Jump to content

Randomized response

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Randomised response is a research method used in structured survey interview. It was first proposed by S. L. Warner in 1965 and later modified by B. G. Greenberg and coauthors in 1969.[1][2] It allows respondents to respond to sensitive issues (such as criminal behavior or sexuality) while maintaining confidentiality. Chance decides, unknown to the interviewer, whether the question is to be answered truthfully, or "yes", regardless of the truth.

For example, social scientists have used it to ask people whether they use drugs, whether they have illegally installed telephones, or whether they have evaded paying taxes. Before abortions were legal, social scientists used the method to ask women whether they had had abortions.[3]

The concept is somewhat similar to plausible deniability. Plausible deniability allows the subject to credibly say that they did not make a statement, while the randomized response technique allows the subject to credibly say that they had not been truthful when making a statement.

Example

[edit]

With a coin

[edit]

A person is asked if they had sex with a prostitute this month. Before they answer, they flip a coin. They are then instructed to answer "yes" if the coin comes up tails, and truthfully, if it comes up heads. Only they know whether their answer reflects the toss of the coin or their true experience. It is very important to assume that people who get heads will answer truthfully, otherwise the surveyor is not able to speculate.

Half the people—or half the questionnaire population—get tails and the other half get heads when they flip the coin. Therefore, half of those people will answer "yes" regardless of whether they have done it. The other half will answer truthfully according to their experience. So whatever proportion of the group said "no", the true number who did not have sex with a prostitute is double that, based on the assumption that the two halves are probably close to the same as it is a large randomized sampling. For example, if 20% of the population surveyed said "no", then the true fraction that did not have sex with a prostitute is 40%.

With cards

[edit]

The same question can be asked with three cards which are unmarked on one side, and bear a question on the other side. The cards are randomly mixed, and laid in front of the subject. The subject takes one card, turns it over, and answers the question on it truthfully with either "yes" or "no".

  • One card asks: "Did you have sex with a prostitute this month?"
  • Another card asks: "Is there a triangle on this card?" (There is no triangle.)
  • The last card asks: "Is there a triangle on this card?" (There is a triangle.)

The researcher does not know which question has been asked.

Under the assumption that the "yes" and "no" answers to the control questions cancel each other out, the number of subjects who have had sex with a prostitute is triple that of all "yes" answers in excess of the "no" answers.

Original version

[edit]

Warner's original version (1965) is slightly different: The sensitive question is worded in two dichotomous alternatives, and chance decides, unknown to the interviewer, which one is to be answered honestly. The interviewer gets a "yes" or "no" without knowing to which of the two questions. For mathematical reasons chance cannot be "fair" (1/2 and 1/2). Let be the probability to answer the sensitive question and the true proportion of those interviewed bearing the embarrassing property, then the proportion of "yes"-answers is composed as follows:

Transformed to yield EP:

Example

[edit]
  • Alternative 1: "I have consumed marijuana."
  • Alternative 2: "I have never consumed marijuana."

The interviewed are asked to secretly throw a die and answer the first question only if they throw a 6, otherwise the second question (). The "yes"-answers are now composed of consumers who have thrown a 6 and non-consumers who have thrown a different number. Let the result be 75 "yes"-answers out of 100 interviewed (). Inserted into the formula you get

If all interviewed have answered honestly then their true proportion of consumers is 1/8 (= 12.5%).

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Warner, S. L. (March 1965). "Randomised response: a survey technique for eliminating evasive answer bias". Journal of the American Statistical Association. 60 (309). Taylor & Francis: 63–69. doi:10.1080/01621459.1965.10480775. JSTOR 2283137. PMID 12261830. S2CID 35435339.
  2. ^ Greenberg, B. G.; et al. (June 1969). "The Unrelated Question Randomised Response Model: Theoretical Framework". Journal of the American Statistical Association. 64 (326). Taylor & Francis: 520–39. doi:10.2307/2283636. JSTOR 2283636.
  3. ^ Abernathy, James R.; et al. (February 1970). "Estimates of induced abortion in urban North Carolina". Demography. 7 (1): 19–29. doi:10.2307/2060019. JSTOR 2060019. PMID 5524615.

Further reading

[edit]
  • Aoki, S.; Sezaki, K. (2014). "Privacy-preserving community sensing for medical research with duplicated perturbation". 2014 IEEE International Conference on Communications (ICC). pp. 4252–4257. doi:10.1109/ICC.2014.6883988. ISBN 978-1-4799-2003-7. S2CID 24050604.
  • Chaudhuri, Arijit; Mukerjee, Rahul (1987). Randomized Response: Theory and Techniques. CRC Press. ISBN 9780824777852 – via Google Books.
  • John, Leslie K.; et al. (2018). "When and Why Randomized Response Techniques (Fail to) Elicit the Truth". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 148: 101–123. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2018.07.004. S2CID 52263233.
  • Lee, Cheon-Sig; et al. (2013). "Estimating at least seven measures for qualitative variables using randomized response sampling". Statistics and Probability Letters. 83 (1): 399–409. doi:10.1016/j.spl.2012.10.004.
  • Ostapczuk, M.; et al. (2009). "Assessing sensitive attributes using the randomized-response-technique: Evidence for the importance of response symmetry". Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 34 (2): 267–87. doi:10.3102/1076998609332747. S2CID 15064377.
  • Ostapczuk, M.; et al. (2009). "A randomized-response investigation of the education effect in attitudes towards foreigners". European Journal of Social Psychology. 39 (6): 920–31. doi:10.1002/ejsp.588.
  • Quercia, D.; et al. (2011). "SpotME if You Can: Randomized Responses for Location Obfuscation on Mobile Phones". 2011 31st International Conference on Distributed Computing Systems. IEEE Icdcs. pp. 363–372. doi:10.1109/ICDCS.2011.79. ISBN 978-1-61284-384-1. S2CID 15454609.