Genetics of dyslexia: the evolving landscape
Johannes Schumacher, Per Hoffmann, Christine Schmäl,Gerd Schulte-Körne, Markus M Nöthen
....................................................................................................................................................................................... J Med Genet 2007;44:289–297.
Dyslexia is among the most common neurodevelopmental
disorders, with a prevalence of 5–12%. At the phenotypic
level, various cognitive components that enable reading and
spelling and that are disturbed in affected individuals can be
distinguished. Depending on the phenotype dimension
investigated, inherited factors are estimated to account for up
to 80%. Linkage findings in dyslexia are relatively consistent
across studies in comparison to findings for other
neuropsychiatric disorders. This is particularly true for
chromosome regions 1p34–p36, 6p21–p22, 15q21 and 18q11.
Four candidate genes have recently been identified through
systematic linkage disequilibrium studies in linkage region
6p21–p22, and through cloning approaches at chromosomal
breakpoints. Results indicate that a disturbance in neuronal
migration is a pathological correlate of dyslexia at the
functional level. This review presents a summary of the latest
insights into the genetics of dyslexia and an overview of
anticipated future developments.
Familial clustering in dyslexia was recognised a few
years after the first description of the disorder by
Hinshelwood in 1895. A child with an affected parent
has a risk of 40–60% of developing dyslexia. This risk
is increased when other family members are also
affected. There is an estimated 3–10-fold increase in
the relative risk for a sibling (ls), with an increase in ls
observed when strict criteria are applied. Twin studies
have confirmed that genetic factors are substantially
responsible for the familial clustering of dyslexia. The
proportion of inherited factors involved in the
development of dyslexia is between 40% and 80%, the
highest estimates being reported for the phenotype
dimensions word reading (up to 58%) and spelling
(70%). Twin studies have allowed for the estimation
of heritabilities and also the impact of shared and nonshared environmental factors. Although shared
environmental effects are low for word reading, they
are substantially higher (at about 14%) for reading and
spelling correlated traits—for example, phonological
awareness.
.............................................................................
Whether or not sex has an influence on heritability is
controversial. Although the results of a US American
twin study (Colorado Twin Study) showed similar
heritability between the
28 29
sexes,Harlaar et al found a higher heritability for boys
in a UK sample (London Twins Early Development
Study).
Through molecular genetic linkage studies in families
with dyslexia, chromosome regions have been
identified in which the presence of dyslexia
susceptibility genes is suspected. As with all complex
disorders, linkage findings are not completely
overlapping between independent studies. However,
greater consistency is reported for dyslexia than for
most other neuropsychiatric disorders, and the
identification of the first candidate genes therefore
came as no surprise.
This review presents the current state of molecular
genetic research on dyslexia, including discussion of
the phenotypic aspects and neuropsychological
concepts of dyslexia that have received increasing
consideration in genetic research over recent years.
Finally, the extent to which our understanding of
dyslexia is likely to be increased through the results of
current and future molecular genetic research is
discussed.
yslexia is among the most common neurodevelopmental disorders, with a prevalence of 5–12%.
The prevalence varies with the use of different
diagnostic criteria and, since reading and spelling are
normally distributed in the population, is influenced by
the cut-off point applied to the psychometric tests.
According to the International Classification of
Diseases-10, dyslexia is ‘‘a disorder manifested by
difficulty learning to read despite conventional
instruction, adequate intelligence and sociocultural
opportunity’’. Longitudinal studies have shown that the
disorder involves an extremely stable developmental
disturbance that does not, in contrast to popular
opinion, disappear with adolescence. The psychosocial
consequences are correspondingly grave. Affected
individuals attain a much lower educational level and
have substantially higher rates of unemployment and
psychosocial stress than would be expected for their
level of intelligence. In childhood, approximately 20%
of those with dyslexia also present with attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), whereas in
adolescence depressive disorders and disorders of
social behaviour are often associated with dyslexia.
Whether dyslexia is more common among boys than
girls has been part of a controversial discussion in the
past, although recent epidemiological studies indicate a
twofold increase in the risk for boys compared with
that in girls. The sex ratio may be influenced by
severity, IQ and assessed cognitive profiles.
12
3
4
See end of article for
authors’ affiliations
........................
Correspondence to:
Professor M M No¨then,
Department of Genomics,
Life & Brain Centre,
University of Bonn,
Sigmund-Freud-Strasse
25, D-53105 Bonn,
Germany;
markus.noethen@ unibonn.de
Received 20 September
2006 Revised 13 January
2007 Accepted 22 January
2007
PublishedOnlineFirst
16February2007
........................
5–7
8–12
13–15
216
17
18–20
19 21–25
25
17 26
17 26 27
27
30
Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder; CNS, central nervous system; DCDC2,
doublecortin domain containing protein 2; DYX1,
dyslexia susceptibility 1; DYX9, dyslexia susceptibility 9;
DYX1C1, dyslexia susceptibility 1 candidate 1; LD,
linkage disequilibrium; QTL, quantitative trait loci;
ROBO1, roundabout Drosophila homolog of 1; SSD,
speech–sound disorder
Downloaded from jmg.bmj.com on 14 April 2008
Schumacher, Hoffmann, Schma¨l, et al
impaired sensitivity of cells within the retinocortical magnocellular
pathway and in the extrastriate areas in the dorsal stream to which
they project. The cerebellar deficit theory suggests that the
automatisation of cognitive processes and motor control in the
cerebellum are disturbed in individuals with dyslexia. The double
deficit hypothesis, which assumes disturbances in phonological
processing and the speed of processing, should also be mentioned in
this context.
Even though evidence for one or the other of these theories is
typically reported in affected individuals, there is no evidence so far
for specific subgroups of dyslexia. A reason for this could be that
although some of the deficits found in affected individuals are
correlated with reading and spelling, they may not be causally
associated with dyslexia. Findings from genetic research may have
the potential to help delineate which cognitive and
neurophysiological processes are causally related.
43
44
45
LINKAGE FINDINGS IN DYSLEXIA
To date, linkage analyses in families with dyslexia have identified
nine chromosome regions (dyslexia susceptibility 1(DYX1)–dyslexia
susceptibility 9 (DYX9)) listed by the HUGO Gene Nomenclature
Committee in which the presence of susceptibility genes is suspected
(table 2). There was initially great hope that it would be possible to
correlate the respective cognitive components of dyslexia (table 1)
with specific linkage regions. Many studies accordingly investigated
individual phenotype components as categorical or quantitative
(quantitative trait loci (QTL)) subdimensions, and linkages with
specific chromosomal regions have been claimed; unfortunately, with
little support from independent studies so far. Nevertheless, the
consistency of linkage findings is impressive in comparison to those
for other neuropsychiatric disorders. This is particularly true of
findings in chromosome regions 1p34–p36, 6p21–p22, 15q21 and
18q11, with support for each of these regions coming from the
investigation of at least two large family samples.
PHENOTYPIC ASPECTS AND NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL
THEORIES
In general, the cognitive processes on which reading and spelling are
based are complex, and differing cognitive dimensions ease the
separate skills of reading and spelling. Such processes include those
of short-term memory, phonological awareness, rapid naming, and
phonological and orthographic coding (table 1). In recent years,
several theories have been developed with the aim of characterising
the basic processes underlying dyslexia. These have taken into consideration the increasing body of knowledge obtained from
neurophysiological and imaging research (eg, event-related
potentials, functional MRI). The phonological deficit theory, which
assumes a disturbance in phonological processing, is currently the
most salient theory. According to this theory, affected individuals
have difficulties in perceiving and segmenting phonemes, leading to
difficulties in establishing a connection between phonemes and
graphemes. The rapid auditory processing theory is another theory
that proposes that phonological deficits are secondary to an auditory
deficit in the perception of short or rapidly varying sounds. Many
individuals with dyslexia perform poorly on auditory tasks including
frequency discrimination and temporal order judgement. Abnormal
neurophysiological responses to auditory stimuli have also been
reported. However, individuals with dyslexia also have visual
perceptual deficits which these theories cannot adequately explain.
The magnocellular theory accounts for disturbances in visual
processing. The theory proposes that in a proportion of individuals
with dyslexia, the perception of visual, rapid moving stimuli and
stimuli of low spatial frequency and low contrast is impaired. This
deficit is associated at the central nervous system (CNS) level with
32
33
34 35
36–38
39–42
36
The largest family samples reported in the literature are from the
USA (Colorado, Seattle and Yale samples), the UK (Cardiff and
Oxford samples), Canada (Toronto and Vancouver samples) and
Germany (German sample). For the sake of clarity, these samples
will be named according to their origin in the following sections.
Results from genomewide linkage studies
66 67
have been reported so far from the Seattle, Oxford and Colorado
samples. In addition, genomewide linkage studies of large multiply
affected families from Holland, Norway and Finland have been
reported. The following section presents results for the individual
regions, and discussion is limited to positive findings only.
49
53
65
56
58 60
DYX1—chromosome 15q21
DYX1 (MIM 127700) lies in chromosome region 15q21, and a total
of four research groups have reported linkage in their family samples
(table 2).
Evidence for linkage was found for word reading and
related phenotype dimensions in three samples (Colorado, Yale and
Seattle samples), and one sample showed evidence of linkage for
spelling (German sample). Two linkage disequilibrium (LD) studies
have been carried out in region DYX1 using short tandem repeat
markers, and positive evidence for association was obtained for one
region of approximately 4 Mb. In both studies, a three-marker
haplotype was associated in a total of three independent trio-samples,
two samples of British origin (Cardiff sample) and one of Italian
origin. Region 15q21 has also shown evidence of linkage to ADHD.
A genome scan carried out in 164 Dutch sib pairs with ADHD
showed the strongest evidence for linkage in this region. The riskconferring gene in DYX1 may contribute to the comorbidity reported
between the two disorders.
46–48 49
46 47 49
48
68 69
68 69
70
DYX2— chromosome 6p21–p22
The chromosome region 6p21–p22 (DYX2, MIM 600202) is
considered to be among the best-replicated regions of linkage for
dyslexia (table 2). Evidence of linkage has been reported using a
QTL approach in both a US-American (Colorado) and a UK
Positive evidence for linkage was also reported
(Oxford) sample.
from a US-American subsample (Yale sample) in which categorical
phenotype dimensions had been considered. A more precise
containment of the phenotype subdimensions associated with DYX2
was not possible. Linkage was found
51–54
with the phenotypes phonological processing and ortho
54 55
graphic processing. Meanwhile, LD mapping in DYX2 led to the
identification of two strong candidate genes (DCDC2 (doublecortin
domain containing protein 2) and K1AA0319). Interestingly,
evidence for linkage has also been found in the chromosome region
6p21–p22 for ADHD.
53 50–52 54
55
51 52
71–75
76
DYX3—chromosome 2p15–p16
The chromosome region 2p15–p16 (DYX3, MIM 604254)
has been identified through linkage analyses in five family samples
The
(including the Oxford, Colorado and Vancouver samples;
linkage peaks of the individual studies lie far apart from each other,
however, and so it is not clear whether they indicate the same
susceptibility locus. As with DYX2, no phenotype dimension has
been found to be specifically linked with this locus, although not all
studies have analysed subdimensions.
53 56–58 77
DYX4—chromosome 6q11–q12
The chromosome region 6q11–q12 (DYX4, MIM 127700) was
identified in the context of a chromosome-wide linkage study of a
large Canadian family sample (Vancouver sample; table 2). The
most strongly linked phenotype dimensions
59
Schumacher, Hoffmann, Schma¨l, et al
were phonological coding and spelling. There has so far been no
independent replication of this finding for DYX4.
DYX5—chromosome 3p12–q13
The chromosome region 3p12–q13 (DYX5, MIM 606 896) showed
linkage in a large Finnish family (table 2). ROBO1 (roundabout
Drosophila homolog of 1) has been identified as a possible candidate
gene in this region. DYX5 also showed a positive evidence for
linkage in 77 US-American families with speech–sound disorder
(SSD). SSD involves impairments in phonological processing, as
with dyslexia.
60
genes have been replicated in independent samples, with the
strongest findings being reported among severely affected
individuals. By contrast, the genes DYX1C1 (dyslexia susceptibility
1 candidate 1) and ROBO1, which were identified through
breakpoint mapping in Finnish patients, seem to be less involved in
the development of dyslexia across different populations. Their
contribution may be limited to a few families in the Finnish
population.
78
DYX6—chromosome 18p11
DYX6 (MIM 606616), which lies in chromosome region 18p11, was
identified in two independent family samples (Oxford and Colorado
samples) through a genome scan applying a QTL approach (table 2).
The strongest evidence for linkage was found for word reading. This
finding was replicated in a third family sample (expanded Oxford
sample), the strongest evidence for linkage being found for the
phenotype subdimension phoneme awareness. The results of a
subsequent multivariate analysis in the two Oxford samples indicate
that a QTL in DYX6 influences multiple aspects of reading ability
and is not correlated with specific phenotype subdimensions.
53
53
79
DYX7—chromosome 11p15
Linkage with markers in the region of DYX7 (MIM 127700), which
lies in chromosome region 11p15, has been described only in one
family sample to date (Vancouver sample; table 2). The authors
selected DYX7 as a candidate region on the basis that the gene for
the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) is localised there. DRD4 is a
possible risk gene for ADHD.
DCDC2 (doublecortin domain containing protein 2)
Initial evidence for the involvement of DCDC2 (MIM 605755) and
dyslexia was obtained through gene-based LD mapping in a genedense 680 kb section of linkage region 6p22 (DXY2; table 3). The
sample was drawn from 114 US-American nuclear families of
predominantly European origin (Colorado sample). Positive evidence
for association was found in two genome loci, in which a total of six
genes
were
localised:
VMP/
DCDC2/KAAG1
and
K1AA0319/TTRAP/THEM2. In a subsequently expanded Colorado
sample (153 nuclear families), the strongest evidence for association
was found in DCDC2 (table 3). Additionally, a deletion of 2.4 kb in
intron 2 of DCDC2, which encodes tandem repeats of putative brainassociated transcription factor binding sites, was identified, which
had an allele frequency of 8.5% in the parents. The tandem repeats in
the deleted region demonstrate several alleles. For the purposes of the
association study, the authors combined the deletion and the rare
repeat alleles into one allele, for which they reported a strong
association with reading performance.
72
74
74
61
80
DYX8—chromosome 1p34–1p36
Three research groups in total have reported linkage between DYX8
(MIM 608995) in chromosome region 1p34–p36 and dyslexia
(including the Yale and Vancouver samples; table 2). Even though
individual studies have shown linkage to differing phenotype
subdimensions of dyslexia, linkage evidence from two studies was
particularly strong when focus was placed on the phonological
aspects of dyslexia.
62–64
63 64
DYX9—chromosome Xq26–q27
Evidence for linkage was found in chromosome region Xq27
(DYX9, MIM 300509) in a Dutch multiplex family with dyslexia
(table 2). The same research group failed to replicate their result in
67 affected sib pairs. However, positive evidence for linkage was
found in region DYX9 in one of the UK samples (Oxford sample;
table 2).
65
53
Additional linkage regions in dyslexia
In addition to the HGNC-listed DYX1–DYX9 regions, linkage with
dyslexia has also been reported for other regions, although without
replication in independent samples. This includes evidence for
linkage on chromosome 13q12 for word reading, and on
chromosome 2q22 for phonological decoding efficiency. Two further
studies have been conducted which aimed to identify chromosomal
loci with pleiotropic effects on dyslexia and ADHD. In the Colorado
sample, families with dyslexia having ADHD problems showed
evidence for linkage in chromosome regions 14q32, 13q32 and
20q11. In families with ADHD, evidence for linkage is shown for
reading ability in regions 10q11, 16p12 and 17q22.
66
67
81
82
Findings from two trio-samples also indicate the involvement of
DCDC2 in the development of dyslexia (German sample; table 3).
Strong evidence for association was shown in both samples at the
single-marker and haplotype level. This effect seemed to be
particularly substantial in severely affected individuals. In the pooled
sample, severely affected individuals showed a genotypic relative
risk of 4.88 on the basis of the homozygous presence of the identified
risk haplotype.
By contrast, investigation of the DCDC2 locus in the two UK
samples (Oxford and Cardiff) had inconsistent results. In the Oxford
sample, evidence of association between DCDC2 variants and
various phenotype components of dyslexia were found, albeit with a
weak level of significance. This association disappeared, however,
when only severely affected cases were included in the analysis.
Interestingly, the 2.4 kb deletion in intron 2 of DCDC2 was more
common than by chance in severely affected patients. There was no
association between dyslexia and DCDC2 in the Cardiff sample.
Joint analysis of the two samples, however, produced evidence of a
possible interaction between DCDC2 and K1AA0319.
75
90
In summary, these results suggest that DCDC2 is involved in the
development of dyslexia. It is unlikely that KAAG1 is the
susceptibility gene at this locus. KAAG1 overlaps at the genomic
level with exon 1 of DCDC2, although KAAG1 does not seem to be
expressed in the CNS. By contrast, DCDC2 is widely expressed in
the CNS, including areas of the brain in which lower activation
patterns have been observed in individuals with dyslexia, such as the
inferior temporal and medial temporal cortices.
74
74 75 91–93
Functionally, DCDC2 is involved in processes of cortical neuronal
migration during brain development and contains a double cortin
homology domain which is typical of this. RNA interference studies
of in utero rats have shown that downregulation of DCDC2 leads to a
significant reduction in neuronal migration. Determining whether the
intron 2 deletion is one of the responsible variants will require further
investigation in larger samples. There is no real rationale for
combining the deletion with rare alleles of the STR polymorphism.
Functional studies of the possible effect of the different
74
CANDIDATEGENEFINDINGSIN DYSLEXIA
Of the newly identified candidate genes, DCDC2 and K1AA0319
seem to be of most significance for dyslexia. Both were identified
through systematic investigation of LD (LD mapping) within DYX2
on chromosome 6p22. Initial findings for both
alleles on expression or splicing are required to justify the THEM2 gene cluster (MIM 609269) was reported in the combining of alleles.
Colorado sample. Association for the same gene cluster was
reported by Francks et al in two independent samples (Oxford KIAA0319 samples),
which was particularly notable in severely affected Besides evidence for association in the region of DCDC2, positive individuals (table 3).
Association in this region was replicated association with variants in the region of the K1AA0319/TTRAP/ in a third UK sample (Cardiff
sample; table 3). There was an
72
73
71
www.jmedgenet.com
Schumacher, Hoffmann, Schma¨l, et al
association with SNPs in the region of KIAA0319 through the use of
a DNA pooling screening step and subsequent replication through
individual genotyping.
Meanwhile, further analyses of the two samples (Oxford and Cardiff)
have shown that the responsible gene variant(s) is (are) probably
localised near exon 1 of K1AA0319. Investigation of both UK
samples has resulted in evidence of a gene–gene interaction between
K1AA0319 and DCDC2.
One sample (German sample), which had reported strong association
with DCDC2, has so far produced no convincing evidence for
association with the KIAA0319/TTRAP/THEM2 gene cluster. There
was no further evidence of association at the K1AA0319 locus from
the extended Colorado sample (153 nuclear families), although the
genomic segment that had shown the strongest association findings in
the two UK samples was insufficiently analysed.
90
75
74
The evidence of association for K1AA0319 obtained from
independent samples is convincing. As with DCDC2, involvement of
the KIAA0319 locus seems to be particularly marked in severely
affected cases. Association findings, which were strongest around
KIAA0319, and results from gene expression and functional studies
suggest that KIAA0319 is the most likely susceptibility gene for
dyslexia in this gene cluster. Allelespecific expression analyses in
lymphoblastoid cells have shown that carriers of the risk-associated
haplotype have a 40% reduction in the expression of KIAA0319,
whereas the expression of other genes in this region remains
unaffected. The expression of KIAA0319 is particularly strong in the
cerebral neocortex of developing mouse and human brain tissue, and,
similar to DCDC2, reduced expression of KIAA0319 through RNA
interference leads to disturbed neuronal migration in rats in utero.
94
94
DYX1C1 lies outside of the linkage peaks. Whether or not DYX1C1
contributes to dyslexia in the Finnish population requires clarification
through larger association studies.
ROBO1
As with DYX1C1, the identification of ROBO1 (MIM 602430) was
achieved through breakpoint mapping of a translocation. A
translocation, which had probably occurred de novo, was diagnosed
in an affected individual from Finland t(3;8)(p12:q11). ROBO1 was
interrupted through the translocation breakpoint, localised in linkage
region 3p12 (DYX5). A rare ROBO1 haplotype was identified in the
Finnish family, in which the original linkage finding for DYX5 had
been found, and cosegregation of this haplotype with dyslexia was
reported. Lymphocyte investigation of four affected family members
showed that expression of the risk haploytpe was reduced.
Investigation of the orthologous gene in Drosophila (robo) and mice
(Robo1) suggests that ROBO1 functions as a neuronal axon guidance
gene involved in brain development.
97
97
98–100
Whether or not ROBO1 actually contributes to the development of
dyslexia is currently not clear. A critical point is that the connection
between the translocation and dyslexia in the original translocation
patient was not imperative: A sibling of the translocation carrier also
had dyslexia without carrying the translocation. Should the dyslexia
of the Finnish multiplex family be based on a rare and highly
penetrant mutation, the causal variant will not be easy to identify,
given its size (990 kb of genomic DNA) and the difficulties involved
in separating the effects of individual variants from the background
variation characterising the haplotype.
CONCLUSIONS
DYX1C1
DYX1C1 (MIM 608706) was cloned in a two-generation Finnish
family with a translocation t(2;15)(q11;q21). DYX1C1, which lies in
chromosome region 15q21, is interrupted through the translocation
breakpoint. All four family members in whom the translocation was
detected showed reading-associated problems. To determine whether
DYX1C1 is of significance for affected cases outside of this family, a
polymorphism discovery approach was used in 20 Finnish
individuals with dyslexia. A total of eight SNPs were identified,
which were then investigated in affected individuals and controls of
Finnish origin. In an initial sample, two SNPs were found to be
associated in the single-marker and haplotype analysis. Replication
was then achieved for one of the two variants in a second sample
(table 3). However, the sample sizes were limited, and a proportion
of the affected individuals in the initial sample were related to each
other.
83
95
83
DYX1C1 is expressed in many tissues, including those of the CNS,
where it is found in cortical neurones and white matter glial cells.
Interestingly, it has recently been shown that DYX1C1, similar to
KIAA0319 and DCDC2, functions in neuronal migration in rodent
neocortex.
Six other association analyses using independent samples of
predominantly European origin have been carried out to date
(including the Oxford, Cardiff, Colorado and Toronto samples).
Overall, the results must be viewed as being negative, since the initial
findings have not been replicated. Positive findings have been
reported from two of these studies, although the association was with
the opposite two-marker haplotype (Oxford and Toronto samples;
table 3). Given this failure to replicate, it is unlikely that DYX1C1
makes a significant contribution to the development of dyslexia in
non-Finnish European populations.
83
96
84–89
84 85
It is highly probable that the linkage findings in chromosome region
15q21 (DYX1) cannot be traced back to DYX1C1, since
Of the candidate genes discussed to date, the evidence for DCDC2
and KIAA0319 is the most convincing. Their identification
represents an important step in our understanding of the molecular
processes that lead to dyslexia. However, many outstanding
questions will need to be addressed by future studies. It is necessary
to clarify whether populationspecific genetic heterogeneity and/or
phenotypic differences between samples have led to differing
findings for the respective loci. Identifying which of the genetic
changes in these candidate genes are causal is also important. The
lack of associated variants in the coding regions suggests that it is
variants influencing generegulation and expression which are
responsible.
The nature of the genes identified to date suggests that a disturbance
in cortical neurone migration and reduced activity in left-hemispheric
brain regions are pathophysiological correlates of dyslexia. With
DCDC2, as with KIAA0319, inhibition leads to poorer neuronal
migration in the neocortex of fetal rats through specific small
interfering RNAs. This concept of disturbed neuronal migration is
also supported by the few results available from postmortem brain
studies of affected individuals, which report cortical malformations
in the region of the perisylvian cortex. The concept of disturbed
neuronal migration in dyslexia is intriguing and will stimulate further
research in this area. In view of the fact that DCDC2 and K1AA0319
only contribute a limited part to the development of dyslexia and that
most susceptibility genes are still unknown, it may be possible in the
future to identify completely new pathophysiological mechanisms.
74 94
101–103
To date, no specific cognitive processes are known to be influenced
by the proposed susceptibility genes. Some studies have already
started to include neurophysiological (eg, eventrelated potential) and
imaging (eg, functional MRI) procedures in their phenotype
characterisation of patients. Such samples are an important
prerequisite for the identification of those
processes that are most proximal to the effects of particular genes and
their associated biological pathways.
Through the availability of detailed clinical data, it should be
possible to associate special phenotype dimensions of dyslexia with
specific risk genes (genotype–phenotype association). Phenotype
subdimensions are, of course, correlated with each other, and the
effects will not affect isolated subdimensions. Nor is it to be expected
that specific genes will affect the whole spectrum of phenotype
dimensions equally. Studies have not yet managed to establish
genotype–phenotype relations convincingly, although samples may
have been too small to demonstrate these effects. However, proof of
genotype–phenotype associations could be facilitated through the
joint analysis of larger samples and the identification of causative
variants.
The molecular genetic studies conducted so far have not considered
sex-specific genetic effects. Differing prevalence rates between males
and females could be suggestive of a sexspecific geneeffect. A
satisfactory power to detect such effects can be provided only when
sex is taken into account during the analysis of results, and this
should be a feature of future studies.
66
Identification of susceptibility genes will allow research into the
molecular background of clinically observed comorbidity. Eight loci
have already been proposed as having pleiotropic effects on dyslexia
The identification of
and ADHD at a linkage level.
susceptibility genes also allows examination of the extent to which
dyslexia-associated disorders, such as SSD and language impairment,
are influenced by the same susceptibility genes. For SSD,
overlapping linkage evidence in DYX5 already provides the first
concrete evidence of such common gene effects.
46–54
70
76
81
82
60 78
The identification of susceptibility genes will enable the analysis of
gene–gene interactions, through which epistatic effects can be
discovered. A first example of this might be the proposed interaction
between DCDC2 and KIAA0319. A further aim of future research
will be to establish a better understanding of gene–environment
interactions in order to identify relevant exogenous risk factors. It has
long been recognised that environmental factors are of great
relevance to the development of dyslexia, but only some of these
factors have been identified so far. If such factors can be modulated,
future dyslexia prevention and individual genetic risk profiling could
be envisaged.
90
104
The genes that accompany the development of dyslexia are naturally
of great interest from an evolutionary perspective. Through the
identification of the gene at the DNA level, comparison with species
that are closely related to us but that do not have the same speech
capacity could be carried out, as well as examination of sequence
variability between humans. Speech-associated genes may have been
under a selection pressure, which proved advantageous for the
development of modern man.
105
As is generally the case with research on complex genetic disorders,
it can be assumed that the speed by which susceptibility genes are
identified will be increased through increasing knowledge and huge
technological advances (eg, genomewide association studies). Future
research efforts will be of a collaborative nature, drawing on
complementary expertise from various scientific disciplines and
involving the combining of large samples, an approach exemplified
by the large multidisciplinary European research consortium
(www.neurodys.com) which integrates the work of research groups
from nine countries.
.......................
Authors’ affiliations Johannes Schumacher, Institute of Human
Genetics, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany
Per Hoffmann, Markus M No¨then, Department of Genomics, Life &
Brain Centre, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany Christine Schma¨l,
Division of Genetic Epidemiology in Psychiatry, Central Institute of
Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany Gerd Schulte-Ko¨rne, Department
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic
Medicine, University of Munich, Munich, Germany
Funding: This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft.
Competing interests: None.
REFERENCES
1
Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Barbaresi WJ, et al. Incidence of reading
disability in a population-based birth cohort, 1976–1982, Rochester, Minn. Mayo
Clin Proc 2001;76:1081–92.
2
Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Fletcher JM, et al. Prevalence of
reading-disability in boys and girls—results of the Connecticut longitudinalstudy. JAMA 1990;264:998–1002.
3
World Health Organization. The ICD-10 classification of mental and
behavioural disorders: diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: World Health
Organization, 1993.
4
Shaywitz SE, Fletcher JM, Holahan JM, et al. Persistence of
dyslexia: the Connecticut Longitudinal Study at adolescence. Pediatrics
5
Bruck M. Outcomes of adults with childhood histories of dyslexia.
1999;104:1351–9.
In: Hulme C, Joshi RM, eds. Reading and spelling:development and disorders.
Mahwah, NJ: L Erlbaum, 1998:179–200.
6
Maughan B. Annotation: long-term outcomes of
developmental reading problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
1995;36:357–71.
7
Strehlow U, Kluge R, Mo¨ller H, et al. Long-term course of dyslexia
beyond the school years: catamnesis from pediatric psychiatric ambulatory
care. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr 1992;20:254–65.
8
August GJ, Garfinkel BD. Comorbidity of ADHD and reading
disability among clinic-referred children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1990;18:29–
45.
9
Kaplan BJ, Dewey DM, Crawford SG, et al. The term comorbidity
is of questionable value in reference to developmental disorders: data and
theory. J Learn Disabil 2001;34:555–65.
10
Purvis KL, Tannock R. Language abilities in children with
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, reading disabilities, and normal
controls. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1997;25:133–44.
11
Shaywitz SE. Dyslexia. N Engl J Med 1998;338:307–12.
12
Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, DeFries JC. Twin study of the
etiology of comorbidity between reading disability and attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet 2000;96:293–301.
13
Frauenheim JG, Heckerl JR. A longitudinal study of
psychological and achievement test performance in severe dyslexic
adults. J Learn Disabil 1983;16:339–47.
14
Naylor CF, Felton RH, Wood FB. Adult outcome in developmental
dyslexia. In: Pavlidis G, eds. Perspectives on dyslexia:cognition, language and
treatment. Vol 2. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons, 1990:29.
15
Schulte-Ko¨rne G, Deimel W, Remschmidt H. Diagnosis of reading
and spelling disorder. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother 2001;29:113–16.
16
Rutter M, Caspi A, Fergusson D, et al. Sex differences in
developmental reading disability—new findings from 4 epidemiological studies.
JAMA 2004;291:2007–12.
17
Olson RK. Dyslexia: nature and nurture. Dyslexia 2002;8:143–59.
18
Hinshelwood J. Word—blindness and visual
memory. Lancet 1895;146:1564–70.
19
Stephenson S. Six cases of congenital word-blindness
affecting three generations of one family. Ophthalmoscope
1907;5:482–4.
20
Thomas CJ. Congenital word blindness and its treatment.
Ophthalmoscope 1905;3:380–5.
21
Hallgren B. Specific dyslexia (congenital word-blindness); a clinical
and genetic study. Acta Neurol Scand (Suppl), 1950;65:1–287.
22
Olson RK, Forsberg H, Wise B. Genes, environment, and
development of orthographic skills. In: Berninger VW, eds. The varieties of
orthographic knowledge I:theoretical and developmental issues. Dordrecht,
Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1994:27–71.
23
Schulte-Ko¨rne G, Deimel W, Mu¨ller K, et al. Familial aggregation
of spelling disability. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996;37:817–22.
24
Stevenson J. Which aspects of processing text mediate geneticeffects. Read Writ 1991;3:249–69.
25
Ziegler A,Ko¨nig IR, Deimel W, et al. Developmental dyslexia—
recurrence risk estimates from a German bi-center study using the single
proband sib pair design. Hum Hered 2005;59:136–43.
26
Plomin R, Kovas Y. Generalist genes and learning disabilities.
Psychol Bull 2005;131:592–617.
27
Gaya´n J, Olson RK. Genetic and environmental influences on
orthographic and phonological skills in children with reading disabilities. Dev
Neuropsychol 2001;20:483–507.
28
Hawke JL, Wadsworth SJ, DeFries JC. Genetic influences on
reading difficulties in boys and girls: the Colorado twin study. Dyslexia
2006;12:21–9.
29
Wadsworth SJ, Knopik VS, DeFries JC. Reading disability in boys
and girls: no evidence for a differential genetic etiology. Read Writ 2000;13:133–
45.
Schumacher, Hoffmann, Schma¨l, et al
30
Harlaar N, Spinath FM, Dale PS, et al. Genetic influences on early
word recognition abilities and disabilities: a study of 7-year-old twins. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 2005;46:373–84.
31
Goswami U, Bryant P. Phonological skills and learning to read.
Hillsdale, NJ: L Erlbaum, 1990.
32
Ramus F, Rosen S, Dakin SC, et al. Theories of developmental
dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain
2003;126:841–65.
33
Tallal P. Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading
disabilities in children. Brain Lang 1980;9:182–98.
34
Ahissar M, Protopapas A, Reid M, et al. Auditory processing
parallels reading abilities in adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97:6832–7.
35
McAnally KI, Stein JF. Auditory temporal coding in dyslexia.
Proc Biol Sci 1996;263:961–5.
36
Nagarajan S, Mahncke H, Salz T, et al. Cortical auditory signal
processing in poor readers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1999;96:6483–8.
37
Kujala T, Myllyviita K, Tervaniemi M, et al. Basic auditory
dysfunction in dyslexia as demonstrated by brain activity measurements.
Psychophysiology 2000;37:262–6.
38
Schulte-Ko¨rne G, Deimel W, Bartling J, et al. Pre-attentive
processing of auditory patterns in dyslexic human subjects. Neurosci Lett
39
Lovegrove WJ, Bowling A, Badcock D, et al. Specific reading
1999;276:41–4.
disability: differences in contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency.
Science 1980;210:439–40.
40
Schulte-Ko¨rne G, Bartling J, Deimel W, et al. Motion-onset VEPs
in dyslexia. Evidence for visual perceptual deficit. Neuroreport 2004;15:1075–8.
41
Stein J, Walsh V. To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of
dyslexia. Trends Neurosci 1997;20:147–52.
42
Talcott JB, Witton C, McLean MF, et al. Dynamic sensory
sensitivity and children’s word decoding skills. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2000;97:2952–7.
43
Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ, Dean P. Developmental dyslexia: the
cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci 2001;24:508–11.
44
Wolf M, Bowers PG. The double-deficit hypothesis for the
developmental dyslexias. J Educ Psychol 1999;91:415–38.
45
Schulte-Ko¨rne G, Zucchelli M, Deimel W, et al. Interrelationship and
familiality of dyslexia related quantitative measures. Ann Hum Genet 2006;70:1–
16.
46
Smith SD, Kimberling WJ, Pennington BF, et al. Specific reading
disability: identification of an inherited form through linkage analysis. Science
1983;219:1345–7.
47
Grigorenko EL, Chang JT. An extension of affected-pedigreemember analyses to triads of relatives. Genet Epidemiol 1997;14:1005–10.
48
Schulte-Ko¨rne G, Grimm T, No¨then MM, et al. Evidence for linkage
of spelling disability to chromosome 15. Am J Hum Genet 1998;63:279–82.
49
Chapman NH, Igo RP, Thomson JB, et al. Linkage analyses of four
regions previously implicated in dyslexia: confirmation of a locus on chromosome
15q. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2004;131:67–75.
50
Cardon LR, Smith SD, Fulker DW, et al. Quantitative trait locus
for reading disability on chromosome 6. Science 1994;266:276–9.
51
Fisher SE, Marlow AJ, Lamb J, et al. A quantitative-trait locus on
chromosome 6p influences different aspects of developmental dyslexia. Am J
Hum Genet 1999;64:146–56.
52
Gaya´n J, Smith SD, Cherny SS, et al. Quantitative-trait locus
for specific language and reading deficits on chromosome 6p. Am J Hum
Genet 1999;64:157–64.
53
Fisher SE, Francks C, Marlow AJ, et al. Independent genomewide scans identify a chromosome 18 quantitative-trait locus influencing
dyslexia. Nat Genet 2002;30:86–91.
54
Kaplan DE, Gayan J, Ahn J, et al. Evidence for linkage and
association with reading disability on 6p21.3-22. Am J Hum Genet
55
Grigorenko EL, Wood FB, Golovyan L, et al. Continuing the search
2002;70:1287–98.
for dyslexia genes on 6p. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet
2003;118:89–98.
56
Fagerheim T, Raeymaekers P, Tonnessen FE, et al. A new
gene (DYX3) for dyslexia is located on chromosome 2. J Med Genet
1999;36:664–9.
57
Petryshen TL, Kaplan BJ, Hughes ML, et al. Supportive evidence for
the DYX3 dyslexia susceptibility gene in Canadian families. J Med Genet
2002;39:125–6.
58
Kaminen N, Hannula-Jouppi K, Kestila¨M, et al. A genome scan for
developmental dyslexia confirms linkage to chromosome 2p11 and suggests a
new locus on 7q32. J Med Genet 2003;40:340–5.
59
Petryshen TL, Kaplan BJ, Fu Liu M, et al. Evidence for a
susceptibility locus on chromosome 6q influencing phonological coding dyslexia.
Am J Med Genet 2001;105:507–17.
60
Nopola-Hemmi J, Myllyluoma B, Haltia T, et al. A dominant gene
for developmental dyslexia on chromosome 3. J Med Genet 2001;38:658–64.
61
Hsiung GY, Kaplan BJ, Petryshen TL, et al. A dyslexia susceptibility
locus (DYX7) linked to dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) region on chromosome
11p15.5. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2004;125:112–19.
62
Rabin M, Wen XL, Hepburn M, et al. Suggestive linkage of
developmental dyslexia to chromosome 1p34-p36. Lancet 1993;342:178.
63
Grigorenko EL, Wood FB, Meyer MS, et al. Linkage studies suggest
a possible locus for developmental dyslexia on chromosome 1p. Am J Med
Genet 2001;105:120–9.
64
Tzenova J, Kaplan BJ, Petryshen TL, et al. Confirmation of a
dyslexia susceptibility locus on chromosome 1p34-p36 in a set of 100
Canadian families. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet
2004;127:117–24.
65
de Kovel CG, Hol FA, Heister JG, et al. Genomewide scan
identifies susceptibility locus for dyslexia on Xq27 in an extended Dutch family.
J Med Genet 2004;41:652–7.
66
Igo RP, Chapman NH, Berninger VW, et al. Genomewide scan for
real-word reading subphenotypes of dyslexia: novel chromosome 13 locus and
genetic complexity. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2006;141:15–27.
67
Raskind WH, Igo RP, Chapman NH, et al. A genome scan in
multigenerational families with dyslexia: identification of a novel locus on
chromosome 2q that contributes to phonological decoding efficiency. Mol
Psychiatry 2005;10:699–711.
68
Marino C, Giorda R, Vanzin L, et al. A locus on 15q15-15qter
influences dyslexia: further support from a transmission/disequilibrium study in an
Italian speaking population. J Med Genet 2004;41:42–6.
69
Morris DW, Robinson L, Turic D, et al. Family-based association
mapping provides evidence for a gene for reading disability on chromosome 15q.
Hum Mol Genet 2000;9:843–8.
70
Bakker SC, van der Meulen EM, Buitelaar JK, et al. A wholegenome scan in 164 Dutch sib pairs with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
suggestive evidence for linkage on chromosomes 7p and 15q. Am J Hum Genet
2003;72:1251–60.
71
Cope N, Harold D, Hill G, et al. Strong evidence that KIAA0319 on
chromosome 6p is a susceptibility gene for developmental dyslexia. Am J Hum
Genet 2005;76:581–91.
72
Deffenbacher KE, Kenyon JB, Hoover DM, et al. Refinement of the
6p21. 3 quantitative trait locus influencing dyslexia: linkage and association
analyses, Hum Genet 2004;115:128–38.
73
Francks C, Paracchini S, Smith SD, et al. A 77-kilobase region of
chromosome 6p22.2 is associated with dyslexia in families from the United
Kingdom and from the United States. Am J Hum Genet 2004;75:1046–58.
74
Meng H, Smith SD, Hager K, et al. DCDC2 is associated with
reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain. Proc Natl
Acad Sci USA 2005;102:17053–8.
75
Schumacher J, Anthoni H, Dahdouh F, et al. Strong genetic
evidence of DCDC2 as a susceptibility gene for dyslexia. Am J Hum Genet
76
2006;78:52–62.
Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Smith SD, et al. Quantitative trait locus
for reading disability on chromosome 6p is pleiotropic for attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet 2002;114:260–8.
77
Fisher SE, DeFries JC. Developmental dyslexia: genetic dissection
of a complex cognitive trait. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3:767–80.
78
Stein CM, Schick JH, Gerry Taylor H, et al. Pleiotropic effects of a
chromosome 3 locus on speech-sound disorder and reading. Am J Hum Genet
2004;74:283–97.
79
Marlow AJ, Fisher SE, Francks C, et al. Use of multivariate linkage
analysis for dissection of a complex cognitive trait. Am J Hum Genet
80
Faraone SV, Doyle AE, Mick E, et al. Meta-analysis of the
2003;72:561–70.
association between the 7-repeat allele of the dopamine D(4) receptor gene and
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:1052–7.
81
Gaya´n J, Willcutt EG, Fisher SE, et al. Bivariate linkage scan for
reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder localizes pleiotropic
loci. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005;46:1045–56.
82
Loo SK, Fisher SE, Francks C, et al. Genome-wide scan of reading
ability in affected sibling pairs with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: unique
and shared genetic effects. Mol Psychiatry 2004;9:485–93.
83
Taipale M, Kaminen N, Nopola-Hemmi J, et al. A candidate gene
for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat
domain protein dynamically regulated in brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2003;100:11553–8.
84
Wigg KG, Couto JM, Feng Y, et al. Support for EKN1 as the
susceptibility locus for dyslexia on 15q21. Mol Psychiatry 2004;9:1111–21.
85
Scerri TS, Fisher SE, Francks C, et al. Putative functional alleles of
DYX1C1 are not associated with dyslexia susceptibility in a large sample of
sibling pairs from the UK. J Med Genet 2004;41:853–7.
86
Meng H, Hager K, Held M, et al. TDT-association analysis of EKN1
and dyslexia in a Colorado twin cohort. Hum Genet 2005;118:87–90.
87
Marino C, Giorda R, Lorusso ML, et al. A family-based association
study does not support DYX1C1 on 15q21.3 as a candidate gene in
developmental dyslexia. Eur J Hum Genet 2005;13:491–9.
88
Bellini G, Bravaccio C, Calamoneri F, et al. No evidence for
association between dyslexia and DYX1C1 functional variants in a group of
children and adolescents from Southern Italy. J Mol Neurosci 2005;27:311–14.
89
Cope NA, Hill G, van den Bree M, et al. No support for association
between dyslexia susceptibility 1 candidate 1 and developmental dyslexia. Mol
Psychiatry 2005;10:237–8.
90
Harold D, Paracchini S, Scerri T, et al. Further evidence that the
KIAA0319 gene confers susceptibility to developmental dyslexia. Mol
Psychiatry 2006;11:1085–91.
91
Horwitz B, Rumsey JM, Donohue BC. Functional connectivity of
the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
1998;95:8939–44.
92
Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Pugh KR, et al. Functional disruption
in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 1998;95:2636–41.
93
Silani G, Frith U, Demonet JF, et al. Brain abnormalities
underlying altered activation in dyslexia: a voxel based morphometry study.
Brain 2005;128:2453–61.
94
Paracchini S, Thomas A, Castro S, et al. The chromosome 6p22
haplotype associated with dyslexia reduces the expression of KIAA0319, a
novel gene involved in neuronal migration. Hum Mol Genet 2006;15:1659–66.
95
Nopola-Hemmi J, Taipale M, Haltia T, et al. Two translocations of
chromosome 15q associated with dyslexia. J Med Genet 2000;37:771–5.
96
Wang Y, Paramasivam M, Thomas A, et al. DYX1C1 functions in
neuronal migration in developing cortex. Neuroscience 2006;143:515–22.
97
Hannula-Jouppi K, Kaminen-Ahola N, Taipale M, et al. The axon
guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental
dyslexia. PLoS Genet 2005;1:e50.
98
Kidd T, Bland KS, Goodman CS. Slit is the midline repellent
for the robo receptor in Drosophila. Cell 1999;96:785–94.
99
Seeger M, Tear G, Ferres-Marco D, et al. Mutations affecting growth
cone guidance in Drosophila: genes necessary for guidance toward or away from
the midline. Neuron 1993;10:409–26.
100
Andrews W, Liapi A, Plachez C, et al. Robo1 regulates the
development of major axon tracts and interneuron migration in the forebrain.
Development 2006;133:2243–52.
101
Galaburda AM, Kemper TL. Cytoarchitectonic abnormalities in
developmental dyslexia: a case study. Ann Neurol 1979;6:94–100.
102
Galaburda AM, Sherman GF, Rosen GD, et al. Developmental
dyslexia: four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Ann Neurol
103
Galaburda AM. Developmental dyslexia and animal studies: at the
1985;18:222–33.
interface between cognition and neurology. Cognition 1994;50:133–49.
104
Kremen WS, Jacobson KC, Xian H, et al. Heritability of word
recognition in middle-aged men varies as a function of parental education.
Behav Genet 2005;35:417–33.
105
Fisher SE, Marcus GF. The eloquent ape: genes, brains and the
evolution of language. Nat Rev Genet 2006;7:9–20, .