Abstract
Childhood emotional abuse impairs emotion regulation and increases risk for major depressive disorder in adulthood. Mounting evidence suggests that decreased resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability, an index of parasympathetic function, represents a transdiagnostic biomarker of emotion dysregulation. We propose that adults with histories of major depressive disorder and childhood emotional abuse represent a subpopulation at particularly high risk to exhibit deficits in parasympathetic control. The current report compared resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability across three groups: (1) depressed women who endorsed childhood emotional abuse (N=11); (2) depressed women without childhood emotional abuse (N=19), and (3) never-depressed women without childhood emotional abuse (N=22).Participants completed childhood trauma self-reports and assessment of resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability. ANCOVAs comparing the three groups after controlling for health-related, psychiatric, and respiratory factors were significant. Depressed women with childhood emotional abuse exhibited lower high-frequency heart rate variability than both groups without childhood emotional abuse (d’s ranging from 0.81—0.92). Surprisingly, psychiatric factors were non-significant predictors, indicating that childhood emotional abuse may have a unique impact on autonomic functioning. Future research on larger samples is needed to disentangle the relative and synergistic burdens of depression and childhood trauma on physiologic indicators of emotion dysregulation.
Keywords: depression, childhood emotional abuse, high-frequency heart rate variability
I. INTRODUCTION
The experience of trauma in childhood is thought to impair development of adaptive emotion regulation, and to underlie risk for psychopathology in adulthood (Heim and Nemeroff, 2001). Emotional abuse is by far the most prevalent form of child trauma (Vachon et al., 2015), and has been specifically implicated in heightened stress reactivity (Shapero et al., 2014) and increased risk for major depressive disorder (Gibb et al., 2003; Gibb et al., 2007). A parallel literature has found that depressed adults exhibit deficits in stress-related emotion regulation (Gross and Muññoz, 1995) and alterations in markers of parasympathetic nervous system function, such as decreased resting-state levels of high-frequency heart rate variability (Rottenberg, 2007). We propose that among depressed adults, the experience of childhood emotional abuse may help to identify a sub-sample of clients with alterations in high-frequency heart rate variability. Given the high prevalence of childhood emotional abuse among depressed adults and its negative impact on depression treatment outcome, clarifying this link has implications for advancing theoretical models regarding the nature of emotion regulation deficits among depressed adults and personalizing depression interventions.
Mounting evidence supports the contention that decreased resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability represents a biomarker of emotion dysregulation as well as psychopathology risk (Beauchaine, 2015). High-frequency heart rate variability, a measure of variation in time between heart beats, indexes parasympathetic input on cardiac function through the vagus nerve. The vagus nerve connects brain regions implicated in emotional reactivity and regulation (Thayer et al., 2012) with peripheral organs, including the heart. As a counterpart to sympathetic excitatory inputs, the parasympathetic ‘vagal brake’ has an inhibitory effect on heart rate. In the absence of stressors, when parasympathetic input dominates, higher resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability indicates greater perceived safety and emotional flexibility. In contrast, lower high-frequency heart rate variability is hypothesized to reflect decreased parasympathetic activation and control, which may lead to more rigid or exaggerated responses to future stressors and diminished emotion regulation capacity (Porges, 2007).
Childhood trauma exerts lasting effects on the central nervous system, impairing the body’s capacity to flexibly regulate emotional responses to environmental demands (Heim and Nemeroff, 2001). The experience of trauma has been associated with decreased high-frequency heart rate variability among adults with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) - indicating that these populations indeed have lower parasympathetic flexibility and control (Cohen et al., 1997; Dale et al., 2009). Initial evidence examining the impact of childhood trauma supports similar decreases in high-frequency heart rate variability among adolescent girls (Miskovic et al., 2009). Given that the experience of childhood trauma predicts poor adjustment in adolescence (Mills et al., 2013), risk for adult psychopathology in general (Kessler et al., 2010; MacMillan et al., 2001), increased risk for adult depression in particular (Gibb et al., 2007), and poorer depression prognosis (including worse treatment response and greater recurrence; Nanni et al., 2012), identifying enduring physiological effects of childhood abuse that may impair adaptive emotion regulation among adults with a history of depression is warranted.
The experience of childhood emotional abuse is common (prevalence estimate 36%) compared with childhood physical abuse 8% and sexual abuse 2% and neglect 4% (Kessler et al., 2010; Stoltenborgh et al., 2012). Additionally, mounting evidence indicates that depression is more strongly associated with the experience of childhood emotional abuse as compared with these other types of childhood trauma (e.g., Gibb et al., 2003; Gibb et al., 2007; for recent metaanalysis see Infurna et al., 2016). Because childhood emotional abuse involves family members directly expressing hurtful statements that make the child feel unloved or unwanted (Bernstein et al., 2003), the experience of childhood emotional abuse may specifically contribute to the development of insecure attachments as well as aspects of cognitive vulnerability implicated in depression risk (Rose and Abramson, 1992). Teasing apart these childhood risk factors is made difficult, however, by the fact that emotional abuse is often comorbid with other less common forms of childhood abuse or neglect (Dong et al., 2004). Regardless, the experience of childhood emotional abuse alone is sufficient to predict broad and potent effects on child psychiatric and behavioral outcomes (Vachon et al., 2015). These data highlight the importance of evaluating the impact that childhood emotional abuse may have on stress-related physiology in depression, such as indicators of parasympathetic activity indexed by measures of high-frequency heart rate variability.
Research on the link between childhood emotional abuse and alterations in high-frequency heart rate variability in depressed populations is limited. Although meta-analytic findings support a significant association between major depressive disorder and decreased high- frequency heart rate variability (Kemp et al., 2010; Koenig et al., 2016; Rottenberg, 2007), the impact of childhood emotional abuse has not been emphasized. One study linked global reports of lifetime trauma with decreased high-frequency heart rate variability among depressed women, compared to non-depressed women and depressed women without a lifetime trauma history (Cyranowski et al., 2011). Further, a history of childhood emotional abuse specifically predicts impairments in stress-related emotional and cardiovascular reactivity among depressed mothers (Cyranowski et al., 2009). The current report seeks to extend these findings by evaluating whether a history of childhood emotional abuse is associated with specific deficits in emotion regulation (decreased high-frequency heart rate variability), in a sample of women with or without a history of major depressive disorder.
We hypothesized that women with a history of depression and childhood emotional abuse would exhibit decreased resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability, relative to women with a history of major depressive disorder but no childhood emotional abuse. For comparison, we also included a group of never-depressed women without childhood emotional abuse. Models were run controlling for the known effects of health-related factors (age, body mass index, and smoking status) on high-frequency heart rate variability (Antelmi et al., 2004; Koenig et al., 2014). We also ran additional tests to examine the robustness of associations after controlling for the influence of psychiatric and respiratory factors.
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants
The initial study included 41 women with a history of major depressive disorder, who were recruited from a larger NIMH-funded randomized controlled trial testing brief psychotherapeutic treatments for depressed mothers with psychiatrically-ill children (Swartz et al., 2016). For a comparison group, we also obtained data from a sample of 25 age-matched mothers without a history of major depressive disorder or other psychiatric disorders (“controls”). Depressed mothers were recruited from child psychiatric treatment clinics and community advertisements; controls were recruited via community advertisements. Participants were excluded for current substance abuse, history of psychotic or bipolar disorders, unstable medical conditions, and active suicidal ideation. Women taking medications known to influence high-frequency heart rate variability (n = 10) and those with abnormal ECG data were removed from the current analyses (n = 3: arrhythmia, premature ventricular contraction, and one extreme outlier, z > 2.5). Because only one of the remaining 23 controls reported a history of childhood emotional abuse, this participant was also excluded, leaving a final analysis sample of 22 controls without childhood emotional abuse, 19 depressed women without childhood emotional abuse, and 11 depressed women with a history of childhood emotional abuse. See Table 2. The University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures.
Table 2:
Controls |
History of Major Depression Disorder |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | No Emotional Abuse (n = 22) |
No Emotional Abuse (n = 19) |
Emotional Abuse (n = 11) |
F(2, 52) | |||
Age: mean, SD b | 47.2 | 4.5 | 44.4 | 7.1 | 40.2 | 6.7 | 5.03** |
Race: White n, % | 17 | 77% | 15 | 80% | 10 | 91% | 0.73 |
Education ≥ Associates: n, % | 15 | 68% | 12 | 63% | 7 | 64% | 1.74 |
Married: n, % | 15 | 68% | 10 | 53% | 4 | 36% | 3.20 |
Income < $30,000: n, % abc | 1 | 5% | 6 | 32% | 6 | 55% | 12.72*** |
BMI: mean, SD | 28.1 | 6.0 | 29.3 | 5.8 | 32.3 | 7.5 | 1.63 |
Smoke: n, % | 3 | 14% | 2 | 11% | 4 | 36% | 1.92 |
Antidepressants: n, % ab | 0 | 0% | 6 | 32% | 3 | 27% | 5.89* |
Lifetime PTSD: n, % | 0 | 0% | 5 | 26% | 0 | 0% | 0.41 |
PROMIS anxiety: mean, SD ab | 10.5 | 3.4 | 17.2 | 5.8 | 17.0 | 6.4 | 10.66*** |
QIDS, n (%) current MDD ab | 0 | 0% | 6 | 32% | 3 | 27% | 8.44*** |
Note: Emotional Abuse= reported history of childhood emotional abuse; Race: all other participants were African American, no-one reported Hispanic ethnicity. Education: achieved an Associates Degree or higher; BMI = Body Mass Index; PTSD = lifetime history of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder; PROMTS: current anxiety symptom levels; QIDS ≥ 9 as clinical threshold for current major depressive disorder (MDD).
Significant group differences between controls and women with MDD history no emotional abuse
Significant group differences between controls and women with MDD and emotional abuse history
Significant group differences between the two MDD groups
p < 0.05,
p ≤ .01,
p ≤ .001
2.2. Measures
Current and lifetime psychiatric diagnoses were assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First et al., 1995). Demographic information, lifestyle characteristics (smoking status, body mass index) and antidepressant use were also assessed during this interview. Participants reported on their current anxiety and depressive symptoms during the laboratory assessment via the Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System, 7-item anxiety scale (PROMIS, α =.92; Pilkonis et al., 2011) and the 16-item Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (QIDS, α =.86; Rush et al., 2003).
Participants reported their history of childhood abuse and neglect via the 28-item Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ, Bernstein et al., 2003). Each item is rated on a 5-point, Likert-type scale: 1= never true, 2 = rarely true, 3 = sometimes true, 4 = often true, 5 = very often true. Subscale scores were calculated by summing responses; established thresholds reflecting moderate abuse were used to create five dichotomous variables: presence versus absence of moderate emotional abuse (≥13), physical abuse (≥10), sexual abuse (≥8), emotional neglect (≥15) or physical neglect (≥10). Internal reliability was strong: α =.92, α =.75, α =.96, α =.90, and α =.86 respectively. The CTQ has demonstrated excellent psychometric properties in clinical and nonclinical samples, including high levels of criterion-related validity with therapists’ ratings of abuse (Bernstein et al., 2003).
The MindWare BioNex and HRV 2.16 software (MindWare Technologies, Gahanna, OH) were used to measure and process ECG and respiratory data. High-frequency heart rate variability was assessed via ECG signals sampled continuously at 1000 Hz with three spot electrodes placed in a Lead II configuration (right clavicle, right and left lower rib-cage). R-wave markers in the ECG signal were processed with artifact detection algorithms, with suspected artifacts corrected manually. Each 1 -minute segment was inspected visually to ensure accurate program R-wave detection. On rare occasions, suspected artifacts were corrected manually (which occurred in < 1% of all beats). Data segments with multiple irregularities that called into question identification of beats (such as those associated with movement artifacts) were removed from analyses (this also occurred in < 1% of available data). After data cleaning was complete, an artifact corrected IBI series was generated, processed using interpolation at 33hz and then detrended (Berntson et al., 1995). A frequency-domain procedure was used to calculate heart rate variability. More specifically, in a frequency-domain approach, the heart rate time series is segregated into separate components (e.g., high-frequency vs. low frequency) according to non-overlapping bandwidths. Thus, frequency is represented on the x-axis, heart rate variability on the y-axis. Heart rate variability was measured within the 0.12 - 0.40 Hz bandwidth in the current study. Fast Fourier transformations (in ms2/Hz) were then applied to the residuals to derive the spectral distribution. Finally, spectral-power analysis was used to identify power values in the 0.12 - 0.40 Hz spectral bandwidth (ms2) to indicate high-frequency heart rate variability values per minute (see Allen et al., 2007; Stein and Kleiger, 1999). Respiration was simultaneously assessed with a continuous transducer sampled at 5 Hz with the HRV 2.16 software. Breaths per minute were calculated by the software and visually confirmed. High-frequency heart rate variability and breaths per each of the six, one-minute segments were calculated individually, and then averaged to provide a measure of resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability (see Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology, 1996). The decision to assess heart rate variability in one-minute segments was based on two opposing concerns. One the one hand, one-minute epochs are not large enough to risk violating the stationary assumption of spectral analysis. Conversely, one-minute segments are large enough to adequately capture adults’ typical respiration rate (Berntson et al., 1997), which is necessary as heart rate variability is intrinsically linked with respiratory cycle. Indeed, accurately controlling for the impact of respiration is critical given their inverse relation (respiration increases heart rate variability decreases) (Cooke et al., 1998; Stark et al., 2000). We calculated the impact of respiration by regressing respiration on high-frequency heart rate variability separately for each of the six one- minute segments, then taking the average of the six predicted residuals.
2.3. Procedures
Participants were instructed to refrain from caffeine and tobacco for three hours and alcohol for 48 hours prior to the laboratory visit. First, informed consent was obtained. Clinical interviews were completed prior to the physiological assessment, during which participants were seated in comfortable chairs and physiological sensors were placed. After completing questionnaires during a 10-minute habituation period, participants were instructed to rest quietly (without speaking) for six minutes, providing a measure of resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability.
2.4. Data Analysis
Three analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) models were run to test the primary hypothesis: that women with a history of both major depressive disorder and childhood emotional abuse (n=11) would exhibit the lowest levels of resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability, compared to both depressed women (n =19) and controls (n =22) without a childhood history of emotional abuse. First, a one-way ANCOVA compared resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability among the three groups after controlling for key health-related factors identified in the broader high-frequency heart period variability literature (namely, age, body mass index, and smoking status). A second ANCOVA controlled for psychiatric variables identified in the literature (lifetime history of PTSD, current depressive symptoms, current anxiety symptoms, on/off antidepressants). Finally, a third model examined whether results were maintained after accounting for variance in high-frequency heart rate variability due to respiration rate (utilizing residual scores). Post-hoc comparisons of group means were conducted via Tukey’s HSD (honest significance difference) tests.
3. RESULTS
Descriptive physiological data related to average heart rate variability, heart rate and respiration rate by group are provided in Table 1. Intra-class correlations of high-frequency heart rate variability across the six 1-minute measures was high (ICC = .966), as were those for heart rate (ICC = .995) and respiration rate (ICC = .959).
Table 1:
Controls: No EA (n = 22) |
MDD: No EA (n = 19) |
MDD: EA (n = 11) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD |
Heart Rate | 69.71 | 12.49 | 72.13 | 10.19 | 78.12 | 8.35 |
Heart Rate Variability | 5.63 | 1.23 | 5.78 | 0.95 | 4.87 | 1.26 |
Respiration | 15.74 | 3.85 | 15.23 | 3.09 | 16.58 | 1.98 |
Note: EA: history of emotional abuse in childhood; MDD = women with a history of major depressive disorder
Demographic and clinical characteristics across the three groups are presented in Table 2. ANOVAs and Chi-square tests were run to test for group differences in demographic, health-related, and psychiatric variables with controls as the reference group. The three groups did not differ on race, education level, marital status, body mass index, or history of smoking cigarettes. As would be expected, the depressed groups reported higher depressive and anxiety symptoms than controls, as well as higher rates of antidepressant use. There was a significant difference in age, with control women being older than depressed women with a history of childhood emotional abuse. Finally, there was a group difference in annual income, with control women being the highest earners (median household annual income between $75,000-$99,000) followed by depressed women without childhood abuse (median $40,000-$49,999) and then depressed women with a history of emotional abuse (median $10,000-$19,999). However, because income did not represent an a priori control and income variability was not independently associated with high-frequency heart rate variability, F(1, 51) = 0.43, p = .52, ƞp2 < .01, income was not included as a covariate.
The ANCOVA model controlling for established health-related predictors of high-frequency heart rate variability (age, body mass index and smoking), was significant, F(5, 51) = 4.04, p = .004, ƞp2 = .31. Group was a significant predictor, F(2, 51) = 3.381, p = .043, ƞp2 = .13. As anticipated, depressed women with childhood emotional abuse exhibited decreased high-frequency heart rate variability (M = 4.92, SE= 0.34) compared to depressed women without childhood emotional abuse (M = 5.95, SE= 0.28; Tukey’s HSD p < .001; Cohen’s d = 0.92), or controls (M = 5.89, SE= 0.27; Tukey’s HSD p = .001; d = 0.81). In contrast, the two groups of women without a history of childhood emotional abuse did not significantly differ (Tukey’s HSD p = .824). See Figure 1 for a scatterplot of these results. Of the covariates in the model, body mass index [F(1, 51) = 10.17, p = .003, ƞp2 = .18] and age [F(1, 51) = 4.50, p = .04, ƞp2 = .09] had significant effects, while smoking status did not [F(1, 51) = 1.78,p = .189, ƞp2 = .03].
The follow-up ANCOVA controlling for both health-related factors and psychiatric factors maintained significance [F(9, 51) = 2.24, p = .039, ƞp2 = .33]. Group continued to predict unique variance in high-frequency heart rate variability [F(2, 51) = 3.24, p = .049, ƞp2 = .14], with comparisons yielding an identical pattern of results. Specifically, depressed women with childhood emotional abuse displayed lower high-frequency heart rate variability than the other two groups. None of the psychiatric predictors had significant independent effects (lowest p = .295).
To test the robustness of this finding, a third model was run after regressing out variance in high-frequency heart rate variability due to respiration. We continued to covary for health-related factors in this model but did not include the non-significant psychiatric covariates. The model was significant [F(5, 51) = 4.24,p = .003, ƞp2 = .32], and group status continued to predict the high-frequency heart rate variability residuals [F(2, 51) = 3.51, p = .038, ƞp2 = .14].
Depressed women with childhood emotional abuse exhibited significantly decreased high-frequency heart rate variability compared to depressed women without emotional abuse (Tukey’s HSD p = .004, d = 0.90) or controls (Tukey’s HSD p = .005, d = 0.87), even after controlling for both respiratory and health-related factors.
While childhood emotional abuse frequently co-occurred with other forms of abuse or neglect in the current sample (i.e., in 9 of 11 cases), the low incidence of the other individual trauma subtypes [physical abuse (n=4), sexual abuse (n=5), emotional neglect (n=10), and physical neglect (n=5)] prevented post-hoc analyses to test unique impacts of other forms of childhood trauma. However, we did conduct a post-hoc analysis to evaluate whether history of any childhood trauma would impact women’s resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability. Resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability levels were compared among women with a history of depression and any childhood trauma (n= 13), women with a history of depression but not childhood trauma (n =17) and control women with no childhood trauma history (n =18). (Given the small cell size, 4 control women with a history of any childhood trauma were removed from this analysis.) Re-running the original model covarying for health-related predictors, we found that while the overall ANCOVA model was significant [F(5,48) = 2.90, p =.024, ƞp2 = .26], the group effect of any childhood trauma history on high-frequency heart rate variability did not reach statistical significance [F(2,48) = 1.32, p =.28, ƞp2 = .06].
4. DISCUSSION
We examined whether a history of both major depressive disorder and childhood emotional abuse would account for variance in resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability, a global indicator of parasympathetic function with potential impacts on emotion regulation. As anticipated, women with a history of both depression and childhood emotional abuse exhibited significantly decreased resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability. Importantly, psychiatric factors including current depression and anxiety severity and diagnosis of PTSD were not significant predictors of high-frequency heart rate variability in the model. This pattern indicates that childhood emotional abuse may be a stronger predictor of parasympathetic control. Further, this finding aligns with our previous research, that found that a lifetime trauma history was associated with decreased high-frequency heart rate variability among depressed women (Cyranowski et al., 2011), and that depressed mothers with a history of childhood emotional abuse show exaggerated emotional and blood pressure responses to child-focused stressors (Cyranowski et al., 2009).
The impact of childhood emotional abuse on depressed mothers’ resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability is significant, as is the fact that the severity of current depression/anxiety symptoms did not significantly predict high-frequency heart rate variability in the current sample (which is consistent with Koenig et al, 2016, but not with Kemp et al, 2010). Prior work has also linked PTSD diagnosis among adults (indicated experience of trauma in adulthood) with lower heart rate variability (e.g., Cohen et al., 1997; Sack et al., 2004). Thus, the lack of utility of PTSD, depression and anxiety severity for predicting heart rate variability in the current model is striking and may indicate that the impact of emotional abuse in childhood is e particularly salient to emotion regulation in adulthood. Moreover, alterations in parasympathetic tone, indicating emotion dysregulation, also has implications for adult interpersonal functioning. That is, research has established that both depression (Beach et al., 1990) and trauma history (Reyome, 2010) negatively impact interpersonal functioning. Parallel work also supports the assertion that decreased high-frequency heart rate variability levels are linked with poorer interpersonal functioning, which may represent a key link across associations among lifetime depression, low social support, and risk for coronary disease (Smith et al., 2011; for review see Smith et al., 2004). These findings align with a growing body of research implicating the developmentally-potent consequences of early trauma across multiple physical and emotional adult health outcomes (Nusslock and Miller, 2016).
Maternal childhood trauma history has also been implicated in the intergenerational transmission of depression risk - particularly for women. Specifically, in a large, population-based Finnish study, female offspring of mothers who had experienced childhood trauma were significantly more likely to develop adult mood disorders (Santavirta et al., 2017). The current finding, indicating decreased parasympathetic function specific to mothers with a history of depression and childhood emotional abuse, may help to further elucidate potential mechanisms underlying the intergenerational transmission of depression risk. Future research is needed, however, regarding the nature and direction of relationships among maternal impairments in parasympathetic function, stress responsivity and parenting, and the impact these may have on child development and depression risk.
The current results have several potential implications for clinicians. As noted above, decreased levels of parasympathetic activity displayed among depressed women with histories of childhood emotional abuse likely relate to observed impairments in regulating physical and emotional responses to stress (Cyranowski et al, 2009). Decreased high-frequency heart rate variability, as an index of emotion regulatory capacity, may not only contribute to depressive episode recurrence, but also undermine positive interpersonal functioning, support-seeking and problem-solving, which may thereby contribute to further “stress generation” observed among women with depression histories (Hammen, 2016). Unfortunately, our small sample size limited the ability to examine how depressed mothers with lower levels of high-frequency heart rate variability faired in response to interpersonal therapy or brief supportive therapy (see Swartz et al., 2016 for primary study outcomes). Childhood trauma has, however, previously been associated with poorer depression prognosis. For example, women with a childhood trauma history are more likely to experience chronic or recurrent episodes that are more treatment-resistant (for review see: Nanni et al., 2012). Taken together, the current results highlight the need for future research that tests whether specific treatments are more effective for this depressed sub-population, and to explore ways to ‘personalize’ depression treatments.
To facilitate such personalized treatment approaches, clinicians would be wise to consider incorporating routine assessment questions (or surveys) regarding the experience of childhood emotional abuse into standard intake protocols. In general, childhood histories of physical and sexual abuse are more widely assessed and more easily identified by clinicians and clients alike. The insidious nature of childhood emotional abuse may make it easier for clinicians to overlook, and far less likely for clients to spontaneously report, at intake assessment. Thus, use of a standardized self-report scales and/or direct assessment of childhood experiences of emotional abuse (such as parents/guardians repeatedly calling one names, saying hurtful things, or making one feel hated or unwanted) may be particularly important to include in routine assessments of clients with a history of major depressive disorder.
Our findings also introduce interesting theoretical questions regarding the temporal association between high-frequency heart rate variability and depression. Decreased high-frequency heart rate variability is now considered a transdiagnostic biomarker of emotion dysregulation as well as psychopathology (Beauchaine, 2015). Meta-analyses support that among adults and adolescents, decreased vagal activity is at least a correlate of ongoing depression (Kemp et al., 2010; Koenig et al., 2016; Rottenberg, 2007), with some research to suggest that lower levels of high-frequency heart rate variability may precede depression onset (Jandackova et al, 2016). The cross-sectional and retrospective nature of the current study limits our capacity to examine these critical questions. Thus, goals for future research will be to clarify the extent to which decreases in high-frequency heart rate variability may develop in response to childhood emotional abuse, and whether decreases in high frequency heart rate variability are implicated in depression etiology and/or represent a ‘scar’ or residual feature of a particular subtype of depression (e.g., Bylsma et al., 2014).
Limitations of the current study include the small sample size and absence of a group of never-depressed women who experienced of childhood emotional abuse. We would note that the select nature of our control condition (which excluded individuals with any history of mood or anxiety disorders) likely contributed to this lack of variability among controls, and undermined our ability to utilize alternate statistical approaches, such as factorial ANOVA designs that could directly assess potential Depression Group x Childhood Emotional Abuse interactions. Future studies that incorporate larger, more representative samples of non-depressed participants (who vary in their experience of childhood emotional abuse) will be needed to replicate and extend this report. We also note the potential limitations inherent to the reliance on retrospective reports of childhood trauma (Monroe, 2008), particularly with the goal of establishing temporal precedence and directional effects between childhood trauma, lifetime depression, and high-frequency heart rate variability. Future longitudinal work will be needed to clarify the temporal dynamics of these potentially interacting risk factors. Further, although we covaried for multiple health, psychiatric and respiratory factors implicated in prior research, our models did not account for other potential sources of influence (such menstrual cycle, physical activity level, assessment time, other non-psychiatric medication intake, or attachment style). Future research examining these additional factors would be warranted. Finally, depressed women included in the current report were mothers of school-aged children (who also met criteria for at least one lifetime internalizing disorder). Therefore, the findings may not generalize to other populations. Future studies will be needed to determine whether the associations found extend to other females or depressed male samples, and to further elucidate potential mechanisms underlying these relationships.
History of childhood emotional abuse was a stronger predictor of high-frequency heart rate variability than history of any childhood trauma in this sample. The pattern of childhood trauma prevalence obtained in the current sample mirrors prior findings (Kessler et al., 2010; Stoltenborgh et al., 2012), in that childhood emotional abuse was most common but frequently co-occurred with other less prevalent trauma subtypes (such as sexual or physical abuse). The low rates of other abuse and neglect subtypes prevented us from comparing the unique individual (or additive) impacts of various childhood trauma subtypes. Taken together, however, the current results suggest that a self-reported history of childhood emotional abuse is itself a useful predictor of emotion dysregulation in adulthood. Given World Health Organization reports regarding the “universality” of childhood emotional abuse - which may be experienced by up to a third of adults globally (Kessler et al., 2010; Stoltenborgh et al., 2012) - the potentially wide spread impact of this risk factor is worthy of further study.
Results of the current report indicate that depressed women with a history of childhood emotional abuse display decreased resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability, an indicator of parasympathetic function that may be associated with emotion dysregulation. Future research replicating this finding is warranted to determine if childhood emotional abuse history accounts for previous reports of diminished high-frequency heart rate variability observed among depressed adults (Kemp et al., 2010; Rottenberg, 2007). Given its prevalence within depressed populations, clinicians should assess for indicators of childhood emotional abuse among adult clients with a history of major depressive disorder, as these clients may benefit from interventions designed to enhance emotion regulation.
Highlights.
Childhood emotional abuse is common, and implicated in emotion regulation deficits
Decreased high-frequency heart rate variability may represent a marker of emotion dysregulation
Women with depression and child emotional abuse had low high-frequency heart rate variability
Therapy for depressed women with childhood emotional abuse should target emotion dysregulation
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by funding from National Institute of Mental Health [R01 MH083647]. The authors are especially grateful to Dr. Lauren Bylsma for her contributions on the preprocessing and analysis of HRV data. We also wish to thank Stacy Martin, LPC for her assistance in data acquisition. Finally, we thank the participants and their families.
Footnotes
Declaration of interest: Holly A. Swartz receives royalties from UpToDate, research funding from Myriad Genetics, and served as a consultant to Myriad Genetics. The current research was supported by funding from National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH083647). Jill M. Cyranowski, Lindsey B. Stone and Marlissa C. Amole have no conflicts of interest to report.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Allen JJ, Chambers AS, Towers DN, 2007. The many metrics of cardiac chronotropy: a pragmatic primer and a brief comparison of metrics. Biol. Psychol, 74, 243–262. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antelmi I, De Paula RS, Shinzato AR, Peres CA, Mansure AJ, Grupi CJ, 2004. Influence of age, gender, body mass index, and functional capacity on heart rate variability in a cohort of subjects without heart disease. Am. J. Cardiol, 93, 381–385. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2003.09.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beach SRH, Sandeen EE, O’Leary KD, 1990. Depression in Marriage: A Model for Etiology and Treatment. Guilford Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Beauchaine TP, 2015. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: a transdiagnostic biomarker of emotion dysregulation and psychopathology. Curr Opin in Psychol. 3, 43–47. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, … Zule W, 2003. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl, 27, 169–190. doi: 10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntson GG, Bigger JT Jr., Eckberg DL, Grossman P, Kaufmann PG, Malik M, … van der Molen MW, 1997. Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology. 34, 623–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntson GG, Cacioppo JT, Quigley KS, 1995. The metrics of cardiac chronotropism: biometric perspectives. Psychophysiology. 32, 162–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bylsma LM, Salomon K, Taylor-clift A, Morris BH, Rottenberg J, 2014. Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia Reactivity in Current and Remitted Major Depressive Disorder. Psychosom. Med, 76, 66–73. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen H, Kotler M, Matar MA, Kaplan Z, Miodownik H, Cassuto Y, 1997. Power spectral analysis of heart rate variability in posttraumatic stress disorder patients. Biol. Psychiatry. 41, 627–629. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00525-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooke WH, Cox JF, Diedrich AM, Taylor JA, Beightol LA, Ames IV JE, … Eckberg DL, 1998. Controlled breathing protocols probe human autonomic cardiovascular rhythms. American Journal of Physiology-Heart and Circulatory Physiology. 274, H709–H718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cyranowski JM, Hofkens RL, Swartz HA, Salomen K, Gianaros PJ, 2011. Cardiac vagal control in nonmedicated depressed women and controls: Impact of depression status, lifetime trauma history, and respiratory factors. Psychosom. Med, 73, 336–343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cyranowski JM, Swartz HA, Hofkens TL, Frank E, 2009. Emotional and cardiovascular reactivity to a child-focused interpersonal stressor among depressed mothers of psychiatrically ill children. Depress. Anxiety. 26, 110–116. doi: 10.1002/da.20515 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale LP, Carroll LE, Galen G, Hayes JA, Webb KW, Porges SW, 2009. Abuse history is related to autonomic regulation to mild exercise and psychological wellbeing. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback. 34, 299–308. doi: 10.1007/s10484-009-9111-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dong M, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, … Giles WH, 2004. The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction. Child Abuse Negl, 28, 771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB, 1995. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders. New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Gibb BE, Butler AC, Beck JS, 2003. Childhood abuse, depression, and anxiety in adult psychiatric outpatients. Depress.Anxiety. 17, 226–228. doi: 10.1002/da.10111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibb BE, Chelminski I, Zimmerman M, 2007. Childhood emotional, physical, and sexual abuse, and diagnoses of depressive and anxiety disorders in adult psychiatric outpatients. Depress. Anxiety. 24, 256–263. doi: 10.1002/da.20238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ, Muñoz RF, 1995. Emotion regulation and mental health. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2, 151–164.doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.1995.tb00036.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Heim C, Nemeroff CB, 2001. The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biol. Psychiatry. 49, 1023–1039. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01157-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Infurna MR, Reichl C, Parzer P, Schimmenti A, Bifulco A, Kaess M, 2016. Associations between depression and specific childhood experiences of abuse and neglect: A meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord, 190, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemp AH, Quintana DS, Gray MA, Felmingham KL, Brown K, Gatt JM, 2010. Impact of Depression and Antidepressant Treatment on Heart Rate Variability: A Review and Meta-Analysis. Biol. Psychiatry. 67, 1067–1074. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, … Williams DR, 2010. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Brit J Psychiat. 197, 378–385. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koenig J, Jarczok MN, Warth M, Ellis RJ, Bach C, Hillecke TK, Thayer JF, 2014. Body mass index is related to autonomic nervous system activity as measured by heart rate.- J. Nutr. Health Aging. 18, 300–302. doi: 10.1007/s12603-014-0022-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koenig J, Kemp AH, Beauchaine TP, Thayer JF, Kaess M, 2016. Depression and resting state heart rate variability in children and adolescents — A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev, 46, 136–150. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacMillan HL, Fleming JE, Streiner DL, Lin E, Boyle MH, Jamieson E, … Beardslee WR, 2001. Childhood Abuse and Lifetime Psychopathology in a Community Sample. Am. J. Psychiatry. 158, 1878–1883. doi:1878.pdf [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mills R, Scott J, Alati R, O’Callaghan M, Najman JM, Strathearn L, 2013. Child maltreatment and adolescent mental health problems in a large birth cohort. Child Abuse Negl. 37, 292–302. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.11.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miskovic V, Schmidt LA, Georgiades K, Boyle M, MacMillan HL, 2009. Stability of resting frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) asymmetry and cardiac vagal tone in adolescent females exposed to child maltreatment. Dev. Psychobiol, 51, 474–487. doi: 10.1002/dev.20387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nanni V, Uher R, Danese A, 2012. Childhood maltreatment predicts unfavorable course of illness and treatment outcome in depression: A meta-analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry. 169, 141–151. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nusslock R, Miller GE, 2016. Early-life adversity and physical and emotional health across the lifespan: A neuroimmune network hypothesis. Biol. Psychiatry. 80, 23–32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pilkonis PA, Choi SW, Reise SP, Stover AM, Riley WT, Cella D, 2011. Item Banks for Measuring Emotional Distress From the Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS®): Depression, Anxiety, and Anger. Assessment. 18, 263–283. doi: 10.1177/1073191111411667 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porges SW, 2007. The polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol, 74, 116–143. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reyome ND, 2010. Childhood Emotional Maltreatment and Later Intimate Relationships: Themes from the Empirical Literature. J.Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma. 19, 224–242. doi: 10.1080/10926770903539664 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rose DT, Abramson LY, 1992. Developmental predictors of depressive cognitive style: Research and theory In Cicchetti D & Toth S (Eds.), Rochester Symposium of Developmental Psychopathology. University of Rochester Press, Rochester, NY, pp. 323–349. [Google Scholar]
- Rottenberg J, 2007. Cardiac vagal control in depression: A critical analysis. Biol. Psychol, 74, 200–211.doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rush AJ, Trivedi MH, Ibrahim HM, Carmody TJ, Arnow B, Klein DN, … Keller MB, 2003. The 16-item Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (QIDS), clinician rating (QIDS-C), and self-report (QIDS-SR): A psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic major depression. Biol. Psychiatry. 54, 573–583. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01866-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sack M, Hopper JW, Lamprecht F, 2004. Low respiratory sinus arrhythmia and prolonged psychophysiological arousal in posttraumatic stress disorder: heart rate dynamics and individual differences in arousal regulation. Biol. Psychiatry. 55, 284–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santavirta T, Santavirta N, Gilman SE, 2017. Association of the World War II Finnish Evacuation of Children With Psychiatric Hospitalization in the Next Generation. JAMA Psychiatry. Published online November 29, 2017. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3511 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapero BG, Black SK, Liu RT, Klugman J, Bender RE, Abramson LY, … Canada G.o.C.N.R.C., 2014. Stressful Life Events and Depression Symptoms: The Effect of Childhood Emotional Abuse on Stress Reactivity. J. Clin. Psychol, 70, 209–223. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith TW, Cribbet MR, Nealey-Moore JB, Uchino BN, Williams PG, Mackenzie J, Thayer JF, 2011. Matters of the variable heart: respiratory sinus arrhythmia response to marital interaction and associations with marital quality. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol, 100, 103–119. doi: 10.1037/a0021136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith TW, Glazer K, Ruiz JM, Gallo LC, 2004. Hostility, Anger, Aggressiveness, and Coronary Heart Disease: An Interpersonal Perspective on Personality, Emotion, and Health. J. Pers, 72, 1217–1270. doi:info:doi/ 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00296.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark R, Schienle A, Walter B, Vaitl D, 2000. Effects of paced respiration on heart period and heart period variability. Psychophysiology. 37, 302–309. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein PK, Kleiger RE, 1999. Insights from the study of heart rate variability. Annu. Rev. Med, 50, 249–261. doi: 10.1146/annurev.med.50.1.249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoltenborgh M, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Alink LRA, van IJzendoorn MH, 2012. The Universality of Childhood Emotional Abuse: A Meta-Analysis of Worldwide Prevalence. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma. 21, 870–890. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2012.708014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Swartz H, Cyranowski J, Cheng Y, Zuckoff A, Brent D, Markowitz J, … Frank E, 2016. Brief Psychotherapy for Maternal Depression: Impact on Mothers and Children. - Abstract - Europe PMC. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 55, 495–503. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology. 1996. Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation. 93, 1043–1065. doi:doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer JF, Ahs F, Fredrikson M, Sollers JJ 3rd, Wager TD, 2012. A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev, 36, 747–756. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vachon D, Krueger R, Rogosch F, Cicchetti D, 2015. Assessment of the Harmful Psychiatric and Behavioral Effects of Different Forms of Child Maltreatment. JAMA Psychiatry. 72, 1135–1142. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1792 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]