Analyses were conducted to investigate the associative and causal relationships between cognitive abilities and digital behaviors. Here, we describe the procedure for the analyses, results, and discuss relationships.
4.2 Results
The summary statistics of cognitive measures and behavioral measures are shown in
Table 6. The results show that cognitive ability varies across individuals and large variances are observed for most cognitive measures indicating a recording of data from a diverse pool of participants.
Information Perception.
Table 7 and
Figure 4 show the results for the associations between cognitive abilities and the users’ information perception behavior. We found substantial effects of psychomotor speed and fluid intelligence on the amount of cumulative text consumed by the users. Regression analyses reveal an association between psychomotor speed and cumulative text rate with a coefficient
\(F(1,18)=19.42\) and an effect size with
\(R^{2}=0.533\) (
\(p\,{\lt}\,0.001\)). Similarly, the results show a significant linear relationship between fluid intelligence and cumulative text per hour with an effect size (
\(F(1,18)=13.21\),
\(R^{2}=0.43\),
\(p=0.002\)). The Shapiro-Wilk test indicated that cumulative text followed a normal distribution (
\(W=0.95,p=0.39\)).
Figure 4(a) also indicates an association between the amount of cumulative text consumed per hour and the measures of each of the two cognitive abilities (psychomotor speed and fluid intelligence). Generally, regression tests revealed that individuals who scored higher in psychomotor speed (higher in the speed of thinking and decision-making) and higher in fluid intelligence were able to scan texts on the screen more quickly than those who scored lower.
Furthermore, we examined the effects of cognitive abilities on the amount of cumulative entropy, as shown in
Figure 4(b). Significant associations between the amount of cumulative entropy per hour and five cognitive abilities were not observed.
Information Processing.
Table 8 and
Figure 5 show the associations between cognitive abilities and the user’s information processing behavior. For tab-changing behavior, three cognitive abilities: processing speed, selective attention, and fluid intelligence, were associated with the tab-change rate with substantial effect sizes. Regression analysis revealed an association between processing speed and tab change rate with a coefficient
\(F(1,18)=8.604\) and an effect size
\(R^{2}=0.34\) (
\(p=0.0093\)). Similarly,
\(F(1,18)=8.61\) and
\(R^{2}=0.34\) (
\(p=0.0092\)) were found for selective attention ability; and
\(F(1,18)=6.28\) and
\(R^{2}=0.35\) (
\(p=0.023\)) were found for fluid intelligence. The Shapiro-Wilk test revealed that the tab changing rate followed a normal distribution (
\(W=0.91,p=0.25\)).
Figure 5(a) presents an illustration of the regression analysis for the tab change rate against measures of the five cognitive abilities. Linear relationships between the number of tabs that changed over time and the three cognitive abilities were found: processing speed, selective attention, and fluid intelligence. Intuitively, this result indicates that users with lower processing speed, lower attention ability, and lower fluid intelligence spent more time on individual tabs than their counterparts who obtained higher scores on these cognitive tests.
Figure 5(b) shows the effects of five cognitive abilities on the user’s page visit behavior. Associations were found between processing speed and page visit rate (
\(F(1,18)=5.98\),
\(R^{2}=0.26\),
\(p=0.026\)) and found to be normally distributed with
\(W=0.97,p=0.87\) for page visit rate. The results indicate that users with higher processing speed visited Web pages more often than users with lower processing speed. In contrast, users with lower processing speed tended to spend more time dwelling on the pages they accessed.
Figure 5(c) shows the effects of five cognitive abilities on the site visit behavior. Interestingly, we found that only selective attention ability was associated with this behavior with a coefficient
\(F(1,18)=5.18\) and an effect size
\(R^{2}=0.23\) (
\(p=0.036\)). The Shapiro-Wilk test also revealed that site visit behavioral measure was normally distributed with
\(W=0.97,p=0.81\). The results suggest that individuals with better selective attention could focus on relevant information better while moving between Websites.
Input Behavior.
Table 9 and
Figure 6 present the results of regression analyses between cognitive abilities and Omnibox typing speed. We found an effect of working memory on how fast the users type in the Omnibox (address bar and search box). The effect was substantial with a coefficient
\(F(1,18)=5.033\) and
\(R^{2}=0.23\) (
\(p=0.038\)). The Shapiro-Wilk test indicated that input behavior data was normally distributed with
\(W=0.91\) and
\(p=0.21\). The results suggest that people who scored lower in the working memory test would take longer to type in the Omnibox. The reason is likely due to the need to recall a Website URL or the content of a query, which may depend on a user’s working memory.
Causal Effects. The linear regression analyses revealed how cognitive differences were associated with multiple behavioral factors. We considered structural equation modeling for path analysis to further understand the causal dependencies of digital behaviors on cognitive abilities. Because prior works have shown possible relations between cognitive abilities [
12] that may explain the direct and indirect effect on user behavior, we also report results using a path model that accounts for correlation and causality between the abilities. Additionally, the path model also revealed associations among behavioral factors, such as information processing, information perception, and input behavior.
Figure 7 presents the results of structural equation models with each node showing a coefficient for each link. Nodes with links in between indicate significant dependencies with
\(p\,{\lt}\,0.05\), and the arrows indicate causal directions. In contrast, nodes without links in between indicate non-significant relations.
The structural equation models confirmed the results of regression analyses. Selective attention was associated with the amount of information processed by the users and also caused individual differences in this behavior—-with a coefficient of 7.73 (\(p=0.009\)). This suggests that higher selective attention causes a better ability to process information from multiple sources, including increased tab change rate and the number of page and site visits per hour. For example, for users with higher selective attention abilities, the cost of switching between tabs and Web pages (the ability to maintain focus on important information while changing tabs) is much lower than for those with lower attention spans. We also found that processing speed and fluid cognitive abilities indirectly affected how users processed information.
Fluid intelligence has a coefficient of 0.17 (\(p=0.03\)), meaning that higher intelligence causes moderately better perception. Users with higher fluid intelligence were faster at scanning textual and visual information than those with lower fluid intelligence. On the other hand, users with lower fluid intelligence could compensate for their lower cognitive ability with a more focused approach. For instance, they could slowly examine the on-screen content so as not to miss any important information, and carefully decide to change the tab so that each interaction would lead to useful information to maximize the information gained per tab unit. The path models confirmed a relationship between information processing behavior and information perception behavior with a coefficient of 0.44 (\(p=0.024\)). In addition, psychomotor speed has a coefficient of 0.51 (\(p=0.004\)) with fluid intelligence, indicating that psychomotor speed may have an indirect effect on the user’s information perception ability.
The results also showed a direct path between working memory and typing speed confirming that faster typing speed was caused by better working memory of individuals. A negative coefficient of \(-0.29\) (\(p=0.035\)) was found for the causal dependence of input behavior on working memory. Consequently, individuals with lower working memory capacity would type more slowly than those with larger working memory capacity. This result must be interpreted in context. The typing speed was measured from typing in the address bar and search boxes, which have a particular requirement of working memory access. Therefore, the result may not generalize to typing or writing more generally.
Furthermore, the path model revealed an association between information perception and information processing with a coefficient of 0.44 (\(p=0.024\)). This result indicates that the amount of information that a user processed was influenced by the number of pages the user opened. However, there were no associations among input behavior information perception, and information processing.