANCE (vowel – retroflex)
constraints in the following way.
(33) *DISTANCE (i-R) >> *DISTANCE (-R) >> *DISTANCE (-R)
The ranking in (33) can be regarded to be universal in the sense that it is
phonetically inviolable: the distance between the articulatory position for an [i] and
that of a retroflex is always larger than that between the gesture of an [] and a
retroflex. The universal ranking in (33) is part of the phonetic space where languagespecific phonetic cues and gestures are chosen from, and is therefore specified in the
phonetics of a speaker. It is, however, not present in every language-specific
phonology, only in those that employ all gestures specified in this hierarchy: a
language without any retroflex category does not contain the hierarchy in (30) in its
Analysis of retroflex processes
181
phonology. Furthermore, the hierarchy interacts with language-specific faithfulness
constraints in languages that employ retroflexes and all three vowel categories. This
point will be further illustrated below.
Vowel retraction occurs also for the mid front vowel /e/, which retracts to the
less distant [] before a retroflex for example in Ponapean. This is given by the
universal constraint hierarchy in (34).
(34) *DISTANCE (e-R) >> *DISTANCE (-R)
As far as vowel lowering is concerned, the retraction hierarchies in (33) and (34) are
inappropriate. We have to assume the same maximal distance for the high front
vowel and the retroflex, i.e. the same *DISTANCE (i-R) constraint is ranked highest.
The constraint on the other end of the hierarchy, however, militates against the
distance between the retroflex and the lowest vowel [a], i.e. *DISTANCE (a-R),
which is the minimal distance one can get for the process of high front vowel
lowering. In-between these two extremes, constraints against articulating the
distance between retroflex and [e], [], and [æ], respectively, can be stated. This
results in the universal ranking given in (35).
(35) *DISTANCE (i-R) >> *DISTANCE (e-R) >> *DISTANCE (-R) >>
*DISTANCE (æ-R) >> *DISTANCE (a-R)
As we saw in section 4.3.2, vowel lowering is not restricted to the high front vowel,
but sometimes also occurs with the high back vowel /u/ (e.g. in Kayardild, Gunya,
Tamil, and Kannada). This vowel is lowered towards the mid back vowel [o] only;
no further lowering (e.g. to the low back []) occurs. The mid back vowel [o] has a
lowered tongue middle and a slightly raised tongue back (towards the velum), and is
thus nearly identical to the tongue position for a retroflex consonant. The nearly
identical gestures of [o] and a retroflex result in a minimal distance for *DISTANCE
(o-R), i.e., this constraint can be assumed to be lower ranked than any of the other
distance constraints. For back vowel lowering, we can thus assume the following
ranking:
(36) *DISTANCE (u-R) >> *DISTANCE (o-R)
The articulatory distance between [u] and a retroflex is the same as the distance
between [] and a retroflex, since [u] and [] differ in the shape of the lips only.
Thus, these two constraints are ranked equally. This and the fact that *DISTANCE (oR) is the lowest ranked of all the *DISTANCE (back) constraints lead to a universal
ranking of (33), (34), (35), and (36) as in figure 6.2 on the next page.
Violation of a higher-ranked *DISTANCE constraint implies simultaneous
violations of the lower-ranked ones. The constraint rankings in figure 6.2 interact
language-specifically with vowel faith constraints. A language with very high
ranked vowel faith constraints (i.e. above *DISTANCE (i-R)) would disallow any
vowel changes. Examples of other language-specific rankings are given below.
182
Chapter 6
*DIST (i-R)
*DIST (e-R)
*DIST (-R)
*DIST (-R) *DIST (-R)
*DIST (æ-R)
*DIST (-R) / *DIST (u-R)
*DIST (a-R)
*DIST (o-R)
Figure 6.2
Ranking of *DISTANCE (back) constraints for vowel lowering and retraction,
where the uppermost constraint *DISTANCE (i-R) is ranked highest, and the
constraint *DISTANCE (o-R) is ranked lowest. The ranking of the constraints on
the right side (responsible for vowel retraction) are not fixed with respect to
those on the left side (responsible for vowel lowering). They can be ranked
higher or lower (further developed in figure 6.3 and 6.4), as long as they
preserve their inherent ranking.
Let us look at vowel retraction and vowel lowering successively.
Retraction. Taking the vowel retraction from /i/ to [] after retroflex fricatives in
Russian as example, this process can be modelled as in (37), with a high-ranked
*REPLACE (+4F2, –2F2) constraint militating against a retraction from /i/ to [], and
a lower ranked *REPLACE (+4F2, +1F2), preventing a retraction from /i/ to [].
Between these two is ranked the *REPLACE (–2F3, 0F3) which prohibits a change in
the retroflex fricative.
(37)
/i/
*DIST
(i-R)
[i]
*!
*REPLACE
*REPLACE
(+4F2, –2F2) (–2F3, 0F3)
*REPLACE
(+4F2, +1F2)
*DIST
(-R)
*
*
[]
*
*!
[si]
*
*!
[]
In Toda, the front high vowel is retracted as far as the back vowel [], as
given in tableau (38). The *REPLACE (+4F2, –2F2) constraint is lower ranked than in
tableau (37), allowing a further retraction of the vowel to [].
(38)
/i/
*DIST
(i-R)
[i]
*!
*REPLACE *DIST
(–2F3,
(-R)
0F3)
*
[]
*REPLACE
(+4F2,
+1F2)
*DIST
(-R)
*
*!
[]
[it]
*REPLACE
(+4F2,
–2F2)
*
*
*
*
*!
*
Analysis of retroflex processes
183
Retraction of the high front rounded /y/ to the back [u], observable in Pingding
Chinese, can be modelled in the same way as the vowel retraction in Toda (by
replacing *DISTANCE (i-R) with *DISTANCE (y-R)), as rounding does not change
anything in the distance between vowel and retroflexion gesture. This process is not
illustrated here.
Ponapean shows retraction of the mid-front vowel /e/ to [ ], which is
accounted for in (39), where *REPLACE (1trill, 2trill) militates against a change from
underlying flap to surface trill.
(39)
/ e/
[ e]
*DISTANCE
(e-R)
*!
*REPLACE
(1trill, 2trill)
*REPLACE
(+4F2, +1F2)
*
[ ]
*!
[re]
According to the general constraint hierarchy of *DISTANCE constraints in figure
6.2, this retraction implies that the high front vowel also retracts, if it exists in the
language under consideration, since *DISTANCE (i-R) is ranked higher than
*DISTANCE (e-R). And indeed, the Ponapean /i/ retracts to [], see the data in (21) in
chapter 4. However, due to the flexible ranking of the constraints *REPLACE (vowel
features), languages that only show a retraction of the mid vowel are possible.
The possible rankings of the markedness constraints for vowel retraction and
the respective faith constraints for the vowels are summarized in figure 6.3.
no retraction
*DIST (i-R)
retraction of /i/ to []
*DIST (-R)
retraction of // to [] or [u]
*DIST (-R)/*DIST (u-R)
no high vowel in R context
*DIST (e-R)
retraction of /e/ to []
*DIST (-R)
no mid front vowel in R
context
*DIST (-R)
*DIST (æ-R)
*DIST (a-R)
*DIST (o-R)
Figure 6.3
Ranking of the *DISTANCE (back) constraints and vowel place faith constraints
(indicated by the dashed lines) for vowel retraction.
184
Chapter 6
In figure 6.3, I make two assumptions. First, the constraints securing the F1 of
the vowels (against vowel lowering) are ranked high (thus, lowering is not included
in this figure). Second, all constraints on the F2 of the vowels are ranked at the same
height, namely that of the dashed lines.
The position of the partial ranking *DISTANCE (e-R) >> *DISTANCE (-R) is
not fixed in figure 6.3, i.e. it can be placed higher than indicated here, as is the case
for Ponapean, see figure 6.4 below. This partial ranking, however, has to stay lower
than *DISTANCE (i-R), since the gestural distance between [i] and a retroflex is
unquestionably larger than that between [e] and a retroflex.
*DIST (i-R)
retraction of /i/ to []
*DIST (e-R)
*DIST (-R)
retraction of /e/ to []
*DIST (-R)
*DIST (-R)/*DIST (u-R)
*DIST (-R)
*DIST (æ-R)
*DIST (a-R)
*DIST (o-R)
Figure 6.4
Ranking of the *DISTANCE (back) constraints and vowel height faith constraints
(indicated by the dashed lines) for vowel lowering in Ponapean.
In figure 6.4, two rankings for the vowel faith constraints are possible: either
*R EPLACE (+4F2, –2F2), which militates against the change from [i] to [], is
ranked below *DISTANCE (i-R), i.e., at the height of the first dashed line, or below
*DISTANCE (-R) and *DISTANCE (e-R), i.e., at the height of the second dashed line.
The vowel faith constraint *REPLACE (+4F2, +1F2), against a change from [e] to []
has to be ranked below the second dashed line. If it were ranked higher, no
retraction of [e] would take place.
Lowering. Vowel lowering and the different rankings necessary to account for it are
depicted in figure 6.5 on the next page. Here, the F2 faith constraints are assumed to
be ranked high (no vowel retraction occurs) and all the vowel faith constraints are at
the same height (at that of the dashed lines).
For the process of vowel lowering, Kayardild will be discussed as a
representative language. In this language both high front /i/ and high back /u/ are
lowered. The respective rankings are shown in figure 6.6, also on the next page.
Analysis of retroflex processes
185
no lowering
*DIST (i-R)
lowering of /i/ to [e]
*DIST (e-R)
lowering of /e/ to [] or []
*DIST (-R) *DIST (-R)
lowering of // to [æ]
*DIST (æ-R)
lowering of /æ/ to [a]
*DIST (a-R)
no front vowel in R context
*DIST (-R)
*DIST (-R)/*DIST(u-R)
lowering of // or /u/ to [o]
*DIST (o-R)
no back vowel in R context
Figure 6.5
Ranking of the *DISTANCE (back) constraints and vowel height faith constraints
(indicated by the dashed lines) for vowel lowering.
*DIST (i-R)
lowering of /i/ to [e]
*DIST (e-R)
*DIST (-R)
*DIST(u-R)
lowering of /u/ to [o]
*DIST (-R) *DIST (-R)
*DIST (æ-R)
*DIST (a-R)
*DIST (o-R)
Figure 6.6
Ranking of the *DISTANCE (back) constraints and vowel height faith constraints
(indicated by the dashed lines) for vowel lowering in Kayardild.
The two lowering processes in Kayardild are given in (40) (in the following tableaux
the candidate with a faithful vowel but a changed retroflex and the respective
constraint are not included anymore). The vowel faith constraint *REPLACE (–3F1,
–1F1), which militates against a change from /i/ to [e] and from /u/ to [o], has to be
lower ranked than the *DISTANCE (e-R) and *DISTANCE (u-R) constraints, otherwise
no vowel lowering would take place. The vowel faith constraint *REPLACE (–3F1,
+1F1) has to be high ranked to avoid a further lowering of /i/ to [], see the third
candidate [].
186
(40)
Chapter 6
/i/
[i]
*DIST
(i-R)
*!
*REPLACE
(–3F1, +1F1)
*DIST
(u-R)
*
[e]
*DIST
(i-R)
*REPLACE
(–3F1, –1F1)
*DIST
(o-R)
*
*
*!
[]
/u/
*DIST
(e-R)
*
*REPLACE
(–3F1, +1F1)
*DIST
(e-R)
[u]
*DIST
(u-R)
*!
[o]
*REPLACE
(–3F1, –1F1)
*DIST
(o-R)
*
*
*
The vowel lowering process in Norwegian is phonetically unusual, as it
involves lowering of the mid front vowel // only (recall the data from (25) in
chapter 4), see (41).
(41)
//
[]
*REPLACE
*DISTANCE
(+1F1, +3F1)
(-R)
*!
*
[æ]
[a]
*REPLACE *DISTANCE
(+1F1, +2F1)
(æ-R)
*
*
*!
The high front vowel /i/ in Norwegian does not change, although we would expect it
from our universal phonetic constraint ranking in figure 6.2 and 6.4 to undergo
lowering, since the *DISTANCE (i-R) constraint is higher ranked than the *DISTANCE
(-R) constraint. This exception has to be treated as language-specific and
phonetically unmotivated, formalized by a high faith constraint for the vowel /i/, see
(42).
(42)
/i/
*REPLACE
(–3F1, –1F1)
[i]
[e]
*!
*DISTANCE
(i-R)
*
*DISTANCE
(e-R)
*
*
Therefore Norwegian is assumed to show the following interaction of *DISTANCE
constraints and perceptual faith constraints (*REPLACE) with respect to the process
of vowel lowering, where the constraint *REPLACE (–3F1, –1F1) militates against
the lowering of /i/ (very high ranked), and the constraint *REPLACE (+1F1, +2F1)
against the lowering of //.
(43) *REPLACE (–3F1, –1F1) >> *DISTANCE (i-R) >> *DISTANCE (-R) >>
*REPLACE (+1F1, +2F1) >> *DISTANCE (æ-R)
Analysis of retroflex processes
187
6.3.3.3 Vowel diphthongization
Vowel diphthongization differs from the processes of vowel lowering and retraction,
in that it involves a change of part of the vowel only. During the articulation of the
vowel the tongue retracts, lowers, or assumes a neutral position, before moving on to
the retroflex articulation. This near-neutral position results in a schwa-like offglide,
and thus a diphthongization of the vowel. The present analysis is not concerned with
the exact phonetic realization of the second half of the vowel. It is represented as a
schwa, but if a retracted front high vowel or a front mid vowel were assumed, the
formalization would not change. The articulation responsible for such a vowel
change can be modelled with the *SYNC (vowel | retroflex) constraint defined in
(12): the tongue middle lowering is not synchronized with the tongue tip raising and
closure, and thus occurs already when the tongue tip is not raised yet. The reverse
process, a raising of the tongue tip far before a tongue tip closure is made, occurs in
the process of vowel retroflexion, discussed in section 6.3.4 below. Vowel
retroflexion is also accounted for by *SYNC (vowel | retroflex).
Diphthongization of /i/ in Gugada is taken here as an example to show the
relevant constraint interaction, see tableau (44).
(44)
/i/
*REPLACE
(+4F2, +1F2)
*SYNC
(vowel | retroflex)
*!
*!
*
[i]
[]
[i]
*INSERT
(0F1 & 0F2)
*
The winning candidate does not violate any vowel faith constraint, for instance
*REPLACE (+4F2, +1F2) if vowel retraction is assumed, because the vowel is, at
least partly, still perceivable. [i] does, however, violate a *INSERT (0F1 & 0F2)
constraint since a new segment feature (the schwa) emerges that was not specified in
the underlying representation.
6.3.3.4 Vowel rounding
The vowel rounding processes in Wembawemba and Wergaia are assumed here to
occur only together with de-retroflexion, following Flemming’s (1995, 2002)
proposal discussed in sections 4.3.3 and 5.3.1. Vowel rounding can thus be
interpreted as a strategy to preserve the underlying retroflex specification, i.e.
[–2F3], as rounding causes a similar cue of lowered F3, by realizing it on the vowel.
An example illustrating vowel rounding is modelled in tableau (45).4
4
The possible candidate [itw] that was introduced in the discussion on vowel rounding by Flemming in
section 5.3.2 is not included in this tableau. In order not to let this candidate win, information on the
low perceptibility of the feature [–2F3] on a stop, compared to its high perceptibility if it is realized on
a vowel, has to be included. This can be done by incorporating the probability of the features to be
properly perceived, i.e. categorized, see Boersma (1998: 285f.).
188
(45)
Chapter 6
/i/
[i]
*DISTANCE *DELETE
(–2F3)
(i-R)
*REPLACE *DELETEPATH *GESTURE
(lips)
(+4F2, +2F2) (stop × –2F3)
*!
*
*!
[it]
*
[yt]
*
*
The most faithful candidate is articulatorily too complex and violates the highranked *DISTANCE (i-R) constraint. The second candidate is articulatory less
complex, but does not realize the underlying [–2F3] specification. The winning
candidate preserves this feature but violates the faith constraints *REPLACE (+4F2,
+2F2) and *DELETEPATH (stop × –2F3), the latter militating against the movement
of the [–2F3] feature from the consonant to the vowel (recall the definition of
*DELETEPATH in (5)). Furthermore, the winner violates the markedness constraint
*GESTURE (lips), because it includes additional rounding of the lips. In
Wembawemba and Wergaia, these three constraints have to be lower-ranked than
the constraint preserving the [–2F3] feature value and the constraint against a
gestural movement from front high vowel to retroflex. This ranking is unexpected
since there is no observable optimisation, neither articulatory nor perceptual.
Perceptually, the faithfulness to both underlying segment classes (low ranked
*REPLACE (+4F2, +2F2) and *DELETEPATH (stop × –2F3)) is less important than the
preservation of one single cue (high ranked *DELETE (–2F3)). Articulatorily, one
gesture is saved (expressed by the high ranked *DISTANCE (i-R)) at the cost of
having an additional gesture (the low ranked *GESTURE (lips)). This unusual ranking
might explain why such a process is cross-linguistically extremely rare.
In sections 4.3.2.1 and 4.3.3, Kod5agu was shown to retract vowels before
retroflexes, and retract and round vowels between a labial and a retroflex. The
example word [po ] is analysed in (46), assuming /pe / as the non-rounded and
non-retracted input to the diachronic development in Kod5agu. A *SYNC constraint
that militates against the synchronization of the end of labialization and the
beginning of the vowel, termed *SYNC (labial | vowel), can account for the rounding
of the following vowel.
(46)
/pe /
*DISTANCE
(e-R)
[pe ]
*!
*SYNC
*REPLACE
(labial | vowel) (+4F2, –4F2)
*
*
[po ]
[pø ]
[p" ]
*REPLACE
(+4F2, –2F2)
*
*!
*!
*
Both the first, most faithful candidate and the third candidate, which shows no
retraction but only vowel rounding, violate the *DISTANCE (e-R), because they do
not reduce the distance between the vowel and the retroflex gesture. The fourth
candidate, without rounding, reduces the gestural distance, but violates the *SYNC
Analysis of retroflex processes
189
(labial | vowel) constraint, which is higher ranked than the vowel faith constraints.
Thus, the rounded, retracted vowel wins.
6.3.3.5 Secondary palatalization
For secondary palatalization of retroflexes, the account looks slightly different from
those for the vowel – retroflex sequences with respect to possible outputs. Since the
articulation of a palatalized retroflex is assumed here to be impossible (as illustrated
in section 2.5), there is no possible output candidate of this type. Instead, the three
realizations as given in (30) and repeated here in (47) are observable in natural
languages. Let us discuss them subsequently.
(47) (a)
(b)
(c)
input
/j/
/j/
/j/
output
[tj]
[]
[j]
process
de-retroflexion
de-palatalization
separate palatal
The first realization of retroflex palatalization is a non-retroflex, palatalized
segment. This process can be found in Russian for the fricative //, which palatalizes
to the segment [#] (the palato-alveolar fricative shows some inherent palatalization
due to its domed tongue body, and is thus not represented with additional secondary
palatalization). Palatalization of coronals is realized as a raising of the second and
third formant, and will be represented here by the feature [+2F3]. The constraint
*DELETE (+2F3) thus militates against the deletion of the palatalization cues.
(48)
/+ /
[]
*DELETE
(+2F3)
*!
*REPLACE
(–2F3, +2F3)
*
[#]
Since the non-realization of palatalization cues is not an option in the symmetric
system of Russian consonants, which are either palatalized or non-palatalized, the
constraint *DELETE (+2F3) is very highly ranked and prohibits the non-palatalized
first candidate. Instead, a change in the inherent cues of the fricative from retroflex
to palato-alveolar is acceptable. Thus, the second candidate wins.
The reverse ranking of the two faith constraints results in the realization of the
retroflex as non-palatalized, the second output occurring for the process of
palatalizing retroflexes, as depicted in (47b). This strategy can be found in Lewis
Scots Gaelic, illustrated in section 4.3.2.5. The following tableau accounts for the
underlying sequence /+ / in the Lewis dialect:
(49)
/+ /
*REPLACE
(–2F3, +2F3)
[]
[t ]
*DELETE
(+2F3)
*
*!
A problem concerning this realization strategy is the fact that the resulting form is
190
Chapter 6
ambiguous, since the hearer is not provided with any perceptual cues as to whether
the segment is palatalized or not. The Lewis form [pa ] ‘a poet’, for example, can be
both nominative singular (without underlying palatalization) and genitive singular
(with underlying but not realized palatalization for the genitive). Additional
contextual information is needed to disambiguate the two forms.
The last possibility of realizing palatalization on a retroflex segment is doing
so not simultaneously but instead with a following palatal glide (45c). It was
claimed in 2.5.2 that this is the strategy applied in Toda. Toda palatalization of
retroflexes can therefore be modelled as follows, where *DELETEPATH (stop ×
+2F3) militates against a change in the overlap of palatalization and stop cues (see
definition of *DELETEPATH constraints in (5)).
(50)
/+ /
*REPLACE
(–2F3, +2F3)
[]
[j]
[t ]
*DELETE
(+2F3)
*!
*DELETEPATH
(stop × +2F3)
*
*!
As explained in section 2.5.2, Toda is assumed to have a phonological category of a
palatalized retroflex, though articulatory this is impossible. Some special restriction
must be therefore assumed in the perception grammar to prevent the listener from
categorizing the articulatory output [j]art as two consecutive segments, analysing it
as one complex segment [ ] instead. In a perception grammar, so-called *WARP
constraints (Boersma 1998: 164) require that the cues of a segment given in the
input are categorized as such. For our purpose, a *WARP (+2F3) constraint requires
that the input segment [j]art is categorized as such, and the *W ARP (stop | +2F3)
constraint that the input [j]art is perceived as the sequence of the two segments
[j]perc.5 This perception process is modelled in tableau (51).
(51)
[j]art
[j]perc
*/j/
*WARP
(+2F3)
*!
*
[ ]perc
[]perc
*WARP
(stop | +2F3)
*!
The constraint */j/ is a language-specific phonotactic constraint acquired by the
learner via probabilistic knowledge of the occurrence of specific segment sequences
in the language. It prohibits the categorization of the sequence [j]art as [j]perc
because the glide /j/ does not occur in this position in Toda, and it thus prevents the
most faithful first candidate from winning. The third candidate, the one without the
glide, violates the high ranked *W ARP (+2F3) constraint. The [j]art sequence,
5
Boersma’s *WARP constraints are applied only to single vowel segments and used to model the
miscategorization of these vowels as acoustically very close ones. This constraint family is not applied
by Boersma to segmental sequences as done here.
Analysis of retroflex processes
191
therefore, can only be interpreted as a palatalized retroflex.
It must be noted that there is a slight mismatch exemplified in (50) and (51).
The faith constraints in (50) refer to rich perceptual features, whereas the perceptual
results of (51) are more abstract featural representations (in accordance with
Boersma 1998).6
6.3.4 Retroflexion of vowels
Retroflexion of vowels was illustrated in section 4.5.2 to originate from a tongue tip
raising at the beginning of the vowel gesture. This is a different kind of nonsynchronization of vowel and retroflex gestures than described for the
diphthongization in section 6.3.3.3. It is caused, however, by the same drive of
avoiding effortful synchronizations of gestures, captured by the *SYNC (vowel |
retroflex) constraint as defined in (12) above. Vowel retroflexion of /a/ in Margany,
for example, can then be accounted for as follows:
(52)
/a/
[a]
[a$]
*SYNC
(vowel | retroflex)
*!
*INSERTPATH
(–3F3 × +3F1)
*
The non-synchronization of the two gestures results in a change in the vowel quality,
which violates the *INSERTPATH (–3F3 × +3F1) constraint (see the definition of
*INSERTPATH constraints in (4)).
6.3.5 Phonotactics of retroflex segments
Section 4.6 illustrated that retroflex segments often do not occur in those positions
where the retroflex is not well perceivable, namely post-consonantally. This was
accounted for by the weaker CV cues of retroflexes, which are perceptually close to
those of apical alveolars, compared to the more distinct VC cues. Such a proposal
was formalized already by Steriade (1995, 2001a), as discussed in section 5.3.3.
Steriade assumes that these are cases of neutralization of apical contrast. In the
following formalization I assume that phonotactic restrictions of this class are not
explained by reference to its distinctiveness with the presence of other classes in the
same inventory. Instead, it is assumed that the retroflex does not occur in specific
positions in certain languages because its *GESTURE (retroflex) constraint interacts
with a family of *DELETE (–2F3) constraints as defined in (2a) above, which are
sensitive to the context restriction as given in (53).
6
If the grammar model exemplified in (51) is applied to (50) and all earlier tableaux in this chapter, the
formulation of the faith constraints would have to be modified appreciably, since they would have to
refer to abstract features. In (50) the perceptual part of the second candidate would become [j]perc, i.e.,
the winning candidate of (51). This means that the second candidate in (50) would become completely
faithful to the underlying form, and hence would violate no faith constraints at all. A formulation of
the current theory in terms of more abstract features has to be postponed until future research.
192
Chapter 6
(53) *DELETE (–2F3 / V_V) >> *DELETE (–2F3 / V_C) >> *DELETE (–2F3 / C_V)
>> *DELETE (–2F3 / C_C)
This ranking of contexts incorporates the fact that retroflex cues are most prominent
in a V_V context, and least prominent in C_C context, like all consonantal cues. The
ranking of the intermediate two constraints *DELETE (–2F3 / V_C) and *DELETE
(–2F3/ C_V) incorporates that the VC cues of retroflexes are stronger than their CV
cues. The constraint hierarchy in (53) is hence a universal comparison of the
contrastiveness of retroflex cues in specific positions. As was the case for the
universal gesture hierarchies for vowel retraction and vowel lowering in section
6.3.3.2, this hierarchy is phonetically motivated and assumed to be present in the
universal phonetic knowledge. It does not exist in the phonology of every language.
Languages with no retroflex gesture at all could be described as ranking *GESTURE
(retroflex) above the complete hierarchy of (53). But as there is no need to refer to a
non-learned gesture with a specific *GESTURE constraint and, more importantly, to
include information on the possible contexts of occurrence for this non-existing
segment class in the grammar of this language, such a formalization is not proposed
here.
For a language such as Kashmiri which allows retroflex segments in all
positions, the *GESTURE (retroflex) constraint could similarly be assumed to be
ranked below the hierarchy in (53). But again, this language needs no further
information on slight differences in the perceptibility of retroflex cues in specific
positions, since these differences are not relevant for the phonology of this language.
Therefore, the hierarchy in (53) is not assumed to be part of the grammatical
knowledge of Kashmiri speakers. The hierarchy in (53) is only present in the
grammar of a language if there are cue-specific prosodic restrictions. On the other
hand, the Djapu dialect of Dhuwala-Dhuwal, a language with retroflex segments in
postvocalic position only, has the *GESTURE (retroflex) constraint ranked between
the *DELETE (–2F3 / V_C) and *DELETE (–2F3 / C_V) on the hierarchy, and this
ranking is contained in the grammar.
Several languages, such as the Australian language Bunuba, have no retroflex
in initial position. In section 4.6.2 it was proposed that this behaviour is not
phonetically motivated, as it comprises both postvocalic and postconsonantal
positions. This restriction has to be stored in the phonology language-specifically,
namely as ranking of the *GESTURE (retroflex) constraint below faith constraints
securing the retroflex features in non-initial context and above faith constraints for
retroflex cues in general.
These languages do, however, allow a retroflex segment in word-initial
position if the consonant in the following syllable is retroflex. These are cases of
long-distance assimilation to be formalized in the next section, independent of the
general phonotactics.
Lastly, some Australian languages show variation in the actual articulation of
the retroflex (and the alveolar) in initial position, for instance observed by
McGregor (1990) for Gooniyandi. For these cases, Boersma’s *PRECISION
constraints as defined in (13) can be applied to a specific context, see (54).
Analysis of retroflex processes
193
(54) *PRECISION (tongue tip: retroflexed | (ω_): “In word-initial position, the tongue
tip does not work up the precision to put itself in a retroflex position.”
This lack of precision in the articulation of the retroflex segment may result in an
apical alveolar (i.e. a non-displaced apical articulation) in some cases and therefore
account for the variation found in languages such as Gooniyandi.
6.3.6 Assimilation of retroflexion
Assimilation processes are gesture reductions licensed by the weak perceptibility of
the cues that are lost by the assimilation. In our FP-OT framework, apical
assimilation involving retroflexes can be depicted as being triggered by a highranked *DISTANCE (tip) constraint (defined in (10a)), which militates against the
change from one tongue tip posture to another. Furthermore, assimilation violates
faith constraints of the segment changed. As was defined already above in section
6.3.5, the cues for retroflexes are stronger in VC position than in CV position,
leading to a context-sensitive *DELETE (–2F3) constraint ranking for phonotactic
restrictions in (55a), analogously to (53). A similar context hierarchy can be
assumed for the constraint *DELETE (0F3) which militates against the deletion of the
mid F3 cues of apical alveolars, see (55b).
(55) (a)
(b)
*DELETE (–2F3 / V_) >> *DELETE (–2F3 / C_)
*DELETE (0F3 / V_) >> *DELETE (0F3 / C_)
These two constraint rankings cannot be ordered with respect to each other in any
specific way, as there is no motivation why for example the deletion of a retroflex
before a vowel should be perceptually more acceptable than the deletion of an apical
alveolar in the same environment. With these two constraint rankings and their
flexible, language-specific ranking with respect to each other, the assimilation
processes illustrated in section 4.7 can be modelled.
For progressive assimilation of an alveolar to a retroflex the two constraint
families in (55) have interact as follows:
(56) *DELETE (–2F3 / V_) >> *DELETE (0F3 / C_)
The progressive and iterative assimilation in Swedish and Norwegian is illustrated in
the tableau in (57), where just a consonant sequence but not the whole word is
given. The sequence occurs after a vowel.
(57)
/ts/
[ts]
[]
[nts]
*DISTANCE
(tip)
*!
*DELETE
(–2F3 / V_)
*DELETE
(0F3 / C_)
**
*!
194
Chapter 6
Progressive assimilation of a retroflex to an alveolar requires the ranking of
the constraint saving the alveolar cues postvocalically above the constraint saving
the retroflex cues postconsonantally, see (58).
(58) *DELETE (0F3 / V_) >> *DELETE (–2F3 / C_)
Kannada has such a process, illustrated in (59), which again only shows the relevant
consonant clusters and the essential constraints.
(59)
/d&/
*DISTANCE
(tip)
[d&]
*!
*DELETE
(0F3 / V_)
*DELETE
(–2F3 / C_)
*!
[&]
*
[dl]
For regressive assimilation of an alveolar to a retroflex, a ranking of all the
faith constraints for the retroflex (55a) above the faith constraints for the alveolars
(55b) must be assumed, see (60).
(60) *DELETE (–2F3 / V_) >> *DELETE (–2F3 / C_) >> *DELETE (0F3 / V_) >>
*DELETE (0F3 / C_)
An example from Sanskrit is given in (61):
(61)
/t/
*DISTANCE
(tip)
[t]
*!
*DELETE
(–2F3 / C_)
*
[]
[td]
*DELETE
(0F3 / V_)
*!
The ranking in (60) predicts that progressive assimilation of alveolar to retroflex
place of articulation occurs as well, i.e., that it is always the place cue of the
retroflex that wins in Sanskrit. This proposal differs from Steriade’s (1995) analysis
of Sanskrit, which treats regressive assimilation of alveolars to retroflexes as being
morphologically conditioned: spreading occurs from the cues of the content words
to those of the functional words. Whitney (1889, §196) illustrates that retroflexes in
Sanskrit can influence adjacent dentals, but does not describe the reverse process nor
gives examples for it, thereby supporting the present analysis of Sanskrit.
Lastly, the process of regressive assimilation of a retroflex to an alveolar has
to be discussed. This involves the following ranking of the cue constraints:
(62) *DELETE (0F3 / V_) >> *DELETE (0F3 / C_) >> *DELETE (–2F3 / V_) >>
*DELETE (–2F3 / C_)
This process of regressive alveolar assimilation occurs in Punjabi, see (63).
Analysis of retroflex processes
(63)
/n/
*DISTANCE
(tip)
[n]
*!
*DELETE
(0F3 / C_)
195
*DELETE
(–2F3 / V_)
*!
[]
[nn]
*
Section 4.7.1 also included the assimilation of a retroflex to a non-coronal
velar or palatal with the example of Colloquial Tamil. For such a process the
*DI S T A N C E (back) constraint (as defined under 10b) that accounts for the
unwillingness of a change in articulator from retracted tongue back for the retroflex
to the raised tongue back for the velar has to be ranked above the coronal faith
constraint *DELETE (+1F2), see (64) (the velar feature [−2F2] was introduced in
table 5.10).
(64)
/k/
*DISTANCE
(back)
[k]
*!
*DELETE
(−2F2)
*DELETE
(+1F2)
*!
[]
*
[kk]
From the data at hand, we cannot determine whether the *DELETE constraints have
to be formulated as context-sensitive or not, i.e. whether // is always assimilated
independent of the context.
The examples of long-distance assimilation in section 4.7.2 were restricted to
assimilation of an alveolar towards a retroflex, for the obvious reason that noncoronal segments are not affected by a distant retroflex. It was assumed that the
retroflex gesture is held during the interval between the two surface retroflex
segments. In contrast to local assimilation, it is not an adjacent gesture that is
changed in this type of assimilation, and hence the direct context of the two
segments involved in the process does not matter. It is a ranking between *REPLACE
(0F3, –2F3) and *REPLACE (–2F3, 0F3) that simply decides on the winning cues and
hence on the direction of assimilation.
The Sanskrit nati rule exemplifies progressive long-distance assimilation.
Only the alveolar nasal can undergo it, and the output violates the manner-specific
constraint *REPLACE (0F3, –2F3 / nasal), which militates against the replacement of
an alveolar by a retroflex nasal. Tableau (65) illustrates the process. In the following
examples, dots are used to indicate intervening, non-coronal material.
(65)
/...n/
*REPLACE
(–2F3, 0F3)
[...n]
[s...n]
[...]
*DISTANCE
(tip)
*!
*REPLACE
(0F3, –2F3 / nasal)
*!
*
196
Chapter 6
The *DISTANCE (tip) constraint is assumed to be sensitive to the change in place of
articulation, only, but is not violated by a change from apical fricative to stop which
also involves a slight change of tongue tip position.
Long-distance assimilation in Sanskrit is blocked by non-nasal coronals
between the trigger retroflex and the nasal, see (66).
(66)
/.t.n/
[.t.n]
*REPLACE
*REPLACE
*DISTANCE
(–2F3, 0F3) (0F3, –2F3 / stop)
(tip)
*
[s.t.n]
[.t.]
[..n]
[..]
*REPLACE
(0F3, –2F3 / nasal)
*!
**!
*!
*!
*
*
*
The third candidate, in which /n/ is retroflexed after a retroflex despite the
intervening alveolar, violates the *DISTANCE (tip) constraint twice because of the
changes from retroflex to alveolar and from alveolar to retroflex articulation. The
fourth candidate, a retroflexion of the plosive instead of the nasal, shows that it is
necessary to distinguish the *REPLACE constraints according to the manner of the
segments: stops with their stronger cues allow less changes than nasals, therefore
*REPLACE (0F3, –2F3 / stop) is higher ranked than *REPLACE (0F3, –2F3 / nasal)
(see Boersma’s 1998 argumentation and Steriade’s 2001 proposal). The last
candidate, where both underlying apicals are retroflexed, violates both mannerspecific *REPLACE constraints.
6.4 Conclusion
In the present chapter it was shown that processes involving retroflex segments
could be modelled by assuming underlying perceptual representations, with
additional articulatory restrictions on their realization. Underlying articulatory
features are absent in my account. Furthermore the present chapter showed that is
not necessary to stipulate additional devices such as a P-map (Steriade 2001a, b) that
stores information on perceptual difference since the constraints in the grammar
refer directly to cues and cue combinations. For the class of retroflexes, the low F3,
i.e. the feature [–2F3], plays a large role in quite a number of processes. At the same
time, the retraction of the tongue tip concomitant with retroflex articulation imposes
restrictions on the realization of specific sequences and accounts for phonological
processes such as front vowel lowering or retraction in a retroflex context or a
change of the retroflex in secondary palatalization.
The analyses of the phonological processes were illustrated with mainly only
one example language each from the respective sections in chapter 4. Examples
from further languages given in chapter 4 were not discussed here in order not to
Analysis of retroflex processes
197
obscure the analysis. Detailed accounts for these non-discussed languages,
especially of languages that involve more than one process, are left for future work.
The analyses presented here included observations on cross-linguistic
variations and descriptions of these variations with universal constraint hierarchies
that interact with language-specific constraints. Two such cases were discussed, the
process of vowel retraction and lowering (6.3.3.2), where universally ranked
articulatory markedness constraints interact with perceptual faith constraints, and
assimilation processes (6.3.6), where universally ranked faith constraints interacted
with markedness constraints that are language-specifically ranked. As explained
before, these rankings are grounded in the phonetic equipment of humans and are in
this sense universal: either the constraint of a more complex articulation is higher
ranked than one militating against a less complex articulation, or the constraint
against a cue in a perceptually less salient context is higher ranked than the
constraint against the same cue in a perceptually more salient context. These
universal constraint hierarchies are assumed to be part of the phonetic knowledge
available to a speaker, and not to belong to a separate innate Universal Grammar.
Furthermore, they are not present in the phonology of every single language, but
only in those that actually refer to these phonetic details in their phonological
processes. The process of vowel lowering in Norwegian (see tableaux (41) and (42))
illustrated that a language can partly follow such universal hierarchies (the lowering
of the vowel []) but show idiosyncratic behaviour for other parts due to high-ranked
faith constraints (as in the case of non-lowering of the vowel [i]).
In sum, the present chapter addressed and covered large parts of the discussion
of all previous chapters of this dissertation: it dealt with the phonological processes
involving retroflexes as illustrated in chapter 4 in the framework of FP introduced in
chapter 5, thus summarized and brought to a conclusion the phonological part of this
dissertation. Moreover, by specifying the class of retroflexes by perceptual features
[–2F3] and [+1F2], based on the perceptual cues that were the topic of chapter 3,
and by restricting their coarticulations by their articulatory characteristics, as
described in chapter 2, it incorporated the phonetic definition of retroflexes given in
the first part of the thesis.
7
Conclusion
The present dissertation has made two main proposals for the segmental class of
retroflexes and for the interplay of phonetic cues and phonological features by
looking in detail at the phonetic realizations and phonological behaviour of
retroflexes.
Firstly, I defined the phonetic space from which languages choose their
retroflex category by proposing four articulatory properties and two perceptual cues.
The articulatory and perceptual properties, their restrictions, and possible
improvements are discussed in section 7.1 below.
Secondly, I argued for a phonological representation of the class of retroflexes
and the processes it undergoes that departs in two ways from traditional
phonological approaches: it assumes an underlying perceptual representation, and it
rejects the notion of universal phonological features. This point will be summarized
in section 7.2, together with a model of the tongue parts that incorporates
interdependencies between the single articulators.
The claims made above concerning the phonological representations imply that
I assume there is no universal class of retroflexes. How this idea can be compatible
with my search for general characteristics of retroflexion is the topic of section 7.3.
Section 7.4 gives an outlook on possible future research.
7.1
Phonetic characteristics of retroflexes
The class of retroflex segments is a heterogeneous class that shows large articulatory
variation, as illustrated in chapter 2. Quite a number of the variants existing in
natural languages do not show any bending backwards of the tongue tip, although
this criterion is often used to define retroflexion. Simonsen et al. (2000), for
example, argue that the apical post-alveolar segments in Norwegian are not retroflex
because they are not articulated with a tongue tip bent backwards. Lee (1999)
reasons similarly for the Mandarin post-alveolar fricatives. It was shown, however,
that the segmental classes in these two languages share a number of articulatory
characteristics with segments in other languages that are clearly retroflex, and that
they, furthermore, show a similar phonological behaviour. This led to the
assumption that a less restrictive definition for the articulatory class of retroflexes is
needed. Further evidence for this need for a more flexible definition comes from
phonetic variation within languages. It was shown that for a single language the
retroflex class can vary between speakers and/or between contexts from an
articulation with the underside of the tongue tip approaching or having contact with
the post-alveolar or palatal region, i.e., the gesture that is referred to as tongue tip
bending backwards, to an apical alveolar articulation.
200
Chapter 7
Due to this large articulatory variation, I proposed an articulatory definition of
retroflexion in chapter 2 using the following four criteria:
(1) (a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
apicality
posteriority
sublingual cavity
retraction
Posteriority, (1b), was shown in section 2.4 to be missing in some types of retroflex
fricatives. Whereas the properties of apicality, posteriority, and sublingual cavity are
fairly well established and have been mentioned before extensively in the phonetic
literature (although in the case of apicality with a slightly more restricted definition),
the proposal that retraction is present in all realizations of retroflexion is new, see
the discussion in section 2.3. Since such a hypothesis can only be refuted on the
basis of negative evidence, which I have not been able to find up to now, it is open
for future research to show whether this property really holds for all languages or
whether some language exists that has a non-retracted retroflex and thus provides
counter-evidence.
The impossibility of proving that all retroflexes are retracted is a point of
criticism to the present dissertation. The proposed four properties can be criticized
on two more grounds. Firstly, the properties were generalized across all manners of
articulation, although their application already showed that there are manner-specific
differences: whereas retroflex stops are always posterior, retroflex fricatives very
often lack this property. As a consequence, one could either assume different
properties according to the manner of articulation, or that the proposed properties do
not hold for all manners, i.e., stops have to be posterior, whereas fricatives do not.
This idea could not be further developed here (for instance with respect to other
manners such as nasals or rhotics) for lack of time and articulatory data.
The second point of criticism of the four properties is the apparent
inconsistency in including implied properties. In section 2.3.4 I proposed that the
property ‘retracted tongue back’ implies a flat, i.e., lowered, tongue middle, see (2a),
and vice versa. The flat tongue middle can be proposed as a separate property, but I
argue in the same section that this is redundant since a retracted tongue back cannot
be achieved without a lowering (i.e., flattening) of the tongue middle. At the same
time, I elaborate in section 2.3.5 that ‘posteriority’ implies a ‘sublingual cavity’, see
(2b) (repeated from (8a) in chapter 2), but still propose that the implied
characteristic of sublingual cavity is a separate property.
(2) (a)
(b)
retraction
posteriority
→
→
flatness
sublingual cavity
This inconsistency can be remedied either by postulating that flatness is a separate
property of retroflexion, or by leaving out both flatness and sublingual cavity as
separate properties since they are implied by others. I reject the latter possibility
since this runs into problems if one assumes a manner-specific absence/presence of
properties. As was indicated above, fricatives do not have to fulfil the property of
Conclusion
201
posteriority. If sublingual cavity is taken only as an implicational property of
posteriority, then this would mean that fricatives have to satisfy neither posteriority
nor sublingual cavity; a result that is clearly not advantageous, given that even the
implications might hold for specific manners only.
In section 2.3.2 the gesture of flapping out was described as a “possible
concomitant of” posteriority and apicality, which could be interpreted as a further
implication, see (3).
(3) posteriority & apicality →
flapping out
This implication is, however, a simplification since flapping out depends very much
on the manner of articulation: a quick movement of the tongue tip as required for
stop releases and for flaps enables a flapping out, whereas more static positions of
the tongue tip as necessary in fricatives and laterals often prohibit it. Therefore
flapping out could be treated as an additional property of retroflexes.
An alternative way of defining retroflexes by articulatory properties that takes
into account the points of criticism is presented in table 7.1.
Table 7.1 Alternative to the defining properties used in the present dissertation with 6
properties instead of 4 and a manner-specific application. A plus indicates that
the specific manner has to have this property in order to be considered retroflex, a
minus that it does not, and a question mark indicates that this cannot be decided
on without further research.
property
apicality
posteriority
sublingual cavity
retraction
flatness
flapping out
stops
fricatives
nasals etc.
+
+
+
?
?
+
+
–
+
+
+
–
+
?
+
?
?
?
The system in table 7.1 is a suggestion for a possible improvement of the four
articulatory characteristics proposed and used in the present dissertation. Further
research is necessary to provide the details that are still missing in this system and to
show whether this proposal is indeed superior to the system used in the chapters
above.
Besides the four articulatory properties, two perceptual characteristics for the
class of retroflexes were proposed in chapter 3, namely a mid or high F2, which
defines them as being coronal, and, more importantly, a low F3, which distinguishes
them from all other coronals. These cues have only relative values, thus a mid or
high F2 of coronals contrasts with a low F2 of labials and velars, and a low F3 of
retroflexes contrasts with a high F3 of non-retroflex coronals. Exact formant values
for specific languages were ignored since the aim was, just as in the case of the
articulatory properties, to find general properties that hold cross-linguistically. For
202
Chapter 7
language-specific descriptions, however, precise values have to be established, see
section 7.3 below.
7.2
Phonological representations of retroflex processes
Retroflexes can umdergo a number of phonological processes that are grounded in
the articulatory and acoustic characteristics of this class as defined above in 7.1.
These processes, discussed in chapter 4, are repeated here in (4).
(4)
(a)
(b)
(c)
(d)
(e)
(f)
input
/rt/
/ut/
/i/
/ /
/V/
//
/t/
/Vt/
output
[]
[u]
[it] or [ ]
[t ] or []
[V ]
(ω[t], C[t]
[]
[V]
process
retroflexion in rhotic context
retroflexion in back vowel context
deretroflexion or vowel retraction
deretroflexion or depalatalization
retroflexion of adjacent vowel
phonotactic restrictions on retroflexes
local assimilation of retroflexes
non-local assimilation of retroflexes
Chapter 4 illustrated that deretroflexion and depalatalization, see (4c), and
retroflexion of adjacent vowels, see (4d), have an exclusively articulatory
motivation, by saving a gesture or avoiding the costful synchronizing of two
gestures. The larger number of these processes, however, makes a perceptual
licensing of such gestural changes necessary, i.e. the output has to be perceptually
very close to the input or has to be less distinguishable from the input in the context
of occurrence. This is the case for retroflexion in rhotic context, see (4a), for
retroflexion in back vowel context (4b), and for phonotactic restrictions and
assimilations of retroflexes, see (4e) and (4f), respectively.
A phonological representation of the processes in (4) therefore has to take their
phonetic groundings into account. It was illustrated in chapter 5 that traditional
featural approaches do not meet these requirements for several reasons. It was
argued that these approaches
(i)
(ii)
(iii)
are (nearly exclusively) based on articulation and thus cannot account
for perceptual motivations,
oversimplify the vocal tract anatomy and the interaction between the
articulators, and therefore provide insufficient means to model the
interrelations between the tongue parts,
assume universal phonological features, which are devices to describe
cross-linguistic observations, but which are incapable of truly
accounting for every phonological process in every language.
Instead of a phonological representation based on articulation only, the account for
retroflex processes given in chapter 6 assumes underlying perceptual
representations, see Boersma (1998, 2001), from which articulations are derived.
This avoids the problem stated under (i) since the proposed approach can account
Conclusion
203
for the perceptual and articulatory motivations of retroflex processes and especially
for changes from one articulation to a totally independent one by false association
errors such as in the process of retroflexing an apical alveolar after a velar rhotic.
The second point of criticism of previous approaches, i.e., the oversimplification of the vocal tract, cf. (ii), was shown to be particularly problematic for
an account of the incompatibility of retroflexes with palatalization and front vowel
gestures and the resulting deretroflexion, changes of vowel, or palatalization, cf. (4c)
above. I proposed a model of four inter-related tongue parts, repeated here in figure
7.1, and in sections 5.4.2 and 6.2 I elaborated the interactions of these tongue parts
and how they can account for the incompatibility of retroflexes and high front
vowels or glides.
Figure 7.1 A schematic illustration of tongue gestures co-occurring with tongue tip
retraction. Underlyingly grey are the tongue parts at rest, from left to right: tip,
blade, middle (or pre-dorsum), and back (or post-dorsum) (repeated from figure
2.10).
The model in figure 7.1 is not restricted to retroflex-specific articulatory
interrelations. As indicated in 6.4.2, the dependence of tongue blade movements on
the movements of the tongue middle can account for complex articulations that can
be found in alveolo-palatal or palatal segments, which were problematic for FG
accounts with their strict division of tongue blade (CORONAL node) and tongue
dorsum (DORSAL node).
With respect to point (iii) of my criticism of traditional approaches, i.e., the
universality of phonological features, it was proposed in the present dissertation that
there is no need in phonological representations to distinguish between (phonetic)
cues and universal phonological features. Instead, the cues that are relevant for
phonological processes can be used for the modelling of these processes, thereby
making the assumption of an additional, universal feature set redundant. It was
argued in chapter 5 that the phonetic basis of features (cues and gestures) renders
such a “search for universals” superfluous: What is universal is everything that is
based in the articulatory and auditory system of the human, and everything that is
based on the capacity of the human to build classes and to generalize.
204
Chapter 7
In terms of the model of language-specific phonologies as represented in
figure 1.1 in chapter 1, the use of phonetic cues for language-specific phonological
processes, and the abstraction away from these to general features for crosslinguistic descriptions can be depicted as in figure 7.2.
Phonetics, “universal”
Phonological descriptive device
for language comparisons
Phonology of a specific language
language-specific
arbitrary processes
language-specific phonetic
details relevant for phonology
language-specific abstraction from
phonetic detail, phonological features
Figure 7.2 Model of phonological knowledge (black box in the middle) as in figure 1.1, plus
arrows indicating the direction of influence and abstraction.
The arrows on the left of figure 7.2 indicate the language-specific choice of cues
(relevant for the description of the phonological classes and their processes) from
the general phonetic space. The arrows on the right illustrate the use of such
phonetic cues to explain cross-linguistic variation. The description of crosslinguistic variation is possible by cues that are the intersection of all languagespecific features necessary to describe the investigated phonological processes of
these languages. This intersection and thus output of generalization differs,
depending largely on the phenomena and even more on the languages under
investigation. In chapter 6, such general features that abstracted away from
language-specific details were used in the form of the unspecified feature [low F3]
to describe retroflexes in all languages I looked at. This allowed a cross-linguistic
comparison of the processes found and their grounding in the phonetic details.
7.3
Alleged universality of the retroflex class
A question that emerges from the present dissertation and especially from this
chapter is why it is necessary to try to find a general, suspiciously universal-looking
definition of the class of retroflexes (section 7.1) if at the same time it is stated that
universal phonological features and resulting classes do not exist and are
furthermore not necessary (section 7.2). This ostensible contradiction can be
explained in the following way. The definition of retroflexion proposed and applied
Conclusion
205
in the present dissertation is not based on phonological features, but instead on
specific phonetic properties, see section 7.1 above. These properties thus delimit the
boundaries of the retroflex class as defined in the present dissertation. Furthermore,
exactly these defining properties were shown to account for the similar phonological
behaviour of retroflexes across different languages, see chapter 4. The ‘universality’
of the retroflex class is therefore grounded in its phonetic defining properties.
It is possible to define the retroflex class differently from what has been
proposed here, and thus to shift the boundaries of this class. The definition of
retroflexes by a bent-backwards tongue tip, for instance, restricts the term retroflex
to subapical post-alveolars or palatals, and refers only to a small subgroup of the
class I defined as retroflex. Another possibility is to exclude the non-posterior
fricative cases such as the Polish post-alveolar (see section 2.4) from the class of
retroflexion by defining it as apical or subapical post-alveolar or palatal (see the
definition by Catford 1977). Again, this is a feasible approach.
A graphic demonstration of the class boundaries of retroflexion according to
these three definitions is given in figure 7.3 on the next page (restricted to coronal
sounds and exemplified by fricatives since they yield the largest coronal inventory).
This is not meant to be a scale or ordering on any dimension, neither articulatory nor
acoustic.
The categories and their boundaries in (a) of figure 7.3 are according to my
definition of retroflexion, those in (b) according to definitions which do not include
the non-posterior retroflexes (to be found in textbook definitions of retroflexion such
as that in Catford 1977), and those in (c) are according to e.g. Ladefoged &
Maddieson (1996), who distinguish between an apical and a subapical post-alveolar
articulation, only the latter referred to as retroflex.
(a)
s
dental
s
alveolar
s
dental
s
alveolar
retroflex
palatoalveolar
alveolopalatal
palatoalveolar
alveolopalatal
(b)
retroflex
(c)
s
s
dental
alveolar
s
·
apical
retropost-alv. flex
palatoalveolar
alveolopalatal
Figure 7.3 Three definitions of retroflex and the resulting category boundaries with the
example of fricative inventories.
206
Chapter 7
As can be seen in figure 7.3, the definition applied in the present dissertation
(a) yields the largest class of retroflexes, and also includes “marginal” cases of
retroflexion such as the non-posterior retroflexes in Polish, Russian, and Mandarin.
This broad definition was chosen because it was the aim to describe the space within
which languages set their retroflex class (recall figure 1.2 which graphically
demonstrated this). Furthermore, this definition was taken because it left the
opportunity to test whether even non-prototypical retroflexes share properties and
some phonological behaviour with more prototypical ones, thus whether the
hypothetic space was correctly delineated, or whether it was too broad. It turned out
that the borderlines of the class of retroflexion were correctly chosen since even the
borderline cases such as the Polish retroflex were shown to share properties with
more prototypical segments of this class (see chapter 4).
The definition of retroflexion as applied here, however, does not imply that all
languages with retroflexes need such a broad retroflex category. For languagespecific descriptions, the definition of the retroflex category has to be modified
according to the inventory, i.e., according to the other segmental classes present in
the language under investigation, and the cues associated with these classes. A
definition as depicted in (c) in figure 7.3 is for instance useful to describe the
fricative system of Toda, which was shown in sections 2.2.6 and 5.2 to have
phonetically and phonologically two categories that could be considered retroflex
according to my definition. This is is illustrated in figure 7.4.
s
laminal
alveolar
s
·
apical retroflex
post-alv.
Toda
palatoalveolar
Figure 7.4 Coronal fricative system of Toda with two categories that fall within my
definition of retroflex (indicated by the thick black line). A distinction according
to Ladefoged & Maddieson’s division between apical post-alveolar and subapical
post-alveolar/ palatals is possible.
Since Toda systematically distinguishes between the two segment classes that fall
within my definition of retroflexes, it follows that such a definition cannot be used
for this language. Instead, Ladefoged and Maddieson’s distinction of apical postalveolar (i.e., [s]) and subapical post-alveolar or palatal (i.e., []) seems adequate.
The assumption of language-specific definitions of articulatory classes can,
furthermore, account for the fact that languages with small (coronal) inventories
allow larger variation of the retroflex class than those with larger inventories (see
section 2.2.6). Since the former do not make use of large parts of the phonetic space,
this space can be partly occupied by the existing classes, see for instance the
example of Norwegian and the Bzyb dialect of Abkhaz in figure 7.5.
Conclusion
207
(a)
s
dental
retroflex
Norwegian
(b)
s
dental
retroflex
palatoalveolar
alveolopalatal
Bzyb dialect
of Abkhaz
Figure 7.5 Coronal fricative system of Norwegian in (a), with dotted lines indicating the
possible articulatory variation, and in the Bzyb dialect of Abkhaz in (b), which
has an apical post-alveolar retroflex but does not allow much variation because of
its large fricative inventory.
Norwegian has only two coronal fricatives (assuming a variety where the palatoalveolar is totally replaced by the retroflex, recall footnote 41 in chapter 4), the
dental [s] and the retroflex []. This reduced inventory allows large articulatory
variation of the retroflex class, as observed by Simonsen et al. (2001), see the
indicated variation in figure 7.5 (a). The Bzyb dialect of Abkhaz, on the other hand,
has three further coronal fricatives, two of them in the post-alveolar region. This
results in a smaller amount of variation and a less posterior articulation of the
retroflex class than is evident in other languages.
In this section, only the articulatory characteristics of retroflexes could be
shown to vary largely, depending on the language under investigation. A similar
variation is assumed to be found for the perceptual cues of retroflexes. Since hardly
any study is concerned with the exact perceptual cues of retroflexes and coronals in
a particular language, let alone their comparison with what can be found in other
languages, this is still an open task. It is expected, however, that despite large
variation across languages in their exact values, the perceptual cues of the retroflex
class always show a F3 that is far lower than that of any other coronal segmental
class.
7.4
Future research
As became obvious from the previous three sections, there are a number of topics
that could not be covered in the present thesis but which seem to be of interest for
future research.
Concerning the articulatory properties of retroflexes, the more refined model
of properties as given in table 7.1 has to be tested, especially with respect to
additional manners of retroflexion such as nasals, laterals, and rhotics. The
difference between retroflex consonants and retroflex vowels in this respect is
another point worthy of further investigation. Most important for the proposed
208
Chapter 7
articulatory properties, however, is the search for possible evidence against or
further evidence in favour of the property of retraction, in order to either validate or
reject the hypothesis that this property is present in all retroflex segments.
The need for language-specific studies on perceptual cues, i.e. the exact values
of F3 and F2 for a retroflex class in specific languages, and their comparison was
already mentioned above. Such a study could confirm or disconfirm my claim that
these perceptual cues also vary greatly from language to language.
With respect to phonological processes involving retroflexes, the list in (4) is
not complete; section 4.7 already gave some indications of further processes such as
retroflexion of velarized non-retroflexes that seem to be grounded in the phonetics
of this class but which could not be studied here. An investigation of their grounding
and their phonological modelling seems to be interesting.
Lastly, it remains open for future research to model the phonology of a specific
language (and not only with respect to a single segmental class) as closely as
possible to the phonetic cues of the segments in these language. The same has to be
done for other languages, so that a comparison of these modellings can illustrate the
different needs in phonological representations of single languages, and the
inadequacy of universal phonological features.
Samenvatting in het Nederlands
In deze dissertatie staat de groep van klinkers en medeklinkers centraal die met de
tongpunt tegen de regio achter de tandkassen wordt gearticuleerd. Deze klankgroep,
de zogeheten retroflexen, laat een grote articulatorische variatie zien, afhankelijk
van factoren zoals spreker, klinker-context, manier van de articulatie, de familie
waartoe de taal met retroflexen behoort, en ook het aantal andere (coronale)
medeklinkers in de taal.
De parameters van variatie in retroflexen worden geïllustreerd in hoofdstuk
twee van deze dissertatie, waar ik ook de vier eigenschappen introduceer voor de
definitie van deze klankgroep: apicaliteit, posterioriteit, sublinguale holte en
retractie. Niet alle klanken die traditioneel worden beschreven als retroflex laten alle
vier de eigenschappen zien. Er is een groep van post-alveolaire fricatieven, zoals die
in het Mandarijns Chinees, welke niet posterieur zijn. Bovendien behoren sommige
klanken die traditioneel niet als lid van de groep van retroflexen worden gezien,
zoals de post-alveolaire fricatieven in het Pools of Russisch, volgens mijn definitie
wel tot de retroflexen. Maar ook deze groep mist de eigenschap van posterioriteit.
Het is echter alleen het criterium van posterioriteit dat weggelaten kan worden in
een retroflexe klank; alle andere criteria moeten aanwezig zijn.
Naast deze herinterpretatie van de definitie van de retroflexen hebben de vier
geïntroduceerde eigenschappen nog een consequentie: zij impliceren de
onverenigbaarheid van een retroflex met secundaire palatalisering. Ik beargumenteer
dat palatalisering van retroflexen resulteert ofwel in een retroflex zonder
palatalisering, ofwel in een gepalataliseerde laminale palato-alveolair (een klinker
die slechts aan twee criteria voor retroflexie voldoet en dus niet tot de klasse van
retroflexen behoort).
De vier articulatorische eigenschappen van retroflexen die hier geïntroduceerd
zijn hebben niet de status van fonologische kenmerken met binaire waarden. Ze
beschrijven alleen de articulatorische realisatie van de retroflexe klasse. Voor een
fonologische beschrijving introduceer ik kenmerken gebaseerd op de akoestische
eigenschappen van deze groep klanken. In hoofdstuk drie worden deze akoestische
eigenschappen van retroflexen in detail geïllustreerd. Hier wordt duidelijk dat alle
retroflexen een lage derde formant hebben (ook als de exacte hoogte van deze
formant varieert en niet altijd even laag is vergeleken met de hoge derde formant
van palato-alveolaire klanken). Om deze reden gebruik ik deze lage derde formant
als het fonologische kenmerk [-2F3], dat alle retroflexen karakteriseert.
Hoofdstuk drie bespreekt verder de notie van ‘weak cues’, die soms wordt
gebruikt voor retroflexe klanken. Ik beargumenteer dat retroflexen geen zwakkere
akoestische eigenschappen hebben dan alle andere klankgroepen.
In hoofdstuk vier worden fonologische eigenschappen van retroflexen
beschreven die niet afhankelijk zijn van taalfamilie maar die desondanks gevonden
210
Samenvatting in het Nederlands
kunnen worden in veel talen met retroflexen. De fonologische processen zijn
retroflexering in de context van r-klanken en achter-klinkers, de-retroflexering in de
context van voor-klinkers, retroflexering van klinkers, fonotactische beperkingen op
retroflexen, en assimilatieprocessen van retroflexen. In hoofdstuk vier toon ik aan
dat de vier articulatorische eigenschappen die samen met de akoestische eigenschap
de retroflexe klasse definiëren, verantwoordelijk zijn voor dit homogene
fonologische gedrag: de processen zijn gebaseerd op en kunnen worden verklaard
door deze gemeenschappelijke eigenschappen, dat wil zeggen de fonologische
processen zijn gebaseerd (‘grounded’) in hun fonetische kenmerken.
In tegenstelling tot traditionele fonologische theorieën, stel ik voor dat de
groep van retroflexe klanken en de fonologische processen waarin ze betrokken zijn
kunnen worden gerepresenteerd door akoestische kenmerken. Hoofdstuk vijf geeft
een overzicht van traditionele representaties van retroflexen met articulatorische
kenmerken en hun zwakke punten. Het hoofdstuk beschrijft verder nieuwere
benaderingen in de fonologie die akoestische informatie representeren in
fonologische modellen.
Een fonologische analyse van de retroflexe processen uit hoofdstuk vier met
akoestische kenmerken voor de retroflexe klasse wordt gegeven in hoofdstuk zes,
gebaseerd op het ‘functional phonology’ model van Paul Boersma.
Ik kom tot de slotconclusie dat er geen universele klasse van retroflexen is die
met aangeboren fonologische features behoeft te worden beschreven. Desalniettemin
gebruiken veel talen een klasse die aan min of meer de vier articulatorische
eigenschappen van retroflexen voldoet, en die daardoor ook vergelijkbare
fonologische processen vertonen.
Curriculum Vitae
Silke Hamann was born in Lampertheim (Germany) on February 25th, 1971. She
finished high school in Heppenheim in 1990 and studied architecture in Weimar in
1991/1992. Hereafter, she studied English literature, sociology, and general
linguistics in Frankfurt, Manchester, and Berlin, and obtained her Master’s degree in
linguistics and English literature in Berlin in 1998.
From 1999 until 2002 Silke held a position as a PhD researcher at the Utrecht
Institute of Linguistics OTS, and worked from January until September 1999 and
from August 2001 until December 2002 as a scientific researcher at the Centre for
General Linguistics (ZAS) in Berlin. The present dissertation is the result of her
research at both institutes.
In January 2003 Silke started working as a post-doc researcher at the ZAS
Berlin for the DFG project “manner changes in phonology”.
References
References containing the indication ROA are available online at the Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://roa.rutgers.edu/.
Ahmed, Rais & S.S. Agrawal (1969) “Significant Features in the Perception of (Hindi) Consonants.”
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 45,3: 758-763.
Allen, William S. (1951) “Some prosodic aspects of retroflexion and aspiration in Sanskrit.” Bulletin of
the School of Oriental and African Studies 13.4; 939-946.
Allen, William S. (1953) Phonetics in ancient India. London: Oxford University Press.
Allen, William S. (1954) “Retroflexion in Sanskrit: prosodic technique and its relevance to comparative
statement.” Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies 16.3; 556-565.
Allen, William S. (1962) Sandhi: the theoretical, phonetic, and historical bases of word-junction in
Sanksrit. ‘S-Gravenhage: Mouton
Andersen, Henning (1968) “IE *s after i, u, r, k in Baltic and Slavic.” Acta Linguistics Hafniensia 11:
171-190.
Anderson, Stephen R. (1985) Phonology in the Twentieth Century: Theories of Rules and Theories of
Representations. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Anderson, Viktoria B. & Ian Maddieson (1994) “Acoustic characteristics of Tiwi coronal stops.” UCLA
Working Papers in Phonetics 87: 131-162.
Anttila, Arto (1995) Deriving variation from grammar: A study of Finnish genitives. Manuscript,
Stanford University. ROA 63.
Archangeli, Diana (1988) “Aspects of underspecification theory.” Phonology 5: 183-207.
Archangeli, Diana & Douglas Pulleyblank (1994) Grounded Phonology. Cambridge (Mass.): MIT
Press.
Avery, Peter & Keren Rice (1989) “Segment Structure and Coronal Underspecification.” Phonology 6:
179-200.
Bailey, T. Grahame (1937) The pronunciation of Kashmiri. London: Royal Asiatic Society.
Balasubramanian, R. (1972) The Phonetics of Colloquial Tamil. PhD thesis, Edinburgh.
Balasubramanian, T. (1982) “The two r’s and the two n’s in Tamil.” Journal of Phonetics 10: 89-97.
Ball, Martin & Joan Rahilly (1999) Phonetics: The Science of Speech. London: Arnold.
Beck, Janet Mackenzie (1997) “Organic Variation of the Vocal Apparatus.” In: W.J. Hardcastle & J.
Laver (eds) The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences. Oxford: Blackwell; 256-297.
Beckman, Jil (1998) Positional Faithfulness. Dissertation manuscript, University of Massachusetts,
Amherst.
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (1996) “Stress and quantity in Old and early Middle English: evidence for an
optimality-theoretic model of language change.” Manuscript University of Manchester. ROA
136.
Bhat, D.N.S. (1973) “Retroflexion: An Areal Feature.” Working Papers on Language Universals 13:
27-67.
Bhat, D.N.S. (1974a) “Retroflexion and Retraction.” Journal of Phonetics 2: 233-237.
Bhat, D.N.S. (1974b) “A general study of palatalisation.” Working Papers on Language Universals 14;
47-92.
Bhat, Roopkrishen (1987) A descriptive study of Kashmiri. Delhi: Amar Prakashan.
Bhatia, Tej K. (1993) Punjabi: A cognitive descriptive grammar. London: Routledge.
Biedrzycki, Leszek (1974) Abriß der polnischen Phonetik. Warszawa: Wiedza Powszechna.
Blake, Barry J. (1979) “Pitta-Pitta.” In: R.M.W. Dixon & B. Blake (eds.); 183-242.
Blake, Barry J. & John Gavan Breen (1971) The Pitta-Pitta dialects. Melbourne: Monash University.
214
References
Blankenship, Barbara, Peter Ladefoged, Peri Bhaskararao & Nichumeno Chase (1993) “Phonetic
Structures of Khonoma Angami.” UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 84: 127-142.
Boersma, Paul (1998) Functional Phonology: Formalizing the interaction between articulatory and
perceptual drives. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics.
Boersma, Paul (1999) “On the need for a separate perception grammar:” In: R. Kirchner, W. Wikeley
& J. Pater (eds.) Papers in Experimental and Theoretical Linguistics. Volume 5. Edmonton:
University of Alberta.
Boersma, Paul (2003a) “The odds of eternal optimization in OT.” In: Eric Holt (ed.) Optimality Theory
and Language Change. Dordrecht: Kluwer; 31-65.
Boersma, Paul (2003b) “Nasal harmony in functional phonology.” In: J. v.d. Weijer, H. v.d. Hulst &
V.v. Heuven (eds.) The phonological spectrum, Vol. 1: Segmental structure. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins; 3-35
Bolla, Kálmán (1981) A Conspectus of Russian Speech Sounds. Budapest: Hungarian Academy of
Science.
Booij, Gert & Jerzy Rubach (1987) “Postcyclic versus Postlexical Rules in Lexical Phonology.”
Linguistic Inquiry 18: 1-44.
Borgstrøm, C.H. (1940) “A linguistic survey of the Gaelic dialects of Scotland, vol. I: The dialects of
the outer Hebrids.” Norsk Tidsskrift for Sprogvidenskap. Suppl. Bind I. Oslo: Aschehoug.
Bradley, John (1980) “Acoustic correlates of seven consonantal positions in Yanyuwa.” University of
Melbourne Working Papers in Linguistics 6: 21-48.
Breen, John Gavan (1981) “Margany and Gunya.” In: R.M.W. Dixon & B. Blake (eds.); 275-392.
Bright, William O. (1957) The Karok language. Berkeley: Univ. of California Press.
Bright, William O. (1975) “The Dravidian Enunciative Vowels.” In: H. Schiffman & C. Eastman (eds.)
Dravidian phonological systems. Seattle: University of Washington; 11-46.
Brosnahan, L.F. & Bertil Malmberg (1970) Introduction to Phonetics. Cambridge: Heffer & Sons.
Browman, Catherine & Louis Goldstein (1989) “Articulatory gestures as phonological units.”
Phonology 6: 201-251.
Browman, Catherine & Louis Goldstein (1992) “Articulatory phonology: an overview.” Phonetica 49:
155-180.
Burrow, Thomas (1955) The Sanskrit language. London: Faber and Faber.
Busby, Peter Andres (1979) A classificatory study of phonemic systems in Australian Aboriginal
languages. Manuscript (M.A. thesis), Australian National University.
Butcher, Andrew (1992) “The phonetics of neutralisation: the case of Australian coronals.” In: Jack
Windson (ed.) Studies in general and English phonetics. London: Routledge; 10-38.
Butcher, Andrew (1995) The Phonetics of Australian languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bybee, Joan (1999) “Usage-based Phonology.” In: M. Darnell et.al. (eds.); 211-242.
Calabrese, Andrea (1988) “Towards a Theory of phonological alphabets.” Ph.D. dissertation,
Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Catford, John C. (1970) “Articulatory possibilities of man.” In: Malmberg (ed.); 309-333.
Catford, John C. (1977) Fundamental Problems in Phonetics.Edinburgh: University Press.
Catford, John C. (1988) A Practical Introduction to Phonetics. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Catford, John C. (1994) “Articulatory Phonetics.” In: Ronald Asher (ed.) The Encyclopaedia of
Language and Linguistics. Oxford: Pergamon Press; 3058-3070.
Cavar, Malgorzata & Silke Hamann (2002) “Polish velar and coronal palatalization – its perceptual
background.” Ms. to be published in Proceedings of the Fourth European Conference on Formal
Description of Slavic Languages (FDSL-4)
Chao, Y. R. (1948) Mandarin Primer. Cambridge (Mass.): Harvard University Press.
Chao, Y. R. (1968) A Grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Cho, Young-Mee (1991) “On the Universality of the Coronal Articulator.” In: Paradis & Prunet (eds.);
159-179.
Chomsky, Noam & Morris Halle (1968) The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.
Christdas, Prathima (1988) The Phonology and Morphology of Tamil. Ann Arbour: UMI.
References
215
Clements, George N. (1985) “The Geometry of Phonological Features.” Phonology Yearbook 2: 225252.
Clements, George N. (1991) “Place of articulation in consonants and vowels: a unified theory.”
Working Papers of the Cornell Phonetics Laboratory 5: 77-123.
Clements, George N. (2001) “Representational economy in constraint-based phonology.” T. Hall (ed.)
Distinctive feature theory. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter; 71- 146.
Clements, G. Nick & Elizabeth Hume (1995) “The Internal Organization of Speech Sounds.” In: J.
Goldsmith (ed); 245-306.
Connell, Bruce & Amalia Arvaniti (eds.) Phonology and phonetic evidence: Papers in laboratory
phonology IV. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1995.
Crowley, Terry (1981) “Anguthimiri (the Mpakwithi Dialect).” In: R.M.W. Dixon & Barry Blake
(eds.); 147-196.
Crowley, Terry (1983) “Uradhi.” R.M.W. Dixon & Barry Blake (eds.); 309-416
Darnell, Michael, Edith Moravcsik, Frederick Newmeyer, Michael Noonan & Kathleen Wheatley
(eds.) Functionalism and Formalism in Linguistics. Volume 1: general papers. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins, 1999.
Dart, Sarah (1991) “Articulatory and Acoustic Properties of Apical and Laminal Articulations.” UCLA
Working Papers in Phonetics 79: 1-155.
Dart, Sarah & Paroo Nihalani (1999) “The articulation of Malayalam coronal stops and nasals.”
Journal of the International Phonetic Association 29,2: 129-142.
Dave, Radhekant (1977) “Retroflex and dental consonants in Gujarati: a palatographic and acoustic
study.” Annual Report of the Institute of Phonetics, University of Copenhagen (ARIPUC) 11: 27156.
Davis, Stuart (1991) “Coronals and the Phonotactics of Nonadjacent Consonants in English.” In: C.
Paradis & J.-F. Prunet (eds.); 49-60.
De Jong, Kenneth (1995) “On the status of redundant features: the case of backing and rounding in
American English.” In: B. Connell & A. Arvaniti (eds.); 68-86.
Diffloth, Gerard (1975) “The south Dravidian obstruent system in Irula.” In H. Schiffman & C.
Eastman (eds.) Dravidian phonological systems. Seattle: University of Washington; 47-56.
Dixit, R. Prakash (1990) “Linguotectal contact patterns in the dental and retroflex stops of Hindi.”
Journal of Phonetics 18: 189-201.
Dixit, R. Prakash & James Flege (1991) “Vowel Context, Rate and Loudness Effects of Linguopalatal
Contact Patterns in Hindi Retroflex /long-legged t/.” Journal of Phonetics 19.2: 213-229.
Dixon, R.M.W. (1980) The Languages of Australia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dixon, R.M.W. & Barry Blake (eds.) Handbook of Australian languages. Volume II. Amsterdam:
Benjamins, 1981.
Dixon, R.M.W. & Barry Blake (eds.) Handbook of Australian languages. Volume III. Amsterdam:
Benjamins, 1983.
Dixon, R.M.W. & Barry Blake (eds.) Handbook of Australian languages. Volume IV. Oxford: Oxford
University Press, 1991.
Dixon, R.M.W. & Barry Blake (eds.) Handbook of Australian languages. Volume V. Oxford: Oxford
University Press, 2000.
Docherty, Gerard J. & D. Robert Ladd (eds.) Papers in Laboratory Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press, 1992.
Dogil, Grzegorz (1990) Hissing and Hushing Fricatives: A Comment on non-anterior Spirants in Polish.
Manuscript, Stuttgart University.
Dommelen, Wim van (2001) “Teenagers’ pronunciation of Norwegian // and //.” Lund University
Working Papers 49: 22-25.
Donaldson, Tamsin (1980) Ngiyamba. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ebert, Karen (1996) Kodava. Muenchen: Lincom Europa.
Edelman, Dzoj I. (1983) The Dardic and Nuristani languages. Moscow: Nauka.
Eliasson, Stig (1986) “Sandhi in Peninsular Scandinavian.” In: Henning Andersen (ed.) Sandhi
Phenomena in the Languages of Europe. Berlin: de Gruyter; 271-300.
216
References
Emeneau, Murray B. (1939) “The vowels of the Badaga language.” Language 15 (1); 43-47.
Emeneau, Murray B. (1970) Dravidian Comparative Phonology: A Sketch. Annamalainagar:
Annamalai University.
Emeneau, Murray B. (1984) Toda Grammar and Texts. Philadelphia: American Philosophical Society.
Endresen, R.T. (1974) “On Retroflex Segments in Norwegian.” Norwegian Journal of Linguistics 28:
73-78.
Evans, Nickolas (1985) Kayardild: the language of the Bentick Islanders of North West Queensland.
Ph.D. dissertation, Australian National University.
Evans, Nickolas (1995) “Current Issues in the Phonology of Australian Languages.” In: J. Goldsmith
(ed.); 723-740.
Fant, Gunnar (1974) Speech Sounds and Features. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Ferguson, Charles A. (1960) Linguistic diversity in South Asia: studies in regional, social and functional
variation. Bloomington (Ind.): Research Center at Indiana University.
Flemming, Edward (1995) Auditory Representations in Phonology. Dissertation UCLA.
Flemming, Edward (1997) “Phonetic detail in phonology: Towards a unified account of assimilation and
coarticulation.” In: K. Suzuki and D. Elzinga (eds.) Proceedings of the 1995 Southwestern
Workshop in Optimality Theory (SWOT), University of Arizona.
Flemming, Edward (2002) Auditory Representations in Phonology. Routledge: London.
Fougeron, Cécile (1999) “Prosodically conditioned articulatory variation: A review.” UCLA Working
Papers in Phonetics 87; 1-73.
Fowler, Carol A. (1994) “Invariants, specifiers, cues: an investigation of locus equation as information
for place of articulation.” Perception and Psychophysics 55: 597-610.
Gair, James W. & John C. Paolillo (1997) Sinhala. Unterschleissheim: Lincom.
Gnanadesikan, Amalia (1993) “The Feature Geometry of Coronal Subplaces.” University of
Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Lingiustics 16: 27-67.
Gnanadesikan, Amalia (1994) ”The Geometry of Coronal Articulations.” Proceedings of the North East
Linguistics Society 24,1: 125-139.
Goeman, Ton & Hans Van de Velde (1999) “Co-occurrence constraints on /r/ and // in Dutch
dialects.” In: H. v. d. Velde & R. v. Hout (eds.); 91-112.
Goldsmith, John (1976) Autosegmental phonology. Ph.D. manuscript, Cambridge: Massachusetts
Institute of Technology.
Goldsmith, John (ed.) The Handbook of Phonological Theory. Cambridge: Blackwell, 1995.
Greenberg, Joseph H. (1978) “Some generalizations concerning initial and final consonant clusters.”
In: J. Greenberg (ed.) Universals of Human Language. Volume 2: Phonology. Stanford: Stanford
University Press; 243-279.
Grierson, George (1911) Standard manual of the Kashmiri language. 2 volumes. Jammu: Light and Life.
Gussenhoven, Carlos & Haike Jakobs (1998) Understanding Phonology. London: Arnold.
Hagiwara, Robert (1995) “Acoustic realizations of American /r/ as produced by women and men.”
UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 90.
Hall, T. Alan (1997a) The Phonology of Coronals. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Hall, T. Alan (1997b) “The Historical Development of Retroflex Consonants in Indo-Aryan.” Lingua
102: 203-221.
Hall, T. Alan (2000) “Typological generalizations concerning secondary palatalization.” Lingua 110:
1-25.
Halle, Morris (1992) “Phonological Features.” In: W. Bright (ed.) Oxford International Encyclopedia of
Linguistics. Oxford; 207-212.
Halle, Morris & Kenneth Stevens (1969) “On the feature ‘advanced tongue root’.” Quarterly Progress
Report 94. Cambridge, MA: Research Laboratory of Electronics, MIT, 209-215.
Halle, Morris & Kenneth Stevens (1997) “The postalveolar fricatives of Polish.” In: S. Kiritani, H.
Hirose & H. Fujisaki (eds.). Speech Production and Language: In Honor of Osamu Fujimura.
Berlin. Mouton de Gruyter; 177-193.
Hamann, Silke (2002a) “Retroflexion and Retraction revised.” In: T.A. Hall, B. Pompino-Marschall &
M. Rochon (eds.) ZAS working papers 28: 13-25.
References
217
Hamann, Silke (2002b) “Postalveolar Fricatives in Slavic Languages as Retroflexes.” In: S. Baauw, M.
Huiskes & M. Schoorlemmer (eds.) OTS Yearbook 2002. Utrecht: Utrecht Institute of
Linguistics; 105-127.
Hamann, Silke (2003) “Is retroflexion licensed by VC cues only? Observations from Norwegian.” Ms.
to be published in P. Svevonius (ed.) Proceedings of the 19th Scandinavian Conference of
Linguistics.
Hamilton, Philip James (1993) “On the Internal Structure of the Coronal Node.” ESCOL 93: 129-140.
Hamilton, Philip James (1996) Phonetic constraints and markedness in the phonotactics of Australian
Aboriginal languages. Toronto: University of Toronto.
Harris, John (1994) English sound structure. Oxford: Blackwell.
Harrison, Sheldon (1995) “Ponapean.” In: D. Tryon, M. Ross, et al. (eds.) Comparative Austronesian
Dictionary: An Introduction. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 905-912.
Heike, Georg (1982) Phonologie. Stuttgart: Metzler.
Heath, Jeffrey (1980) Basic materials in Ritharngu: grammar, texts, and dictionary. Canberra:
Australian National University.
Hercus, Luise A. (1986) Viktorian languages: a late survey. Canberra: Australian National University.
Himmelmann, N.P. (1991) “Tonini-Tolitoli sound structures.” In: James Sneddon (ed.) Studies in
Sulawesi Linguistics II. Jakarta: Nusa; 49-70.
Hudson, Joyce & Eirlys Richards (1969) “The phonology of Walmatjari.” Oceanic Linguistics 8: 171189.
Hume, Elizabeth (1994) Front Vowels, Coronal Consonants and their Interaction in Nonlinear
Phonology. London: Garland.
Hume, Elizabeth & Keith Johnson (2001) Phonology: The Role of Speech Perception in Phonology. San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Hume, Elizabeth & Georgios Tserdanelis (2002) “Labial unmarkedness in Sri Lankan Portuguese
Creole.” Manuscript, Ohio State University. ROA 545.
Hunter, G.G. & E.V. Pike (1969) “Phonology of tone sandhi of Molinos Mixtec.” Linguistics 47: 24-40.
Hyman, Larry (1973) “The Feature [grave] in Phonological Theory.” Journal of Phonetics 1: 329-337.
Iverson, Gregory K. & M. L. Onderra (1985) “On Basque Palatalization.” Folia Linguistica 19: 51-61.
Jakobson, Roman, Gunnar Fant & Morris Halle (1952) Preliminaries to Speech Analysis: The
Distinctive Features and their Correlates. Cambridge (Mass.): MIT Press.
Jakobson, Roman & Morris Halle (1956) Fundamentals of Language. The Hague: Mouton.
Jassem, Wiktor (1979) “Classification of Fricative Spectra using Statistical Discrimant Functions.” In:
B. Lindblom & S. Öhman (eds.) Frontiers of Speech Communication Research. New York:
Academic Press; 77-91.
Johnson, Keith (1997) Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics. Oxford: Blackwell.
Jones, Daniel & Dennis Ward (1969) The Phonetics of Russian. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Jun, Jongho (1995) “Place assimilation as the result of conflicting perceptual and articulatory
constraints.” West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics 14.
Karunatillake, W.S. (1992) An Introduction to spoken Sinhala.
Keating, Patricia A. (1991) “Coronal Places of Articulation.” In: C. Paradis & J.-F. Prunet (eds.), 29-48.
Kehrein, Wolfgang (2002) Phonological Representation and Phonetic Phasing. Tuebingen: Max
Niemeyer Verlag.
Kelkar, Ashok R. & Pran Nath Trisal (1964) “Kashmiri Word Phonology: A First Sketch.”
Anthropological Linguistics 6,1: 13-22.
Kingston, John & Mary Beckman (eds.) Papers in Laboratory Phonology I: Between the Grammar and
Physics of Speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1990.
Kingston, John & Randy L. Diehl (1994) “Phonetic knowledge.” Language 70,3: 419-454.
Kingston, John & Randy L. Diehl (1995) “Intermediate properties in the perception of distinctive
feature values.” In: B. Connell & A. Arvaniti (eds.); 7-27.
Kirchner, Robert (1997) ”Contrastiveness and Faithfulness.” Phonology 14.1: 83-111.
218
References
Krakow, Rena (1999) “Physiological organization of syllables: a review.” Journal of Phonetics 27: 2354.
Krauss, Michael E. (1973) “Na-Dené.” In T. Sebeok (ed.), Linguistics in North America. Current
Trends in Linguistics Vol. 10. The Hague: Mouton.
Krauss, Michael E. (1979) “Na-Dené and Eskimo-Aleut.” in L. Campbell & M. Mithun (eds.) The
Languages of Native North America: An Historical and Comparative Assessment. Austin:
University of Texas Press; 803-901.
Kristoffersen, Gjert (2000) The Phonology of Norwegian. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Krull, Diana (1990) “Relating acoustic properties to perceptual responses: A study of Swedish voiced
stops.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 88,6: 2557-2570.
Krull, Diana, Björn Lindblom, B.-E. Shia & D. Fruchter (1995) “Crosslinguistic Aspects of
Coarticulation: An Acoustic and Electropalatographic Study of Dental and Retroflex
Consonants.” Proceedings of the XIIIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences
(Stockholm).Volume 4: 436-439.
Ladefoged, Peter (1964) A Phonetic Study of West African Languages: An Auditory-Instrumental
Survey. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ladefoged, Peter (1971) Preliminaries to Linguistic Phonetics. Chicago: UCP.
Ladefoged, Peter (1975) A Course in Phonetics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. 2nd ed. 1982.
Ladefoged, Peter (1994) “The links between theory and fieldwork in phonetics.” UCLA Working
Papers in Phonetics 88: 13-28.
Ladefoged, Peter (2001) Vowels and consonants: An introduction to the sounds of languages. Oxford:
Blackwell.
Ladefoged, Peter & Ian Maddieson (1986) “Some of the Sounds of the World’s Languages:
preliminary version.” UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 64.
Ladefoged, Peter & Ian Maddieson (1996) The Sounds of the World’s Languages. Oxford: Blackwell.
Ladefoged, Peter & Peri Bhaskararao (1983) “Non-quantal Aspects of Consonant Production: A Study
of Retroflex Consonants.” Journal of Phonetics 11: 291-302.
Ladefoged, Peter & Zongji Wu (1984) “Places of Articulation: An Investigation of Pekingese Fricatives
and Affricates.” Journal of Phonetics 12: 267-278.
Ladefoged, Peter, A. Cochran & S. Disner (1977) “Laterals and trills.” Journal of the International
Phonetic Association 7: 46-54.
Lahiri, Aditi & Sheila E. Blumstein (1984) “A re-evaluation of the feature Coronal.” Journal of
Phonetics 12,2: 133-146.
Laver, John (1994) Principles of Phonetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lee, Wai-Sum (1999) “An articulatory and acoustical analysis of the syllable-initial sibilants and
approximants in Beijing Mandarin.” Proceedings of the International Congress on Phonetic
Sciences 1999, San Francisco, 413-416.
Lin, Yen-Hwei (1989) “The Retroflex as a Complex Segment.” ESCOL 6: 182-193.
Lindau, Mona (1985) “The story of /r/.” In: Victoria Fromkin (ed.) Phonetic Linguistics: Essays in
Honour of Peter Ladefoged. Orlando (Fl.): Academic Press; 157-168.
Lindau, Mona & Peter Ladefoged (1986) “Variability of feature specification.” In: J.S. Perkell & D.H.
Klatt (eds.) Invariance and Variability in Speech Processes. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 465-478.
Lindblad, Per (1980) “Svenskans sje- och tje-ljud i ett allmänfonetisk perspektiv.” Travaux de l’Institut
de Linguistique de Lund 16, Lund: Gleerup.
Lindblad, Per & Sture Lundqvist (1994) “The Production of Swedish Sibilants – an EPG Analysis.”
Working Papers of the Lund University Department of Linguistics and Phonetics 43: 88-91.
Lindblad, Per & Sture Lundqvist (1997) “The production of coronals in hyper- and hypospeech.”
Phonum 4: 29-33.
Lindblad, Per & Sture Lundqvist (1999) “How and why do the tongue gestures of [t], [d], [l], [n], [s],
and [r] differ?” Proceedings of the International Congress on Phonetic Sciences 1999, San
Francisco, 417-420.
Lindblom, Björn (1986) “Phonetic universals in vowel systems.” In: J.J. Ohala & J.J. Jaeger (eds.)
Experimental Phonology. Orlando: Academic Press; 13-44.
References
219
Lindblom, Björn & Johan Sundberg (1971) “Acoustical consequences of lip, tongue, jaw, and larynx
movement.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 50,4 (2): 1166-1179.
Lobacz, Piotra (1996) Polska fonologia dziecieca. Warszawa: Energeia.
Löhken, Sylvia C. (1997) Deutsche Wortprosodie: Abschwächungs- und Tilgungsvorgänge. Studien zur
deutschen Grammatik 56, Tübingen: Stauffenburg Verlag.
Lombardi, Linda (1995) “Laryngeal neutralization and syllable wellformedness.” Natural Language
and Linguistic Theory 13: 39-74.
Maddieson, Ian (1984) Patterns of Sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Maddieson, Ian (1986) “The size and structure of phonological inventories: Analysis of UPSID.” In: J.
Ohala & J. Jaeger (eds.) Experimental Phonology. Orlando (Fl.): Academic Press; 105-123.
Masica, Colin (1991) The Indo-Aryan languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mayrhofer, Manfred (1989) “Vorgeschichte.” In: R. Schmitt (ed.); 4-24.
McCarthy, John (1988) “Feature geometry and dependency: a review.” Phonetica 43: 84-108.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1993) Prosodic morphology I: Constraint interaction and satisfaction.
Manuscript, University of Massachusetts and Rutgers University.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1995) “Faithfulness and reduplicative identity.” In: J.Beckmann, S.
Urbanczyck, & L. Walsh (eds.) Papers in Optimality Theory. University of Massachusetts
Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18; 249-384. ROA 60.
McCawley, James (1966) Further revisions in Finnish rules. Manuscript, University of Chicago.
McDonald, Maryalyce (1977) “A study of the phonetics and phonology of Yaraldi and associated
dialects.” M.A. thesis manuscript, Australian National University.
McDonough, Joyce & Keith Johnson (1997) “Tamil liquids: An investigation into the basis of the
contrast among five liquids in a dialect of Tamil.” Journal of the International Phonetic
Association 27: 1-26.
McGregor, William (1990) A functional grammar of Gooniyandi. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
McKay, Graham (2000) “Ndjébbana.” Dixon, R.M.W. & Barry Blake (eds.); 155 – 354.
Miller, Wick R. (1965) Acoma grammar and texts. University of California Publications in Linguistics.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Misra, B.G. (1967) “Historical phonology of modern standard Hindi: Proto-Indo European to the
present.” Ph.D. dissertation, Cornell University.
Morgenstierne, Georg (1927) An etymological vocabulary of Pashto. Oslo: J. Dybwad.
Morgenstierne, Georg (1929) “The Language of the Ashkun Kafirs.” Norsk Tidsskrift for
Sprogvidenskap 2: 192.
Morgenstierne, Georg (1941) “The Phonology of Kashmiri.” Acta Orientalia 19,1: 79-99.
Morphy, Frances (1983) “Djapu, a Yolngu dialect.” In: R.M.W. Dixon & B. Blake (eds.); 1-188.
Narayanan, Shrikanth & Abigail Kaun (1999) “Acoustic modeling of Tamil retroflex liquids.”
Proceedings of the International Congress on Phonetic Sciences 1999, San Francisco, 20972101.
Nathan, Geoffrey S. (1994) “What Functionalists can learn from Formalists in Phonology.” In: Darnell
et al. (eds.) Volume 1; 305-327.
Nearey, Terrance M. (1995) “A double-weak view of trading relations: comments on Kingston and
Diehl.” In: B. Connell & A. Arvaniti (eds.); 28-40.
Newman, Paul (1980) “The two R’s in Hausa.” African Language Studies 17: 77-87.
Odden, David (1978) “Further Evidence for the Feature [grave].” Linguistic Inquiry 9: 141-144.
Öhman, S.E.G. (1966) “Perception of Segments of VCCV Utterances.” Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America 40,5: 979-988.
Ohala, John J. (1981) “Speech Timing as a Tool in Phonology.” Phonetica 38: 204-212.
Ohala, John J. (1985) “Around Flat.” In: V. Fromkin (ed.) Phonetic Linguistics: Essays in Honor of
Peter Ladefoged. Orlando etc: Academic Press; 223-241.
Ohala, John J. (1989) “Sound change is drawn from a pool of synchronic variation.” In: L. Breivik &
E.H. Jahr (eds.) Language change: contributions to the study of its causes. Berlin: Mouton; 173198.
220
References
Ohala, John J. (1990) “The phonetics and phonology of aspects of assimilation.” In: J. Kingston & M.
Beckman (eds.); 258-275.
Ohala, John J. (1992) “Alternatives to the sonority hierarchy for explaining segmental sequential
constraints.” Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistic Society 26,2: 319-338.
Ohala, John J. (1993) “The percetual basis of some sound patterns.” In: B.A. Connell & A. Arvaniti
(eds.); 87-92.
Ohala, Manjari (1983) Aspects of Hindi phonology. Delhi: Motilal Banarsidas.
Ohala, Manjari (1994) “Hindi.” Journal of the International Phonetics Association 24.1: 35-38.
Ohala, Manjari & John Ohala (2001) “Acoustic VC transitions correlate with degree of perceptual
confusion of place contrast in Hindi.” N. Grø & J. Rischel (eds.) Travaux du cercel
Linguisticque de Copenhague. Vol. XXXI. Copenhagen: Reitzel; 265-284.
Ohde, Ralph N. & Donald J. Sharf (1992) Phonetic Analysis of Normal and Abnormal Speech. London:
Macmillan.
Oliverius, Z. F. (1974) Fonetika Russkogo Jazyka. Prague: Statni Pedagogicke Nakladatelstvi.
Padgett, Jaye (2001) “Contrast Dispersion and Russian Palatalization.” In: B. Hume & K. Johnson
(eds.); 187-218.
Padgett, Jaye (2003) “The emergence of contrastive palatalization in Russian.” In: Eric Holt (ed.)
Optimality Theory and Language Change. Dordrecht: Kluwer; 307-335.
Pandit, P.B. (1954) “Indo-Aryan sibilants in Gujarati.” Indian Linguistics 14: 503-511.
Papazian, Erik (1994) “Om sje-lyden in norsk, og ombyttina av den med-kje-lyden.” Norskrift 83: 1105.
Paradis, Carole & Jean-Francois Prunet (eds.) The Special Status of Coronals: Internal and External
Evidence. San Diego: Academic Press, 1991.
Passy, Paul (1891) Etude sur les changements phonétiques et leurs caractères généraux. Paris: Librairie
Firmin-Didot.
Patz, Elisabeth (1991) “Djabugay.” In: R.M.W. Dixon & B. Blake (eds.); 244-347.
Payne, John (1989) “Pamir languages.” In: R. Schmitt (ed.); 417-444.
Penzl, Herbert (1969) Geschichtliche deutsche Lautlehre. Muenchen: Huebner.
Pierrehumbert, Janet (1999) “Formalizing Functionalism.” In: M. Darnell et. al. (eds.); 287-304.
Pierrehumbert, Janet, Mary Beckman & D. Robert Ladd (1996) “Laboratory Phonology.” In: Jacques
Durand & Bernard Laks (eds.) Current Trends in Phonology: Models and Methods. Salford:
ESRI; 136-149.
Platt, John T. (1972) An outline grammar of the Gugada dialect: South Australia. Canberra: AIAS.
Popperwell, Ronald G. (1963) The pronunciation of Norwegian. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Prince, A. & P. Smolensky (1993) Optimality Theory: Constrait Interaction in Generative Grammar.
Brunswick: Rutgers University. ROA 537 (version from 2002).
Prost, G. (1967) “Phonemes of the Chacobo language.” Linguistics 35: 61-65.
Pulleyblank, Edwin G. (1989) “The Role of Coronal in Articulator Based Features.” Papers from the
Chicago Linguistics Society 25: 279-393.
Recasens, Daniel, Jordi Fontdevila, & Maria Dolors Pallarès (1995) “A production and perceptual
acount of palatalization.” In: Bruce Connell & Amalia Arvaniti (eds.); 265-281.
Rehg, Kenneth L. (1973) “On the history of Ponapean Phonology.” Working Papers in Linguistics,
University of Hawaii, 5 (8): 17-56.
Rice, Keren (1994) “Peripheral in Consonants.” Canadian Journal of Linguistics 39: 191-216.
Rubach, Jerzy (1984) Cyclic and Lexical Phonology. The Structure of Polish. Dordrecht: Foris.
Rubach, Jerzy (1995) “Representations and the Organization of Rules in Slavic Phonology.” In: J.
Goldsmith (ed.); 848-866.
Rumsey, Alan (2000) “Bunuba.”In: R.M.W. Dixon & B. Blake (eds.); 35-152.
Sagey, Elizabeth C. (1986) The representation of features and relations in non-linear phonology. MIT
Working Papers in Linguistics, Cambridge (Mas.): MIT.
Sakthivel, Subbiah (1976) Phonology of Toda with Vocabulary. Annamalaingar: Sri Velan Press.
Sakthivel, Subbiah (1977) A Grammar of the Toda Language. Annamalainagar: Sri Velan Press.
References
221
Scatton, Ernest A. (1975) Bulgarian Phonology. Columbus: Slavica Publishers.
Schiffman, Harold (1983) A Reference Grammar of spoken Kannada. Seattle: University of
Washington Press.
Schmitt, Rüdiger (ed.) Compendium linguarum Iranicum. Wiesbaden: Reichert, 1989.
Shadle, Christine, C.U. Dobelke & C. Scully (1992) “Spectral Analysis of Fricatives in Vowel Context.”
Proceedings J. de Physique IV; 295-298.
Shalev, Michael, Peter Ladefoged & Peri Bhaskararao (1993) “Phonetics of Toda.” UCLA Working
Papers in Phonetics 84: 89-123.
Sharpe, Margaret (1972) Alawa phonology and grammar. Canberra: AIAS.
Simonsen, Hanne G., Inger Moen & Steve Cowen (2000) “Retroflex consonants in Norwegian: are
they really? Evidence from EMA and EPG.” Processings of the 5th Seminar on Speech
Production, Zeon, Germany.
Skjærvø, Prods (1989a) “Modern East Iranian languages.” In: Schmitt (ed.); 370-383.
Skjærvø, Prods (1989b) “Pashto.” In: Schmitt (ed.); 384-410.
Skjærvø, Prods (1989c) “Yidgha and Munjı.” In: Schmitt (ed.); 410-410.
Smalley, William (1961) Manual of Articulatory Phonetics. South Passadena: Carey.
Spajic, Sinisa, Peter Ladefoged & Peri Bhaskararao (1996) “The trills of Toda.” Journal of the
International Phonetic Association 26,1: 1-21.
Spencer, Andrew (1984) “Eliminating the feature [lateral].” Journal of Linguistics 20: 23-43.
Sridhar, S.N. (1990) Kannada. London: Routledge.
Steriade, Donca (1995) Positional Neutralization. Manuscript, University of California, Los Angeles.
Steriade, Donca (1997) Phonetics in Phonology: the case of laryngeal neutralization. Manuscript,
University of California, Los Angeles.
Steriade, Donca (1999) “Licensing laryngeal features.” UCLA Working Papers in Phonology 3: 25146.
Steriade, Donca (2001a) “Directional Asymmetries in Place Assimilation.” In: Hume & Johnson (eds.);
219-250.
Steriade, Donca (2001b) The Phonology of Perceptability Effects: the P-map and its consequences for
constraint organization. Manuscript, University of California, Los Angeles.
Stevens, Kenneth (1996) “Articulatory-Acoustic-Auditory Relationship.” In: W. Hardcastle & J. Laver
(eds) The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences. Oxford: Blackwell; 462-506.
Stevens, Kenneth (1998) Acoustic Phonetics. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
Stevens, Kenneth & Sheila Blumstein (1975) “Quantal Aspects of Consonant Production and
Perception: a Study of Retroflex Stop Consonants.” Journal of Phonetics 3: 215-233.
Stevens, Kenneth & Sheila Blumstein (1981) “The search for invariant acoustic correlates of phonetic
features.” In: P.D. Eimas & J.L. Miller (eds.) Perspectives on the Study of Speech. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum; 1-38.
Stevens, Kenneth & Samuel Jay Keyser (1989) “Primary features and their enhancement in
consonants.” Language 65: 81-106.
Stevens, Kenneth, Samuel Keyser & Haruko Kawasaki (1986) “Toward a phonetic and phonological
theory of redundant features.” In: J.S. Perkell & D.H. Klatt (eds.) Invariance and Variability in
Speech Processes. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. 426-449.
Sundberg Johan & Björn Lindblom (1990) “Acoustic Estimations of the Front Cavity in Apical Stops.”
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 88, 3: 1313-1317.
Sussman, Harvey M. & J. Shore (1996) “Locus equations as phonetic descriptors of consonantal place
of articulation.” Perception & Psycholinguistics 58: 936-946.
Sussman, Harvey M., Nicola Bessell, Eileen Dalston & Tivoli Majors (1997) “An investigation of stop
place of articulaton as a function of syllable position: a locus equation perspective.” Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America 101, 5: 2826-2838.
Svantesson, Jan-Olof (2001) “Phonology of a southern Swedish idiolect.” Lund University Working
Papers 49: 156-159.
Svarny, O. & Kamil Zvelebil (1955) “Some remarks on the articulation of the ‘cerebral’ consonants in
Indian languages, especially in Tamil.” Archiv Orientalni 23: 374-434.
222
References
Tharp, George (1972) “The position of Tsetsaut among the Northern Athapaskans.” International
Journal of American Linguistics 38: 14-25.
Torp, Arne (2001) “Retroflex consonants and dorsal /r/: mutually excluding innovations? On the
diffusion of dorsal /r/ in Scandinavian.” In: H. v. d. Velde & R. v. Hout (eds.); 75-90.
Trask, Robert L. (1996) A Dictionary of Phonetics and Phonology. London: Routledge.
Trubetzkoy, Nikolai S. (1939) “Grundzüge der Phonologie.” Travaux du Cercle linguistique de Prague
7, Prague: Prazsky Lingvisticky Krouzek.
Tuttle, Siri (1998) Metrical and Tonal Structures in Tanana Athabaskan. PhD dissertation, University of
Washington.
Van der Velde, Hans & Roeland van Hout (eds.) ‘r-atics: Sociolinguistic, phonetic and phonological
characteristics of /r/. Universite Libre de Bruxelles: Etudes & Travaux, 1999.
Vanvik, Arne (1972) “A Phonetic-Phonemic Analysis of Standard Eastern Norwegian. Part I.” Norsk
Tidsskrift for Sprogvidenskap 26.2: 119-164.
Vanvik, Arne (1973) “A Phonetic-Phonemic Analysis of Standard Eastern Norwegian. Part II.” Norsk
Tidsskrift for Sprogvidenskap 27: 101-139.
Vennemann, Theo (1974) “Sanskrit ‘Ruki’ and the Concept of a Natural Class.” Linguistics 130: 91-97.
Verstraeten, Bart & Hans Van de Velde (2001) “Socio-geographical variation of /r/ in Standard
Dutch.” In: H. v. d. Velde & R. v. Hout (eds.); 45-62.
Wängler, Hans H. (1958) Atlas deutscher Sprachlaute. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.
Wakelin, M.F. (1972) English dialects: An introduction. London: Athlene Press.
Walley, A.C. & T.D. Carrell (1983) “Onset spectra and formant transitions in the adult’s and child’s
perception of place.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 73: 1011-1022.
Walsh Dickey, Laura (1997) The phonology of Liquids. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts at
Amherst.
Warner, Natasha (1999) “Timing of perception of vocalic distinctive features: Implications for vowel
system universals.” Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, San
Francisco, 1961-1964.
Wells, John C. (1982) Accents of English. 3 volumes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Whitney, William Dwight (1889) Sanskrit Grammar. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Wierzchowska, Boz· ena (1965) Wymowa polska. Warszawa: Panstwowe Zaklady Wydawnictw
Szkolnych.
Wierzchowska, Boz· ena (1980) Fonetyka I Fonologia Jezyka Polskiego. Wroclaw: Zaklad Narodowy
imienia Ossolinskich.
Wiese, Richard (2001) “The unity and variation of (German) /r/.” In: H. v. d. Velde & R. v. Hout
(eds.); 11-26.
Wilkinson, Karina (1988) “Prosodic Structure and Lardil Phonology.” Linguistic Inquiry 19,2: 325-334.
Williamson, Kay (1977) “Multivalued features for consonants.” Language 53: 843-871.
Winitz, H., M.E. Scheib & J.A. Reeds (1972) “Identification of stops and vowels for the burst portion of
/p, t, k/ isolated from conversational speech.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 51:
1309-1317.
Wood, Sidney A.J. (1996) “Assimilation or coarticulation? Evidence from the temporal co-ordination
of tongue gestures for the palatalization of Bulgarian alveolar stops.” Journal of Phonetics 24:
139-164.
Yip, Moira (1996) “Lexicon Optimization in Languages without Alternations.” In: Jacques Durand &
Bernard Laks (eds.) Current Trends in Phonology: Models and Methods. Salford: ESRI; 354385.
Zoll, Cheryl S. (1996) Parsing below the segment in a constraint based framework. Doctoral thesis,
University of California at Berkely. ROA 143.
Zvelebil, Kamil (1970) Comparative Dravidian Phonology. The Hague: Mouton.
Index
Abkhaz, 32, 207
Acoma, 92, 94, 95, 106
acquisition, 4
Adynyamathanha (Wailpi), 25, 29
affricate, 20, 24, 25, 38, 41
Agrawal, S.S., 73
Ahmed, Rais, 69, 73
Akan, 92
Allen, William, 108-111, 121
alveolar ridge, 13, 19, 25, 26, 28, 29, 35, 42
alveolo-palatal, 14, 30, 35, 43, 48
Andersen, Henning, 108
Anderson, Stephen, 134
Anderson, Victoria, 68, 72
Anguthimri, 25
Anttila, Arto, 176
Anum, 20
apicality, 11, 12, 33-35, 40, 42-44, 46-47, 51,
53, 55, 64, 96, 128, 200, 201
Archangeli, Diana, 4, 168
areal spread, 6
articulator, 12-16, 19-21, 26-28, 31, 33-35, 42,
47
Articulatory Phonology, 2
articulatory variation, 1, 7, 11, 14, 18, 29, 30,
33, 51
Arvaniti, Amalia, 133
Athapaskan languages, 25
Australian languages, 3, 15, 17, 20, 25, 26, 27,
29, 30, 31, 52
Avery, Peter, 143
Avestan, 86, 108, 111
Badaga, 36, 39, 112, 114, 129, 140
Bailey, T. Graham, 49
Balasubramanian, R., 28
Balasubramanian, T., 18, 20
Ball, Martin, 19, 25, 29, 87, 111, 121
Baltic, 108, 111
Beck, Janet Mackenzie, 15
Beckman, Jil, 151
Beckman, Mary, 131, 133
Beijing, 65, 100, 106
Bengali, 18, 113, 114
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo, 176
Bhaskararao, Peri, 3, 15, 16, 19, 28, 30, 39, 64,
67, 96, 117
Bhat, D.N.S., 1, 2, 6, 17, 27, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39,
45, 49, 81, 82, 85, 86, 88-90, 92, 121, 122,
128
Bhatia, Tej, 117, 119, 122
Biedrzycki, Leszek, 42
Blake, Barry, 72, 113
Blankenship, Barbara, 96
Blumstein, Sheila, 54, 55, 59, 60, 63, 64, 68,
69, 132, 133
Boersma, Paul, 2, 4, 7, 8, 20, 77, 131, 134,
143, 144, 148, 154-157, 159, 160, 162, 167173, 178, 188, 191, 193, 197, 203
Bolla, Kálmán, 36, 42, 43, 46, 50
Booij, Gert, 98
Borgstrøm, C.H., 48, 102
Bright, William, 24
Bradley, John, 55, 61
Brahui, 119
Breen, John Gavan, 91, 100, 113, 115
Bright, William, 95, 97, 101
Brosnahan, L.F., 36, 37, 58, 78
Browman, Catherine, 2, 123, 157, 162
Bulgarian, 38, 41, 46
Bunuba, 98, 105, 106, 112, 115, 180, 193
Burrow, Thomas, 121
Burushaski, 24
Busby, Peter, 55, 60, 61, 62
Butcher, Andrew, 17, 20, 29, 30, 31, 116
Calabrese, Andrea, 171
Catford, John C., 12-15, 27, 28, 34, 37, 38, 42,
43, 93, 205
Caucasian languages, 32
Cavar, Malgorzata, 46
Cham, 86
Chao, Y. R., 24, 65,
Chinese, 23, 24, 96, 98, 100, 103, 106, 107,
183
Cho, Young-Mee, 142
Chomsky, Noam, 4, 34, 131, 132, 134, 136138
Christdas, Prathima, 30, 112, 117
Clements, George N., 1, 2, 37, 137-139, 142
click, 18
constraints
*DELETE, 168, 169, 174-180, 188-190, 192,
193, 197
224
*DISTANCE, 172, 173, 177-189, 193-197
*INSERT, 168, 170, 173, 190
*GESTURE, 4, 171, 172, 188, 192, 193
LAZY, 171
MAX, 169
PATH, 168-170, 173, 188, 190, 191
*REPLACE, 168, 169, 176, 178-180, 182,
183, 185-190, 192, 194-197
*PRECISION, 171, 173, 176, 193
*SYCHRONIZE, 172, 173, 180, 187, 189,
191, 192
Crowley, Terry, 25, 91
cues
misparsing of, 88, 167
re-association of, 93, 178
Czech, 41
Dart, Sarah, 15, 20, 21, 30, 55, 58, 59-61, 64,
68, 69
Dave, Radhekant, 72
Davis, Stuart, 4
dental, 14, 16, 20, 30, 31, 34, 38, 39, 45, 54,
60, 61, 64, 69-71, 73-76, 99, 108, 118-124,
126, 172
Diffloth, Gerard, 102, 117
Dixit, R. Prakash, 1, 11, 13, 16, 17, 19, 20
Dixon, R.M.W., 26, 27, 90, 115-117, 135
Docherty, Gerard, 133
Dogil, Grzegorz, 41, 42, 66, 143
Dommelen, Wim van, 111
Donaldson, Tamsin, 115
dorsal, 46, 138-141, 143, 144, 150, 163, 170,
203
Dravidian languages, 3, 19, 21, 28, 30, 32, 48,
58, 62, 66, 86, 97, 100-102, 112, 117-120,
122, 124
Dutch, 29, 38, 87, 132, 158
Eastern Arrernte, 15, 30, 31, 55, 56
Ebert, Karen, 97, 117
Edelman, Dzoj, 24
Effutu, 20
ejective, 18
Eliasson, Stig, 84, 119
Emeneau, Murray, 26, 39, 45, 48, 97, 112, 126,
129
Endresen, R.T., 83, 84
English, 20, 21, 27-29, 32, 117, 128, 136, 146,
147
American, 27, 28, 60, 88, 90, 92, 94
British, 27, 38, 87, 112, 121, 123
EPG, 15-17
Index
Evans, Nickolas, 55, 62, 91, 100, 113, 115,
123, 124
Even, 25, 34, 91, 93, 142
Ewe, 19, 20, 29, 31, 32, 137, 161
Fant, Gunnar, 6, 60, 63, 89, 131, 134, 135
Feature Geometry (FG), 1, 2, 137-140, 142144, 148, 150, 157, 163-165, 170, 203
features
acute, 63, 134, 135, 149
anterior, 13, 29, 35, 39, 81, 83, 86, 89, 94,
136-143, 148, 150, 152, 153
compact, 135, 149
diffuse, 63, 135, 149
distributed, 39, 48, 136-142, 150, 157, 162
grave, 134-136, 143, 149
peripheral, 143
robust, 5, 75, 76, 149
weak, 5, 6
Ferguson, Charles, 113
Finno-Ugric languages, 20
Flege, James, 16, 17
Flemming, Edward, 2, 5, 6, 77, 98, 101, 104,
105, 107, 109, 110, 118, 134, 143-148, 152,
154, 157, 160, 161, 188
Fowler, Carol, 59
French, 20, 21, 29, 146
fricative, 3, 14, 17-18, 21-24, 31, 32, 34, 35,
38, 41-48, 51, 59, 63-68, 71, 72, 77, 78, 8486, 92, 94-96, 98, 99, 107-111, 121, 125127, 168, 189, 196
functional, 7, 8, 132, 143, 154, 164, 167
Functional Phonology (FP), 2, 154-157, 161,
165, 193, 198
Ga, 20
Gair, James, 91
Gbaya, 26
geminate, 18, 21
German, 21, 29, 73-75, 111, 132, 158
Germanic languages, 29
Ghanaian languages, 20
Gnanadesikan, Amalia, 1, 97, 98, 109, 110,
115, 116, 138-141
Goeman, Ton, 87
Goldsmith, John, 133, 138
Goldstein, Louis, 2, 123, 157, 162
Gondi, 119
Gooniyandi, 55, 116, 193
Greenberg, Joseph, 3
Grierson, George, 49
Gugada, 72, 100, 106, 187
Gujarati, 24, 72, 96, 106, 107
Index
Gunya, 91, 93, 100, 105-107, 113, 115, 181
Gupapuyngu, 29
Gussenhoven, Carlos, 143
G, 29
Hagiwara, Robert, 60
Hall, T. Alan, 18, 19, 30, 37, 38, 41, 45, 46, 48,
65, 87, 96, 98, 107-109, 139-141, 143
Halle, Morris, 4, 6, 34, 37, 63, 65, 66, 89, 131138, 149
Hamann, Silke, 36, 39, 41, 46, 73-76, 110, 160
Hamilton, Philip, 5, 15, 16, 38, 55, 60, 62, 75,
99, 113, 115-117, 136, 138, 144, 149-152,
154, 157, 171
Harris, John, 128
Harrison, Sheldon, 82
Hausa, 27, 88
Heath, Jeffrey, 123, 124
Heike, Georg, 13
Hercus, Luise, 98, 101
Himmelmann, N.P., 92
Hindi, 3, 15-20, 24, 26, 28-30, 39, 52, 54, 59,
62, 70-73, 117
Hudson, Joyce, 116
Hume, Elizabeth, 30, 47, 48, 92, 138, 139
Hunter, G.G., 92, 96
Hyman, Larry, 136
Igbo, 37
implosive, 18
Indo-Aryan languages, 21, 23, 25, 26, 28
inventories, 3, 5, 6
Iranian, 86
Iskasm, 86
Iverson, Gregory, 110
Jad, 87
Jakobson, Roman, 6, 63, 89, 131, 134-136,
143, 149
Jaqaru, 24
Jassem, Wiktor, 136
Johnson, Keith, 27, 57, 69, 71
Jones, Daniel, 43
Jun, Jongho, 151
Kannada, 21, 30, 83, 97, 100, 102, 106, 117,
120, 181, 194
Karok, 24, 95, 106
Karunatillake, 91
Kashmiri, 39, 45, 48-50, 52, 77, 193
Kaun, Abigail, 56, 60, 71
Kawasaki, Haruko, 37
Kayardild, 55, 62, 100, 106, 107, 113, 116,
181, 185, 186
225
Keating, Patricia, 2, 18, 21, 30, 35, 41-43, 52,
65, 136, 137
Kehrein, Wolfgang, 24, 25, 171
Kelkar, Ashok, 49
Keyser, Samuel Jay, 5, 37, 53, 76
Khonoma Angami, 96, 106
Kingston, John, 133, 144
Kirchner, Robert, 172
Kolami, 119
Konda, 119
Kota, 3, 112
Krakow, Rena, 107, 127
Krauss, Michael, 25, 82
Kristoffersen, Gjert, 83-85, 99, 111, 119
Krull, Diana, 16, 72, 73
Kunwinjku, 31
Kurukh, 119
Kuvi, 119
Ladefoged, Peter, 3, 7, 11-17, 19-30, 32, 36,
37, 39, 41, 42, 45, 46, 54, 56, 58, 60, 63-65,
67, 72, 88, 92, 93, 96, 117, 125, 132, 137,
146, 147, 174, 205, 207
Lahiri, Aditi, 54, 132
Lardil, 36, 38, 40, 116, 140
Late, 20
lateral, 18, 20, 25, 26, 38, 56, 57, 60, 61, 69,
71, 86, 91-93, 98, 106, 119, 120, 126, 147
Laver, John, 13, 14, 20, 23, 24, 26-28, 36, 37,
44, 58,
Lee, Wai-Sum, 24, 65, 199
lexicon, 8, 159
Lin, Yen-Hwei, 24, 37, 98, 100, 139, 140, 142
Lindau, Mona, 88, 132
Lindblad, Per, 18, 21, 29, 63, 64, 68, 71
Lindblom, Björn, 16, 35, 55, 57, 145
Lobacz, Piotra, 4
Logba, 29
Löhken, Sylvia, 176
Lombardi, Linda, 151
Lugbara, 86, 88
Lundqvist, Sture, 18, 21, 29
Maddieson, Ian, 3, 11-14, 17, 19-25, 32, 36,
37, 41-43, 45, 49, 55, 56, 58, 60, 63-65, 68,
72, 88, 92, 93, 125, 146, 147, 174, 205, 207
Malayalam, 15, 17, 20, 27, 58-61, 64, 68, 69,
97, 100, 106, 120, 123
Malmberg, Bertil, 36, 37, 58, 53, 59, 65, 66, 78
Malto, 119
Mandarin, 11, 23, 24, 33, 34, 41, 51, 95, 96,
147, 199, 206
Marathi, 18
226
Margany, 91, 93, 100, 105-107, 113, 115, 177,
178, 191
Marrithiyel, 91, 113, 114
Masica, Colin, 25, 26, 49, 86, 121
Maung, 26
Mayrhofer, Manfred, 108
Mazatec, 92, 94
McCarthy, John, 137, 141, 168-170
McCawley, James, 37
McDonald, Maryalyce, 55
McDonough, Joyce, 27, 69, 71
McGregor, William, 55, 60-62, 68, 72, 116,
193
McKay, Graham, 83, 85, 115, 116
Mester, Armin, 142
Micronesian languages, 24, 38
Miller, Wick, 95
Minto-Nenana, 25
Miriwung, 15
Misra, B.G., 108, 121
Mixtec, 92-94, 96, 106
Morgenstierne, Georg, 49, 50, 86, 109
Morphy, Frances, 113, 117
Murrinh-Patha, 31
Narayanan, Shrikanth, 56, 60, 71
nasal, 3, 15, 18, 20, 25, 47, 58, 60-62, 73, 74,
85, 91, 93, 117, 119, 120, 122-124, 126,
138, 177, 196, 197
Nathan, Geoffrey, 8
Navajo, 25
Nearey, Terrance, 132
Newman, Paul, 27
Nihalani, 15, 20, 59, 61, 64, 68, 69
Norwegian, 1, 3, 15, 16, 29, 31, 54, 73-75, 83,
84, 86-88, 99, 106, 110, 111, 119, 120, 174176, 186, 187, 198, 199, 207
Nyangumarta, 31
Nyawaygi, 90, 91, 93
Odden, David, 136
O’Grady, 25
Ohala, John, 5, 54, 55, 58, 59, 62, 70-74, 76,
77, 88, 143, 157
Ohala, Manjari, 14, 24, 30, 54, 55, 59, 62, 7074, 76
Ohde, Ralph N, 12
Öhman, S.E.G., 73
Old Persian, 108, 111
Oliverius, Z. F., 43
Ostyak, 20, 24
Optimality Theory (OT), 2, 4, 144, 145, 149,
152, 155, 161, 167, 168, 171, 176, 178, 193
Index
'O'odham , 58
Padgett, Jaye, 5, 6
Pahari, 25
palatal, 13, 14, 18, 19, 21, 23, 30-32, 34, 35,
46, 49, 58, 64-67, 94, 96, 102, 103, 109111, 124-128, 179, 189, 190, 195
palatalization, 1, 6, 11, 12, 39, 40, 45-49, 51,
81, 94, 102-106, 110, 111, 126, 167, 179,
189, 190, 197, 203
palato-alveolar, 12, 23, 30, 34, 41, 61, 64, 67,
68, 70, 71, 75, 76, 78, 92, 95, 96, 102, 108111, 189
Pama-Nyungan languages, 25, 29
Pandit, P.B., 24, 96
Papazian, Erik, 111
Paradis, Carole, 4
Parji, 119
Pashto, 86
Passy, Paul, 145
Patz, Elisabeth, 75
Payne, John, 86
Pengo, 119
Penzl, Herbert, 111
pharyngealization, 36, 37, 39, 45, 46
pharynx, 12, 36, 60, 134
phonetic grounding, 1, 2, 4, 8, 9, 76, 82, 90,
95, 107, 127, 131, 136, 202
Pierrehumbert, Janet, 7, 8, 131, 144
Pike, E.V., 92, 96
Pingding, 98, 100, 103, 106, 183
Pitjantjatjara, 90
Pitta-Pitta, 72, 113, 114, 116, 135
Platt, John, 72, 100
Polish, 4, 24, 32, 33, 35, 38, 41-43, 46-48, 51,
59, 65, 66, 77, 78, 95, 98, 99, 102, 106, 136,
146, 147, 180, 205, 206
Ponapean, 24, 38
Popperwell, Ronald, 85
post-alveolar, 12-16, 18, 19, 21, 23, 26-32, 34,
37-39, 41-48, 51, 52
posteriority, 11, 12, 33-35, 40, 42-45, 47, 51,
52, 53, 55, 56, 58, 64, 77, 125, 128, 200,
201
Prince, Alan, 2, 4, 144, 167-170
Prost, G., 96
Prunet, Jean-Francois, 4
Pulleyblank, Edwin, 1, 24, 138-142, 168
Punjabi, 26, 117, 119, 122, 123, 154, 192, 195
Rahilly, 19, 25, 87, 111, 121
Recasens, Daniel, 143
Rehg, Kenneth, 24, 38, 82, 98
Index
retraction, 1, 11, 12, 16-18, 33-40, 42-48, 5153, 55, 56, 58, 59, 77, 81, 85, 91-94, 96100, 103, 105, 106, 112-114, 128, 167, 172,
173, 179-185, 187-189, 192, 197, 200-203,
208
rhotic, 16, 24, 26, 27, 29, 48, 81, 89, 91, 93,
102, 106, 108, 111, 112, 120, 126-128, 168,
173-179
Rice, Keren, 82, 143
Ritharngu, 123, 124
Rubach, Jerzy, 24, 37, 41, 98, 139, 140, 142
Rumsey, Alan, 98, 112, 115
Russian, 2, 6, 24, 32, 33, 36, 41-43, 45, 46, 50,
51, 78, 84, 95, 99, 102, 106, 107, 180, 182,
189, 190, 206
Sagey, Elizabeth, 1, 2, 34, 137, 139, 142
Sakthivel, Subbiah, 48, 125-127
Sanskrit, 3, 86, 87, 107-111, 117, 121-124,
140, 141, 154, 195, 196
Scatton, Ernest, 46
Schiffman, Harold, 83, 117, 120
Scots Gaelic, 48, 102, 190
segment inventory, 14, 15, 24, 29-33, 49, 51,
192
Serbian, 52
Serrano, 27
Shadle, Christine, 55, 58, 60, 63, 64, 67-69
Shalev, Michael, 30, 32, 33, 39, 44, 117, 136
Sharf, Donald J., 12
Sharpe, Margaret, 116
Shona, 26
Simonsen, Hanne, 1, 15, 16, 29, 31, 199, 207
Sindhi, 26, 86, 88
Sinhala, 91, 93
Siwu, 29
Skjærvø, Prods, 86
Slavic languages, 6, 12, 24, 32, 41
South-American languages, 27
Spajic, Sinisa, 27, 39, 45, 48-50, 102
Spencer, Andrew, 37, 42
Spiti, 87
Sridhar, S.N., 21
Sri Lankan Portuguese Creole, 91, 93
Steriade, Donca, 1, 2, 5, 72, 115, 116, 118,
123, 124, 133, 134, 144, 148, 151-154, 192,
197
Stevens, Kenneth, 5, 36-38, 53- 60, 62-66, 68,
69, 76, 77, 88, 104, 133, 159, 161
stop, 3, 11, 17, 19-21, 23-28, 30-32, 39, 47
subapical, 13-15, 18, 21, 23, 26-32, 34, 40
sublaminal, 13, 14, 20, 35
227
sublingual cavity, 11-13, 32, 33, 35, 40, 42-47,
51, 53, 55-58, 64, 67, 77, 125, 128, 200,
201
Sundberg, Johan, 35, 55, 57, 69
Sussman, Harvey, 59, 69, 72
Svantesson, Jan-Olof, 85, 87, 89, 99-101, 128
Swedish, 3, 16, 18, 21, 29, 52, 63, 64, 71-73,
83-89, 99, 100, 101, 105, 106, 119, 120,
128, 194
Svarny, O., 16, 19, 26, 28
Tamil, 11, 16, 18-21, 23, 24, 26-28, 30, 31, 34,
39, 60, 69, 71, 72, 86, 93, 97, 100-102, 106,
107, 112, 117, 120, 122-124, 181, 195
Tarascan, 27
Telugu, 26, 28, 120, 121, 123, 124
Tharp, George, 82
Tlingit, 25
Toda, 21, 23, 24, 27, 30, 31, 39, 44, 45, 48-52,
53, 55, 59, 60, 64, 66-69, 72, 97, 102, 106,
117, 125, 126, 128, 136, 142, 165, 180, 183,
190, 191, 206, 207
Tolitoli, 92, 93
tongue
back, 1, 13, 16, 36, 37, 39, 44, 45, 58, 92,
93, 103, 113, 140, 163-165, 171, 172,
178-181, 195, 200
body, 1, 13, 17, 21, 23, 28, 36-39, 43, 44,
53, 55, 59, 78, 92, 93, 139-141, 147,
148, 150, 163, 189
dorsum, 12, 13, 36-39, 45, 143, 163, 203
middle, 13, 14, 16, 32, 34, 37, 44, 45, 47,
65, 103, 113, 163, 164, 171-173, 179181, 187, 200, 203
tip, 11-13, 15-21, 23, 26-29, 31-35, 38, 40,
42, 43
Torp, Arne, 84, 85
Trask, Robert, 11, 14, 28, 36
trill, 17, 25-27, 85, 88, 126, 173-177, 183, 126
Trubetzkoy, Nikolai, 134
Tserdanelis, Georgios, 92
Tungus languages, 93
Tuttle, Siri, 25
Universal Grammar, 4, 8
Upper Kuskokwim, 25
Uradhi, 91, 93
uvular, 15
Van de Velde, Hans, 29, 87
Vanvik, Arne, 83
velar, 13, 27, 38, 50
228
velarization, 6, 36-39, 43, 45, 46, 50, 81, 111,
125, 129
Vennemann, Theo, 108-111
Verstraeten, Bart, 29
Wakelin, M.F., 27, 87, 112, 121
Wakh, 86
Walley, A.C., 54
Walsh Dickey, Laura, 138
Wängler, Hans, 21
Ward, Dennis, 43
Wargamay, 90, 91
Warlpiri, 26, 31
Warner, Natasha, 72
Watjarri, 85, 86, 115
Wells, John, 27
Wembawemba, 15, 98, 101, 104-106, 146,
147, 188
Wergaia, 101, 104-106, 146, 147, 188
West African languages, 16, 19, 20
Western Arrernte, 29
Western Desert, 31
Western Torres Strait, 30
Whitney, William, 107-109, 121, 122, 124
Wierzchowska, Boz·ena, 24, 39, 41, 42
Wiese, Richard, 29
Wilkinson, Karina, 38
Williamson, Kay, 34
Winitz, H., 54
Wood, Sidney, 38
Wu, Zongji, 7, 23, 24, 65
Yanyuwa, 55
Yaraldi, 55
Yidgha, 86, 88, 175, 176
Yip, Moira, 96
Zoll, Cheryl, 169
Zvelebil, Kamil, 16, 19, 26, 28, 86, 97, 100102, 112, 117, 119, 120, 122, 123
Index