Affective neuroscience: Difference between revisions
m Added missing punctuation (apostrophe) and corrected grammar |
m Removed unnecessary words for accuracy and clarity |
||
Line 94: | Line 94: | ||
=== Role of the vmPFC in emotion regulation of older adults === |
=== Role of the vmPFC in emotion regulation of older adults === |
||
The [[ventromedial prefrontal cortex]] (vmPFC) has a significant influence on emotion regulation, especially regarding high emotional arousing stimuli.<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal |last1=Suzuki |first1=Yukihiro |last2=Tanaka |first2=Saori C. |date=2021-09-14 |title=Functions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion regulation under stress |journal=Scientific Reports |language=en |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=18225 |doi=10.1038/s41598-021-97751-0 |issn=2045-2322|doi-access=free |pmid=34521947 |pmc=8440524 |bibcode=2021NatSR..1118225S }}</ref> Compared to other areas of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), the vmPFC loses volume at a much lower rate. Due to this, an older person's emotional regulation abilities are not heavily impacted by brain changes associated with aging. Additionally, the [[anterior cingulate cortex]] (ACC) is an important area of the brain that is used for emotion regulation. The ACC has proven to be a key player in emotion regulation in not just young adults, but also in older adults.<ref name=":2"/> In older adults the ACC is important to create connections with from the vmPFC in order to regulate emotions. This connection was the most salient when negative emotions were reappraised. This demonstrates that older adults use the vmPFC to regulate their emotions in a more positive manner. Despite other areas of the brain decreasing in functionality as humans age, the connection between the vmPFC and ACC remains strong to reappraise negative emotions into more positive emotions. This is different from younger adults who rely |
The [[ventromedial prefrontal cortex]] (vmPFC) has a significant influence on emotion regulation, especially regarding high emotional arousing stimuli.<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal |last1=Suzuki |first1=Yukihiro |last2=Tanaka |first2=Saori C. |date=2021-09-14 |title=Functions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion regulation under stress |journal=Scientific Reports |language=en |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=18225 |doi=10.1038/s41598-021-97751-0 |issn=2045-2322|doi-access=free |pmid=34521947 |pmc=8440524 |bibcode=2021NatSR..1118225S }}</ref> Compared to other areas of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), the vmPFC loses volume at a much lower rate. Due to this, an older person's emotional regulation abilities are not heavily impacted by brain changes associated with aging. Additionally, the [[anterior cingulate cortex]] (ACC) is an important area of the brain that is used for emotion regulation. The ACC has proven to be a key player in emotion regulation in not just young adults, but also in older adults.<ref name=":2"/> In older adults the ACC is important to create connections with from the vmPFC in order to regulate emotions. This connection was the most salient when negative emotions were reappraised. This demonstrates that older adults use the vmPFC to regulate their emotions in a more positive manner. Despite other areas of the brain decreasing in functionality as humans age, the connection between the vmPFC and ACC remains strong to reappraise negative emotions into more positive emotions. This is different from younger adults, who rely more on other areas of the PFC. |
||
=== Neuropsychology behind older adults emotion regulation differences === |
=== Neuropsychology behind older adults emotion regulation differences === |
Latest revision as of 20:38, 15 November 2024
Part of a series on |
Psychology |
---|
Affective neuroscience is the study of how the brain processes emotions. This field combines neuroscience with the psychological study of personality, emotion, and mood.[1] The basis of emotions and what emotions are remains an issue of debate within the field of affective neuroscience.[2]
The term "affective neuroscience" was coined by neuroscientist Jaak Panksepp, at a time when cognitive neuroscience focused on parts of psychology that did not include emotion, such as attention or memory.[3]
Affective neuroscience
[edit]Emotions are thought to be related to activity in brain areas that direct our attention, motivate our behavior, and help us make decisions about our environment. Early stages of research on emotions and the brain was conducted by Paul Broca,[4] James Papez,[5] and Paul D. MacLean.[6] Their work suggests that emotion is related to a group of structures in the center of the brain called the limbic system. The limbic system is made up of the following brain structures:
Limbic system
[edit]- Amygdala – The amygdala is made up of two small, round structures located closer to the forehead (anterior) to the hippocampi near the temporal poles. The amygdalae are involved in detecting and learning which parts of our surroundings are important and have emotional significance.[7][8][9] They are critical for the production of emotion. They are known to be very important for negative emotions, especially fear.[10] Amygdala activation often happens when we see a potential threat. The amygdala uses our past and related memories to help us make decisions about what is currently happening.[11]
- Thalamus – The thalamus is involved in combining sensory and motor signals and then sending that information to the cerebral cortex.[12] The thalamus plays an important role in regulating sleep and wakefulness.[13]
- Hypothalamus – The hypothalamus is involved in producing a physical response (for example, crying) with an emotion. The hypothalamus is also used in reward circuits which are associated with positive emotions.[14]
- Hippocampus – The hippocampus is a structure that is mainly involved in memory.[15] It works to make new memories and also connects senses such as visual input, smell or sound to memories. The hippocampus allows long term memories to be stored and retrieves them when necessary.[16]
- Fornix – The fornix is the main connection between the hippocampus to the mammillary bodies. It is important for spatial memory functions, episodic memory and executive functions.[17]
- Mammillary body – Mammillary bodies are important for recollective memory.[18] They are found by the brain stem and cerebrum.
- Olfactory bulb – The olfactory bulbs are the first cranial nerves.[19] They are involved in smell (olfaction) and memory that is connected with specific smells.[20]
- Cingulate gyrus – The cingulate gyrus is located above the corpus callosum.[21] The parts of the cingulate gyrus have different functions, and are involved with affect, visceromotor control, response selection, skeletomotor control, visuospatial processing, and in memory access.[22] The anterior cingulate cortex is important for conscious, subjective emotional awareness as well as motivation.[23] The subgenual cingulate is more active during both experimentally induced sadness and during depressive episodes.[24]
Research has shown the limbic system is directly related to emotion, but there are other brain areas and structures that are important for producing and processing emotion.[25]
Other brain structures
[edit]- Basal ganglia – Basal ganglia are groups of nuclei found on either side of the thalamus. Basal ganglia play an important role in motivation,[26] action selection and reward learning.[27]
- Orbitofrontal cortex – The orbitofrontal cortex is involved in decision making and helping us understand how emotions have influenced our decision making.[28]
- Prefrontal cortex – The prefrontal cortex is the front of the brain, behind the forehead and above the eyes. It plays a role in regulating emotion and behavior by anticipating consequences. The prefrontal cortex also plays an important role in delayed gratification by maintaining emotions over time and organizing behavior toward specific goals.[29]
- The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is a portion of the prefrontal cortex that has shown to have a significant influence on emotion regulation.[30] Through studies, the vmPFC has shown high activation when presented with highly emotional stimuli. This suggests that this portion of the brain is essential for high emotional arousal.
- Ventral striatum – The ventral striatum is a group of structures thought to play a role in emotion and behavior. An area of the ventral striatum known as the nucleus accumbens is involved in the experience of pleasure.[31] It is common for individuals with addictions to exhibit increased activity in this area when they are exposed to the object of their addiction.
- Insula – This area of the brain plays a significant role in bodily emotions due to its connections to other neural structures that control automatic functions such as heart rate, breathing, and digestion.[32] The insula is also implicated in empathy and awareness of emotion.[33]
- Cerebellum – Cerebellum has many uses. Has a very important role in emotion perception and emotion attributions.[34] Cerebellar dysfunction has been shown to decrease positive emotions during lesion studies.[35] Over the course of evolution, the cerebellum may have evolved into a circuit that helps reduce fear in order to enhance survival. The cerebellum may also play a regulatory role in the neural response to rewarding stimuli, such as money,[36] addictive drugs,[37] and orgasm.[38]
- A "cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome" has been described resulting in personality change and how the person shows emotions.[39]
- Lateral prefrontal cortex – Using our emotions, the lateral prefrontal cortex is responsible for helping us reach our goals by suppressing harmful behaviors or selecting productive ones.[40]
- Primary sensorimotor cortex – The somatosensory cortex is involved in each stage of emotional processing.[41] We use it to collect information that helps us in identifying and creating emotion, and then regulate that emotion once it has started.
- Temporal cortex – This brain area is important in processing sound, speech, and language use, as well as helping us understand others faces and others emotions based on facial cues.[42] The temporal cortex is responsible for determining the quality and content of our emotional memories.[43]
- Brainstem – The brainstem is composed of three parts: ascending (sensory information), descending (motor information), and modulatory.[44] The brainstem takes information from our environment (ascending) and creates a bodily response (descending) such as crying. The information from the environment and our body's responses to the information we receive is combined in the modulatory part of the brain stem and we are then able to label an emotion.
Right hemisphere
[edit]Many theories about the role of the right hemisphere in emotion has resulted in several models of emotional functioning.After observing decreased emotional processing after right hemisphere injuries, C.K. Mills hypothesized emotions are directly related to the right hemisphere.[45][46] In 1992, researchers found that emotional expression and understanding may be controlled by smaller brain structures in the right hemisphere.[47] These findings were the basis for the right hemisphere hypothesis and the valence hypothesis.
Right hemisphere hypothesis
[edit]It is believed that the right hemisphere is more specialized in processing emotions than the left hemisphere.[48] The right hemisphere is associated with nonverbal, synthetic, integrative, holistic and gestaltic mental strategies.[47] As demonstrated by patients who have increased spatial neglect when damage affects the right brain rather than the left brain, the right hemisphere is more connected to subcortical systems of autonomic arousal and attention.[49] Right hemisphere disorders have been associated with abnormal patterns of autonomic nervous system responses.[50] These findings suggest the right hemisphere and subcortical brain areas are closely related.
Valence hypothesis
[edit]According to the valence hypothesis, although the right hemisphere is involved in emotion, it is primarily involved in the processing of negative emotions, while the left hemisphere is involved in processing positive emotions. In one explanation, negative emotions are processed by the right brain, while positive emotions are processed by the left.[51] An alternative explanation is that the right hemisphere is dominant when it comes to feeling both positive and negative emotions.[52][53] Recent studies indicate that the frontal lobes of both hemispheres play an active role in emotions, while the parietal and temporal lobes process them.[54] Depression has been associated with decreased right parietal lobe activity, while anxiety has been associated with increased right parietal lobe activity.[55] Based on the original valence model, increasingly complex models have been developed as a result of the increasing understanding of the different hemispheres.[56]
Cognitive neuroscience
[edit]While emotions are integral to thought processes, cognition has been investigated without emotion until the late 1990s, focusing instead on non-emotional processes such as memory, attention, perception, problem solving, and mental imagery.[57] Cognitive neuroscience and affective neuroscience have emerged as separate fields for studying the neural basis of non-emotional and emotional processes. Despite the fact that fields are classified according to how the brain processes cognition and emotion, the neural and mental mechanisms behind emotional and non-emotional processes often overlap.[58]
Cognitive neuroscience tasks in affective neuroscience research
[edit]Emotion go/no-go
[edit]Emotion go/no-go tasks are used to study behavioral inhibition, especially how it is influenced by emotion.[59] A "go" cue tells the participant to respond rapidly, but a "no-go" cue tells them to withhold a response. Because the "go" cue occurs more frequently, it can be used to measure how well a subject suppresses a response under different emotional conditions.[60]
This task is often used in combination with neuroimaging in healthy individuals and patients with affective disorders to identify relevant brain functions associated with emotional regulation.[59][61][62] Several studies, including go/no-go studies, suggest that sections of the prefrontal cortex are involved in controlling emotional responses to stimuli during inhibition.[63]
Emotional Stroop
[edit]Adapted from the Stroop, the emotional Stroop test measures how much attention you pay to emotional stimuli.[64][65] In this task, participants are instructed to name the ink color of words while ignoring their meanings.[66] Generally, people have trouble detaching their attention from words with an affective meaning compared with neutral words.[67][68] It has been demonstrated in several studies that naming the color of neutral words results in a quicker response.[65]
Selective attention to negative or threatening stimuli, which are often related to psychological disorders, is commonly tested with this task.[69] Different mental disorders have been associated with specific attentional biases.[69][70] Participants with spider phobia, for example, tend to be more inclined to use spider-related words than negatively charged words.[71] Similar findings have been found for threat words related to other anxiety disorders.[69] Even so, other studies have questioned these conclusions. When the words are matched for emotionality, anxious participants in some studies show the Stroop interference effect for both negative and positive words.[72][73] In other words, the specificity effects of words for various disorders may be primarily due to their conceptual relation to the disorder's concerns rather than their emotionality.[69]
Ekman 60 faces task
[edit]The Ekman faces task is used to measure emotion recognition of six basic emotions.[74][75] Black and white photographs of 10 actors (6 male, 4 female) are presented, with each actor displaying each emotion. Participants are usually asked to respond quickly with the name of the displayed emotion. The task is a common tool to study deficits in emotion regulation in patients with dementia, Parkinson's, and other cognitively degenerative disorders.[76] The task has been used to analyze recognition errors in disorders such as borderline personality disorder, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder.[77][78][79]
Dot probe (emotion)
[edit]The emotional dot-probe paradigm is a task used to assess selective visual attention to and failure to detach attention from affective stimuli.[80][81] The paradigm begins with a fixation cross at the center of a screen. An emotional stimulus and a neutral stimulus appear side by side, after which a dot appears behind either the neutral stimulus (incongruent condition) or the affective stimulus (congruent condition). Participants are asked to indicate when they see this dot, and response latency is measured. Dots that appear on the same side of the screen as the image the participant was looking at will be identified more quickly. Thus, it is possible to discern which object the participant was attending to by subtracting the reaction time to respond to congruent versus incongruent trials.[80]
The best documented research with the dot probe paradigm involves attention to threat related stimuli, such as fearful faces, in individuals with anxiety disorders. Anxious individuals tend to respond more quickly to congruent trials, which may indicate vigilance to threat and/or failure to detach attention from threatening stimuli.[80][82] A specificity effect of attention has also been noted, with individuals attending selectively to threats related to their particular disorder. For example, those with social phobia selectively attend to social threats but not physical threats.[83] However, this specificity may be even more nuanced. Participants with obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms initially show attentional bias to compulsive threat, but this bias is attenuated in later trials due to habituation to the threat stimuli.[84]
Fear potentiated startle
[edit]Fear-potentiated startle (FPS) has been utilized as a psychophysiological index of fear reaction in both animals and humans.[85] FPS is most often assessed through the magnitude of the eyeblink startle reflex, which can be measured by electromyography.[86] This eyeblink reflex is an automatic defensive reaction to an abrupt elicitor, making it an objective indicator of fear.[87] Typical FPS paradigms involve bursts of noise or abrupt flashes of light transmitted while an individual attends to a set of stimuli.[87] Startle reflexes have been shown to be modulated by emotion. For example, healthy participants tend to show enhanced startle responses while viewing negatively valenced images and attenuated startle while viewing positively valenced images, as compared with neutral images.[88][89]
The startle response to a particular stimulus is greater under conditions of threat.[90] A common example given to indicate this phenomenon is that one's startle response to a flash of light will be greater when walking in a dangerous neighborhood at night than it would under safer conditions. In laboratory studies, the threat of receiving shock is enough to potentiate startle, even without any actual shock.[91]
Fear potentiated startle paradigms are often used to study fear learning and extinction in individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and other anxiety disorders.[92][93][94] In fear conditioning studies, an initially neutral stimulus is repeatedly paired with an aversive one, borrowing from classical conditioning.[95] FPS studies have demonstrated that PTSD patients have enhanced startle responses during both danger cues and neutral/safety cues as compared with healthy participants.[95][96]
Learning
[edit]Affect plays many roles during learning. Deep, emotional attachment to a subject area allows a deeper understanding of the material and therefore, learning occurs and lasts.[97] The emotions evoked when reading in comparison to the emotions portrayed in the content affects comprehension. Someone who is feeling sad understands a sad passage better than someone feeling happy.[98] Therefore, a student's emotion plays an important role during the learning process.
Emotion can be embodied or perceived from words read on a page or in a facial expression. Neuroimaging studies using fMRI have demonstrated that the same area of the brain that is activated when feeling disgust is activated when observing another's disgust.[99] In a traditional learning environment, the teacher's facial expression can play a critical role in language acquisition. Showing a fearful facial expression when reading passages that contain fearful tones facilitates students learning of the meaning of certain vocabulary words and comprehension of the passage.[100]
Models
[edit]The neurobiological basis of emotion is still disputed.[101] The existence of basic emotions and their defining attributes represents a long lasting and yet unsettled issue in psychology.[101] The available research suggests that the neurobiological existence of basic emotions is still tenable and heuristically seminal, pending some reformulation.[101]
Basic emotions
[edit]These approaches hypothesize that emotion categories (including happiness, sadness, fear, anger, and disgust) are biologically basic.[102][103] In this view, emotions are inherited, biologically based modules that cannot be separated into more basic psychological components.[102][103][104] Models following this approach hypothesize that all mental states belonging to a single emotional category can be consistently and specifically localized to either a single brain region or a defined network of brain regions.[103][105] Each basic emotion category also shares other universal characteristics: distinct facial behavior, physiology, subjective experience and accompanying thoughts and memories.[102]
Psychological constructionist approaches
[edit]This approach to emotion hypothesizes that emotions like happiness, sadness, fear, anger and disgust (and many others) are constructed mental states that occur when brain systems work together.[106] In this view, networks of brain regions underlie psychological operations (e.g., language, attention, etc.) that interact to produce emotion, perception, and cognition.[107] One psychological operation critical for emotion is the network of brain regions that underlie valence (feeling pleasant/unpleasant) and arousal (feeling activated and energized).[106] Emotions emerge when neural systems underlying different psychological operations interact (not just those involved in valence and arousal), producing distributed patterns of activation across the brain. Because emotions emerge from more basic components, heterogeneity affects each emotion category; for example, a person can experience many different kinds of fear, which feel differently, and which correspond to different neural patterns in the brain.[108]
Aging
[edit]People typically associate aging with a decline in the functioning of all mental processing abilities; however, this is not the case for emotion regulation. Older adults typically have a stronger drive to maintain and improve on their emotional well being.[109] Thus providing them to utilize emotion regulation skills that provide a higher satisfaction in life.
Role of the vmPFC in emotion regulation of older adults
[edit]The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) has a significant influence on emotion regulation, especially regarding high emotional arousing stimuli.[110] Compared to other areas of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), the vmPFC loses volume at a much lower rate. Due to this, an older person's emotional regulation abilities are not heavily impacted by brain changes associated with aging. Additionally, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is an important area of the brain that is used for emotion regulation. The ACC has proven to be a key player in emotion regulation in not just young adults, but also in older adults.[109] In older adults the ACC is important to create connections with from the vmPFC in order to regulate emotions. This connection was the most salient when negative emotions were reappraised. This demonstrates that older adults use the vmPFC to regulate their emotions in a more positive manner. Despite other areas of the brain decreasing in functionality as humans age, the connection between the vmPFC and ACC remains strong to reappraise negative emotions into more positive emotions. This is different from younger adults, who rely more on other areas of the PFC.
Neuropsychology behind older adults emotion regulation differences
[edit]As people age, most cognitive functions decline. This is not the case when it comes to emotion regulation. A study conducted by Carstensen and colleagues (2000) found that as people increase in age so does their ability to regulate their emotions.[111] It is important to note that just because older adults had better emotion regulation skills, does not mean they live more stable daily lives. In fact, they tend to have more unstable negative emotions especially in comparison to the stability of their positive emotions.[112] The major difference observed in how older adults and younger adults regulate their emotions when negative emotional stimuli are present can be explained by numerous theories.
Theories of emotion regulation in aging
[edit]Passive method of emotion regulation
[edit]How older adults handle emotionally salient events or stimuli are often vastly different from younger adults, and even middle aged adults. There does not appear to be many differences in ways that younger, middle, and older adults handle social situations; however, when a social situation becomes emotionally charged differences emerge.[113] When intense emotions in a social situation were evoked for older adults, they tended to the situation in a more passive manner in comparison to middle aged adults. They also tend to rely more on their previous problem solving skills than both younger and older adults. This is because as people age, there tends to be a shift in preferences to maintain a more positive emotional affect. In fact, there seems to be a decrease in negative emotions felt by older adults once until they reach the age of 60, in which this decrease stops.[111] It is important to note that while the frequency of negative emotion decreases with age, the intensity of the emotions experienced does not change. Additionally, emotional satisfaction is not lower just because they experience less frequent negative emotions.
Socioemotional selectivity theory
[edit]Carstensen (2003) hypothesized that the reason that older adults tended to have better emotion regulation skills than younger adults is due to the socioemotional selectivity theory.[114] This theory highlights the role of social interactions in the ability to regulate emotions. Social interactions, while are often positive, can sometimes lead to negative emotional arousal. Since older adults have been alive longer, they have more dense social networks. This creates a drastic increase in social interaction that cause positive emotional arousal. On the chance they experience a negative emotional reaction from a social event, they are likely to be able to pair it with something that is more positively emotionally salient. This causes the negative emotion to be less potent, and therefore increase their hedonic perspective on life.
Meta-analyses
[edit]A meta-analysis is a statistical approach to synthesizing results across multiple studies. Included studies investigated healthy, unmedicated adults and that used subtraction analysis to examine brain areas that were more active during emotional processing than during a neutral (control) condition.
Phan et al. 2002
[edit]In the first neuroimaging meta-analysis of emotion, Phan et al. (2002) analyzed the results of 55 peer reviewed studies between January 1990 and December 2000 to determine if the emotions of fear, sadness, disgust, anger, and happiness were consistently associated with activity in specific brain regions. All studies used fMRI or PET techniques to investigate higher-order mental processing of emotion (studies of low-order sensory or motor processes were excluded). The authors' tabulated the number of studies that reported activation in specific brain regions. For each brain region, statistical chi-squared analysis was conducted. Two regions showed a statistically significant association. In the amygdala, 66% of studies inducing fear reported activity in this region, as compared to ~20% of studies inducing happiness, ~15% of studies inducing sadness (with no reported activations for anger or disgust). In the subcallosal cingulate, 46% of studies inducing sadness reported activity in this region, as compared to ~20% inducing happiness and ~20% inducing anger. This pattern of clear discriminability between emotion categories was in fact rare, with other patterns occurring in limbic regions, paralimbic regions, and uni/heteromodal regions. Brain regions implicated across discrete emotion included the basal ganglia (~60% of studies inducing happiness and ~60% of studies inducing disgust reported activity in this region) and medial prefrontal cortex (happiness ~60%, anger ~55%, sadness ~40%, disgust ~40%, and fear ~30%).[115]
Murphy et al. 2003
[edit]Murphy, et al. 2003 analyzed 106 peer reviewed studies published between January 1994 and December 2001 to examine the evidence for regional specialization of discrete emotions (fear, disgust, anger, happiness and sadness) across a larger set of studies. Studies included in the meta-analysis measured activity in the whole brain and regions of interest (activity in individual regions of particular interest to the study). 3-D Kolmogorov-Smirnov (KS3) statistics were used to compare rough spatial distributions of 3-D activation patterns to determine if statistically significant activations were specific to particular brain regions for all emotional categories. This pattern of consistently activated, regionally specific activations was identified in four brain regions: amygdala with fear (~40% of studies), insula with disgust (~70%), globus pallidus with disgust (~70%), and lateral orbitofrontal cortex with anger (80%). Other regions showed different patterns of activation across categories. For example, both the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex and the rostral anterior cingulate cortex showed consistent activity across emotions (happiness ~50%, sadness ~50%, anger ~ 40%, fear ~30%, and disgust ~ 20%).[116]
Barrett et al. 2006
[edit]Barrett, et al. 2006 examined 161 studies published between 1990 and 2001. The authors compared the consistency and specificity of prior meta-analytic findings specific to each notional basic emotion. Consistent neural patterns were defined by brain regions showing increased activity for a specific emotion (relative to a neutral control condition), regardless of the method of induction used (for example, visual vs. auditory cue). Specific neural patterns were defined as separate circuits for one emotion vs. the other emotions (for example, the fear circuit must be discriminable from the anger circuit, although both may include common brain regions). In general, the results supported Phan et al. and Murphy et al., but not specificity. Consistency was determined through the comparison of chi-squared analyses that revealed whether the proportion of studies reporting activation during one emotion was significantly higher than the proportion of studies reporting activation during the other emotions. Specificity was determined through the comparison of emotion-category brain-localizations by contrasting activations in key regions that were specific to particular emotions. Increased amygdala activation during fear was the most consistently reported across induction methods (but not specific). Both meta-analyses associated the anterior cingulate cortex with sadness, although this finding was less consistent (across induction methods) and was not specific. Both meta-analyses found that disgust was associated with the basal ganglia, but these findings were neither consistent nor specific. Neither consistent nor specific activity was observed across the meta-analyses for anger or happiness. This meta-analysis introduced the concept of the basic, irreducible elements of emotional life as dimensions such as approach and avoidance.[106]
Kober et al. 2008
[edit]Kober reviewed 162 neuroimaging studies published between 1990 and 2005 in order to determine if specific brain regions were activated when experiencing an emotion directly and (indirectly) through the experience of someone else.[117] According to the study, six different functional groups showed similar activation patterns. The psychological functions of each group were discussed in more basic terms. These regions may also play a role in processing visual information and paying attention to emotional signals.[118]
Group | Regions | Notes |
---|---|---|
Core limbic | left amygdala, hypothalamus, periaqueductal gray/thalamus regions, and amygdala/ventral striatum/ventral globus pallidus/thalamus regions | A functional emotional center responsible for evaluating affective significance. |
Lateral Paralimbic | ventral anterior insula/frontal operculum/right temporal pole/ posterior orbitofrontal cortex, the anterior insula/ posterior orbitofrontal cortex, the ventral anterior insula/ temporal cortex/ orbitofrontal cortex junction, the midinsula/ dorsal putamen, and the ventral striatum /mid insula/ left hippocampus | Contributes to motivation, especially in reward, by making stimuli more valuable. |
Medial Prefrontal Cortex | dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, pregenual anterior cingulate cortex, and rostral dorsal anterior cingulate cortex | Plays a role in both the generation and regulation of emotion. |
Cognitive/ Motor Network | right frontal operculum, the right interior frontal gyrus, and the pre-supplementray motor area/ left interior frontal gyrus, regions | Plays a general role in information processing and cognitive control. |
Occipital/ Visual Association | V8 and V4 areas of the primary visual cortex, the medial temporal lobe, and the lateral occipital cortex | |
Medial Posterior | posterior cingulate cortex and area V1 of the primary visual cortex |
Vytal et al. 2010
[edit]Vytal, et al. 2010 examined 83 neuroimaging studies published between 1993–2008 to examine whether neuroimaging evidence supports biologically discrete, basic emotions (i.e. fear, anger, disgust, happiness, and sadness). Consistency analyses identified brain regions associated with individual emotions. Discriminability analyses identified brain regions that were differentially active under contrasting pairs of emotions. This meta-analysis examined PET or fMRI studies that reported whole brain analyses identifying significant activations for at least one of the five emotions relative to a neutral or control condition. The authors used activation likelihood estimation (ALE) to perform spatially sensitive, voxel-wise (sensitive to the spatial properties of voxels) statistical comparisons across studies. This technique allows for direct statistical comparison between activation maps associated with each discrete emotion. Thus, discriminability between the five discrete emotion categories was assessed on a more precise spatial scale than in prior meta-analyses.
Consistency was first assessed by comparing the cross-study ALE map for each emotion to ALE maps generated by random permutations. Discriminability was assessed by pair-wise contrasts of emotion maps. Consistent and discriminable activation patterns were observed for the five categories.
Emotion | Peak | Regions |
---|---|---|
Happiness | right superior temporal gyrus, left rostral anterior cingulate cortex | 9 regional brain clusters |
Sadness | left medial frontal gyrus | 35 clusters - especially, left medial frontal gyrus, right middle temporal gyrus, and right inferior frontal gyrus |
Anger | left inferior frontal gyrus | 13 clusters - bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, and in right parahippocampal gyrus |
Fear | left amygdala | 11 clusters - left amygdala and left putamen |
Disgust | right insula/ right inferior frontal gyrus | 16 clusters - right putamen and the left insula. |
Lindquist et al. 2012
[edit]Lindquist, et al. reviewed 91 PET and fMRI studies published between January 1990 and December 2007. Induction methods were used to elicit fear, sadness, disgust, anger, and happiness. The goal was to compare basic emotions approaches with psychological constructionist approaches.[117]
It was found that many brain regions activated consistently or selectively for one emotion category when experienced or perceived. As predicted by constructionist models, no region demonstrated functional specificity for fear, disgust, happiness, sadness, or anger.
The authors suggest that certain brain areas traditionally assigned to certain emotions are incorrect and instead correspond to different emotion categories. There is some evidence that the amygdala, anterior insula, and orbitofrontal cortex all contribute to "core affect", which are feelings of pleasure or discomfort.
Region | Role |
---|---|
Amygdala | Identifying whether external sensory information is motivationally relevant, new, or evokes uncertainty |
Anterior insula | Describes the core affective feelings, mostly driven by body sensations, across all emotion categories |
Orbitofrontal cortex | Guides behavior by combining sensory information from the body and our environment |
The anterior cingulate and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex play a key role in attention, which is closely related to core affect. By using sensory information, the anterior cingulate directs attention and motor responses. According to psychological constructionist theory, emotions are conceptualizations connecting the world and the body, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex facilitates executive attention. As well as playing an active role in conceptualizing, the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus also simulate previous experiences. In several studies, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, which supports language, was consistently active during emotion perception and experience.[107]
See also
[edit]- Affective science
- Affective spectrum
- Affect (psychology)
- Endocrinology
- Experience machine
- Feeling
- Music therapy
- Neuroendocrinology
- Outline of brain mapping
- Outline of the human brain
- Psychiatry
- Psychophysiology
- S.M. (patient)
- Wirehead (science fiction)
References
[edit]- ^ Panksepp, Jaak (1990), "A Role for Affective Neuroscience in Understanding Stress: The Case of Separation Distress Circuitry", Psychobiology of Stress, Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, pp. 41–57, doi:10.1007/978-94-009-1990-7_4, ISBN 978-94-010-7390-5, retrieved 2022-11-27
- ^ Celeghin, Alessia; Diano, Matteo; Bagnis, Arianna; Viola, Marco; Tamietto, Marco (2017-08-24). "Basic Emotions in Human Neuroscience: Neuroimaging and Beyond". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 1432. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 5573709. PMID 28883803.
- ^ Panksepp, Jaak (2004). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions (1st ed.). Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0195178050.
- ^ Broca, Paul (1878). "Anatomie comparée des circonvolutions cérébrales: le grand lobe limbique". Rev. Anthropology. 1: 385–498.
- ^ PAPEZ, JAMES W. (1937-10-01). "A Proposed Mechanism of Emotion". Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry. 38 (4): 725. doi:10.1001/archneurpsyc.1937.02260220069003. ISSN 0096-6754.
- ^ MacLean, Paul D. (1952). "Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (Visceral brain)". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 4 (4): 407–418. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(52)90073-4. ISSN 0013-4694. PMID 12998590.
- ^ The Regents of the University of California. "Behavior". Memory and Aging Center. University of California San Francisco. Retrieved 8 October 2024.
- ^ Šimić, Goran; Tkalčić, Mladenka; Vukić, Vana; Mulc, Damir; Španić, Ena; Šagud, Marina; Olucha-Bordonau, Francisco E.; Vukšić, Mario; R. Hof, Patrick (31 May 2021). "Understanding Emotions: Origins and Roles of the Amygdala". Biomolecules. 11 (6): 823. doi:10.3390/biom11060823. hdl:10234/194226. PMID 34072960.
- ^ Pessoa, Luiz (October 2010). "Emotion and cognition and the amygdala: From "what is it?" to "what's to be done?"". Neuropsychologia. 48 (12): 3416–3429. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.038. PMC 2949460. PMID 20619280.
- ^ LeDoux, Joseph E. (1995). "Emotion: Clues from the Brain". Annual Review of Psychology. 46 (1): 209–235. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233. ISSN 0066-4308. PMID 7872730.
- ^ Breiter, Hans C; Etcoff, Nancy L; Whalen, Paul J; Kennedy, William A; Rauch, Scott L; Buckner, Randy L; Strauss, Monica M; Hyman, Steven E; Rosen, Bruce R (1996). "Response and Habituation of the Human Amygdala during Visual Processing of Facial Expression". Neuron. 17 (5): 875–887. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80219-6. PMID 8938120. S2CID 17284478.
- ^ Sherman, S. (2006). "Thalamus". Scholarpedia. 1 (9): 1583. Bibcode:2006SchpJ...1.1583S. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.1583. ISSN 1941-6016.
- ^ Steriade, M; Llinás, R R (1988). "The functional states of the thalamus and the associated neuronal interplay". Physiological Reviews. 68 (3): 649–742. doi:10.1152/physrev.1988.68.3.649. ISSN 0031-9333. PMID 2839857.
- ^ The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience. Jorge Armony, Patrik Vuilleumier. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 2013. ISBN 978-1-107-31401-6. OCLC 825767970.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: others (link) - ^ Rolls, Edmund T. (2022). "The hippocampus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and episodic and semantic memory". Progress in Neurobiology. 217: 102334. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102334. PMID 35870682. S2CID 250722212.
- ^ Fischer, Håkan; Wright, Christopher I; Whalen, Paul J; McInerney, Sean C; Shin, Lisa M; Rauch, Scott L (2003). "Brain habituation during repeated exposure to fearful and neutral faces: A functional MRI study". Brain Research Bulletin. 59 (5): 387–392. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(02)00940-1. PMID 12507690. S2CID 43705711.
- ^ Modi, Shilpi; Trivedi, Richa; Singh, Kavita; Kumar, Pawan; Rathore, Ram K.S.; Tripathi, Rajendra P.; Khushu, Subash (2013). "Individual differences in trait anxiety are associated with white matter tract integrity in fornix and uncinate fasciculus: Preliminary evidence from a DTI based tractography study". Behavioural Brain Research. 238: 188–192. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.007. PMID 23085341. S2CID 1741104.
- ^ Vann, Seralynne D. (2010). "Re-evaluating the role of the mammillary bodies in memory". Neuropsychologia. 48 (8): 2316–2327. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.019. PMID 19879886. S2CID 2424758.
- ^ Imai, Takeshi (2014). "Construction of functional neuronal circuitry in the olfactory bulb". Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology. 35: 180–188. doi:10.1016/j.semcdb.2014.07.012. ISSN 1084-9521. PMID 25084319.
- ^ Tong, Michelle T.; Peace, Shane T.; Cleland, Thomas A. (2014-07-07). "Properties and mechanisms of olfactory learning and memory". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 8: 238. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00238. ISSN 1662-5153. PMC 4083347. PMID 25071492.
- ^ Rolls, Edmund T. (2019), "The cingulate cortex and limbic systems for action, emotion, and memory", Cingulate Cortex, Handbook of Clinical Neurology, vol. 166, Elsevier, pp. 23–37, doi:10.1016/b978-0-444-64196-0.00002-9, ISBN 978-0-444-64196-0, PMID 31731913, S2CID 208060755, retrieved 2022-11-27
- ^ Weissman, D.H. (2004-07-06). "Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Resolves Conflict from Distracting Stimuli by Boosting Attention toward Relevant Events". Cerebral Cortex. 15 (2): 229–237. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh125. ISSN 1460-2199. PMID 15238434.
- ^ Medford, Nick; Critchley, Hugo D. (2010). "Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response". Brain Structure and Function. 214 (5–6): 535–549. doi:10.1007/s00429-010-0265-x. ISSN 1863-2653. PMC 2886906. PMID 20512367.
- ^ Drevets, Wayne C.; Savitz, Jonathan; Trimble, Michael (2008). "The Subgenual Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Mood Disorders". CNS Spectrums. 13 (8): 663–681. doi:10.1017/S1092852900013754. ISSN 1092-8529. PMC 2729429. PMID 18704022.
- ^ Dalgleish, Tim (2004). "The emotional brain". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 5 (7): 583–589. doi:10.1038/nrn1432. ISSN 1471-003X. PMID 15208700. S2CID 148864726.
- ^ Da Cunha, Claudio; Gomez-A, Alexander; Blaha, Charles D. (2012). "The role of the basal ganglia in motivated behavior". Reviews in the Neurosciences. 23 (5–6): 747–767. doi:10.1515/revneuro-2012-0063. ISSN 0334-1763. PMID 23079510. S2CID 30904211.
- ^ Mikhael, John G.; Bogacz, Rafal (2016-09-02). Blackwell, Kim T. (ed.). "Learning Reward Uncertainty in the Basal Ganglia". PLOS Computational Biology. 12 (9): e1005062. Bibcode:2016PLSCB..12E5062M. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005062. ISSN 1553-7358. PMC 5010205. PMID 27589489.
- ^ Bechara, A. (2000-03-01). "Emotion, Decision Making and the Orbitofrontal Cortex". Cerebral Cortex. 10 (3): 295–307. doi:10.1093/cercor/10.3.295. PMID 10731224.
- ^ Davidson, Richard J; Sutton, Steven K (1995). "Affective neuroscience: the emergence of a discipline". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 5 (2): 217–224. doi:10.1016/0959-4388(95)80029-8. PMID 7620310. S2CID 14639993.
- ^ Suzuki, Yukihiro; Tanaka, Saori C. (2021-09-14). "Functions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion regulation under stress". Scientific Reports. 11 (1): 18225. Bibcode:2021NatSR..1118225S. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-97751-0. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 8440524. PMID 34521947.
- ^ Kringelbach, Morten L.; Berridge, Kent C. (2010). "The functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness". Discovery Medicine. 9 (49): 579–587. ISSN 1944-7930. PMC 3008353. PMID 20587348.
- ^ Rolls, Edmund T. (2016-12-01). "Functions of the anterior insula in taste, autonomic, and related functions". Brain and Cognition. Food for thought: The functional and neural mechanisms of food perception and choice. 110: 4–19. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.07.002. ISSN 0278-2626. PMID 26277487. S2CID 5961714.
- ^ Gu, Xiaosi; Hof, Patrick R.; Friston, Karl J.; Fan, Jin (2013). "Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness: Anterior Insular Cortex and Emotional Awareness". Journal of Comparative Neurology. 521 (15): 3371–3388. doi:10.1002/cne.23368. PMC 3999437. PMID 23749500.
- ^ Adamaszek, Michael; Manto, Mario; Schutter, Dennis J. L. G. (2022), Adamaszek, Michael; Manto, Mario; Schutter, Dennis J. L. G. (eds.), "Current and Future Perspectives of the Cerebellum in Affective Neuroscience", The Emotional Cerebellum, vol. 1378, Cham: Springer International Publishing, pp. 303–313, doi:10.1007/978-3-030-99550-8_19, ISBN 978-3-030-99549-2, PMID 35902479, retrieved 2023-11-16
- ^ Turner, Beth M.; Paradiso, Sergio; Marvel, Cherie L.; Pierson, Ronald; Boles Ponto, Laura L.; Hichwa, Richard D.; Robinson, Robert G. (2007-03-25). "The cerebellum and emotional experience". Neuropsychologia. 45 (6): 1331–1341. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.023. ISSN 0028-3932. PMC 1868674. PMID 17123557.
- ^ Martin-Sölch, C.; Magyar, S.; Künig, G.; Missimer, J.; Schultz, W.; Leenders, K. L. (2001). "Changes in brain activation associated with reward processing in smokers and nonsmokers. A positron emission tomography study". Experimental Brain Research. 139 (3): 278–286. doi:10.1007/s002210100751. ISSN 0014-4819. PMID 11545466. S2CID 29989526.
- ^ Sell, L. A.; Morris, J.; Bearn, J.; Frackowiak, R. S.; Friston, K. J.; Dolan, R. J. (1999). "Activation of reward circuitry in human opiate addicts". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 11 (3): 1042–1048. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00522.x. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-A04C-5. ISSN 0953-816X. PMID 10103096. S2CID 12004748.
- ^ Zhou, Huan-Xiang (2008-11-05). "The debut of PMC Biophysics". PMC Biophysics. 1 (1): 1. doi:10.1186/1757-5036-1-1. ISSN 1757-5036. PMC 2605105. PMID 19351423.
- ^ Parvizi, J. (2001-09-01). "Pathological laughter and crying: A link to the cerebellum". Brain. 124 (9): 1708–1719. doi:10.1093/brain/124.9.1708. PMID 11522574.
- ^ Barbas, Helen; García-Cabezas, Miguel Ángel (2017), "Prefrontal Cortex Integration of Emotion and Cognition", The Prefrontal Cortex as an Executive, Emotional, and Social Brain, Tokyo: Springer Japan, pp. 51–76, doi:10.1007/978-4-431-56508-6_4, ISBN 978-4-431-56506-2, retrieved 2022-11-27
- ^ Kropf, Erika; Syan, Sabrina K.; Minuzzi, Luciano; Frey, Benicio N. (2019). "From anatomy to function: the role of the somatosensory cortex in emotional regulation". Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry. 41 (3): 261–269. doi:10.1590/1516-4446-2018-0183. ISSN 1809-452X. PMC 6794131. PMID 30540029.
- ^ Goghari, V. M.; MacDonald, A. W.; Sponheim, S. R. (2010-05-19). "Temporal Lobe Structures and Facial Emotion Recognition in Schizophrenia Patients and Nonpsychotic Relatives". Schizophrenia Bulletin. 37 (6): 1281–1294. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbq046. ISSN 0586-7614. PMC 3196942. PMID 20484523.
- ^ Kumfor, Fiona; Irish, Muireann; Hodges, John R.; Piguet, Olivier (2014-06-24). "Frontal and temporal lobe contributions to emotional enhancement of memory in behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 8: 225. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00225. ISSN 1662-5153. PMC 4067999. PMID 25009480.
- ^ Venkatraman, Anand; Edlow, Brian L.; Immordino-Yang, Mary Helen (2017-03-09). "The Brainstem in Emotion: A Review". Frontiers in Neuroanatomy. 11: 15. doi:10.3389/fnana.2017.00015. ISSN 1662-5129. PMC 5343067. PMID 28337130.
- ^ Mills, C. K. (1912a). "The cerebral mechanism of emotional expression". Transactions of the College of Physicians of Philadelphia. 34: 381–390.
- ^ Mills, C. K. (1912b). "The cortical representation of emotion, with a discussion of some points in the general nervous mechanism of expression in its relation to organic nervous disease and insanity". Proceedings of the American Medico-Psychological Association. 19: 297–300.
- ^ a b Borod, J. C. (1992). "Intel-hemispheric and intrahemispheric control of emotion: A focus on unilateral brain damage". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 60 (3): 339–348. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.60.3.339. PMID 1619088.
- ^ Borod, J. C., Koff, E., & Caron, H. S. (1983). Right hemispheric specialization for the expression and appreciation of emotion: A focus on the face. In E. Perecman (Ed.), Cognitive functions in the right hemisphere (pp. 83-110). New York: Academic Press.
- ^ Heilman, K. M. (February 1982). Discussant comments. In J. C. Borod & R. Buck (Chairs), Asymmetries in facial expression: Method and meaning. Symposium conducted at the International Neuropsychological Society, Pittsburgh, PA.
- ^ Yokoyama, K.; Jennings, R.; Ackles, P.; Hood, B. S.; Boiler, F. (1987). "Lack of heart rate changes during an attention-demanding task after right hemisphere lesions". Neurology. 37 (4): 624–630. doi:10.1212/wnl.37.4.624. PMID 3561774. S2CID 38096461.
- ^ Silberman, E. K.; Weingartner, H. (1986). "Hemispheric lateralization of functions related to emotion". Brain and Cognition. 5 (3): 322–353. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(86)90035-7. PMID 3530287. S2CID 2636348.
- ^ Fox, N. A. (1991). "If it's not left, it's right". American Psychologist. 46 (8): 863–872. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.46.8.863. PMID 1928939.
- ^ Davidson, R.; Ekman, P.; Saron, C. D.; Senulis, J. A.; Friesen, W V (1990). "Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology I". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 58 (2): 330–341. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.330. PMID 2319445.
- ^ Borod, J.C.; Cicero, B.A.; Obler, L.K.; Welkowitz, J.; Erhan, H.M.; Santschi, C.; et al. (1998). "Right hemisphere emotional perception: Evidence across multiple channels". Neuropsychology. 12 (3): 446–458. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.446. PMID 9673999.
- ^ Engels, AS; Heller, W; Mohanty, A; Herrington, JD; Banich, MT; Webb, AG; Miller, GA (2007). "Specificity of regional brain activity in anxiety types during emotion processing". Psychophysiology. 44 (3): 352–363. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00518.x. PMID 17433094.
- ^ Spielberg, J.; Stewart, J; Levin, R.; Miller, G.; Heller, W. (2008). "Prefrontal Cortex, Emotion, and Approach/Withdrawal Motivation". Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2 (1): 135–153. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00064.x. PMC 2889703. PMID 20574551.
- ^ Cacioppo, J.T.; Gardner, W.L. (1999). "Emotion". Annual Review of Psychology. 50: 191–214. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.191. PMID 10074678.
- ^ Davidson, R.J. (2000). "Cognitive neuroscience needs affective neuroscience (and vice versa)". Brain and Cognition. 42 (1): 89–92. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.487.9015. doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1170. PMID 10739607. S2CID 22183667.
- ^ a b Drevets, W.C.; Raichle, M.E. (1998). "Reciprocal suppression of regional cerebral blood flow during emotional versus higher cognitive processes: Implications for interactions between emotion and cognition". Cognition and Emotion. 12 (3): 353–385. doi:10.1080/026999398379646.
- ^ Schulz, K.P.; Fan, J.; Magidina, O.; Marks, D.J.; Hahn, B.; Halperin, J.M. (2007). "Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (2): 151–160. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.001. PMC 2562664. PMID 17207962.
- ^ Elliott, R.; Ogilvie, A.; Rubinsztein, J.S.; Calderon, G.; Dolan, R.J.; Sahakian, B.J. (2004). "Abnormal ventral frontal response during performance of an affective go/no go task in patients with mania". Biological Psychiatry. 55 (12): 1163–1170. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.03.007. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-A247-8. PMID 15184035. S2CID 6511699.
- ^ Elliott, R.; Rubinsztein, J.S.; Sahakian, B.J.; Dolan, R.J. (2000). "Selective attention to emotional stimuli in a verbal go/no-go task: An fMRI study". Brain Imaging. 11 (8): 1739–1744. doi:10.1097/00001756-200006050-00028. PMID 10852235. S2CID 16872952.
- ^ Hare, T.A.; Tottenham, N.; Davidson, M.C.; Glover, G.H.; Casey, B.J. (2005). "Contributions of amygdala and striatal activity in emotion regulation". Biological Psychiatry. 57 (6): 624–632. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.038. PMID 15780849. S2CID 17690328.
- ^ Stroop, J.R. (1935). "Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 18 (6): 643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-5ADB-7.
- ^ a b Epp, A.M.; Dobson, K.S.; Dozois, D.J.A.; Frewen, P.A. (2012). "A systematic meta-analysis of the stroop task in depression". Clinical Psychology Review. 32 (4): 316–328. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.02.005. PMID 22459792.
- ^ Pratto, F.; John, O. P. (1991). "Automatic vigilance: The attention grabbing power of negative social information". Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 61 (3): 380–391. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380. PMID 1941510.
- ^ Wentura, D.; Rothermund, K.; Bak, P. (2000). "Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of approach and avoidance-related social information". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (6): 1024–1037. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.6.1024. PMID 10870906.
- ^ Williams, J.M.; Broadbent, K. (1986). "Distraction by emotional stimuli: use of a Stroop task with suicide attempters". British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 25 (2): 101–110. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1986.tb00678.x. PMID 3730646.
- ^ a b c d Williams, M.G.; Matthews, A.; MacLeod, C. (1996). "The emotional stroop task and psychopathology". Psychological Bulletin. 120 (1): 3–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3. PMID 8711015.
- ^ Gotlib, I.H., Roberts, J.E., & Gilboa, E. (1996). Cognitive interference in depression. in I.G. Sarason, G.R. Pierce, & B.R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc., Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 347–377.
- ^ Watts, E N.; McKenna, E P.; Sharrock, R.; Trezise, L. (1986). "Colour naming of phobia-related words". British Journal of Psychology. 77: 97–108. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb01985.x. PMID 2869817.
- ^ Martin, M.; Williams, R. M.; Clark, D. M. (1991). "Does anxiety lead to selective processing of threat-related information?". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 29 (2): 147–160. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(91)90043-3. PMID 2021377.
- ^ Mogg, K.; Mathews, A. M.; Weinman, J. (1989). "Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states: A replication". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 27 (4): 317–323. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(89)90001-6. PMID 2775141.
- ^ Diehl-Schmid, J.; Pohl, C.; Ruprecht, C.; Wagenpfeil, S.; Foerstl, H.; Kurz, A. (2007). "The Ekman 60 faces test as a diagnostic instrument in frontotemporal dementia". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (4): 459–464. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.024. PMID 17360152.
- ^ Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.
- ^ Diehl-Schmida, J.; Pohla, C.; Ruprechta, C.; Wagenpfeilb, S.; Foerstla, H.; Kurza, A. (2007). "The Ekman 60 faces test as a diagnostic instrument in frontotemporal dementia". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (4): 459–464. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.024. PMID 17360152.
- ^ Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N.; Junque, C.; Tolosa, E.; Marti, M.; Valldeoriola, F.; Bargalloand, N.; Zareio, M. (2009). "Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired decision-making and facial emotion recognition in early Parkinson's disease". European Journal of Neuroscience. 30 (6): 1162–1171. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06892.x. PMID 19735293. S2CID 413809.
- ^ Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G.; Bio, D.S.; David, D.P.; Santos, D.R.; Kerr, D.S.; Moreno, R.A.; Machado-Vieira, Rodrigo; Moreno, Ricardo Albeto (2012). "COMT Met (158) modulates facial emotion recognition in bipolar I disorder mood episodes". Journal of Affective Disorders. 136 (3): 370–376. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2011.11.021. PMID 22222175.
- ^ Ebert, A.; Haussleiter, I.S.; Juckel, G.; Brune, M.; Roser, P. (2012). "Impaired facial emotion recognition in a ketamine model of psychosis". Psychiatry Research. 200 (2–3): 724–727. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.034. PMID 22776754. S2CID 43744551.
- ^ a b c Koster, E.H.W.; Crombez, G.; Verschuere, B.; De Houwer, J. (2004). "Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 42 (10): 1183–1192. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001. PMID 15350857.
- ^ MacLeod, C.; Mathews, A.; Tata, P. (1986). "Attentional bias in emotional disorders". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 95 (1): 15–20. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.95.1.15. PMID 3700842.
- ^ Mogg, K.; Bradley, B. P. (1998). "A cognitive–motivational analysis of anxiety". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 36 (9): 809–848. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00063-1. PMID 9701859.
- ^ Asmundson, G.J.G.; Stein, M.B. (1994). "Selective processing of social threat in patients with generalized social phobia: Evaluation using a dot-probe paradigm". Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 8 (2): 107–117. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(94)90009-4.
- ^ Amir, N.; Naimi, S.; Morrison, A.S. (2009). "Attenuation of attention bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 47 (2): 153–157. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.020. PMC 2662360. PMID 19046576.
- ^ Davis, M. (1986). "Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of fear conditioning using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm". Behavioral Neuroscience. 100 (6): 814–824. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.100.6.814. PMID 3545257.
- ^ Baskin-Sommers, A.R.; Vitale, J.E.; MacCoon, D.; Newman, J.P. (2012). "Assessing emotion sensitivity in female offenders with borderline personality symptoms: Results from a fear-potentiated startle paradigm". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121 (2): 477–483. doi:10.1037/a0026753. PMC 3358451. PMID 22250659.
- ^ a b Vaidyanathan, U.; Patrick, C.J.; Cuthbert, B.N. (2009). "Linking dimensional models of internalizing psychopathology to neurobiological systems: Affect-modulated startle as an indicator of fear and distress disorders and affiliated traits". Psychological Bulletin. 135 (6): 909–942. doi:10.1037/a0017222. PMC 2776729. PMID 19883142.
- ^ Lang, P. J.; Bradley, M. M.; Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). "Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex". Psychological Review. 97 (3): 377–395. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.97.3.377. PMID 2200076.
- ^ Vrana, S. R.; Spence, E. L.; Lang, P. J. (1988). "The startle probe response: A new measure of emotion?". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 97 (4): 487–491. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.97.4.487. PMID 3204235.
- ^ Norrholm, S.D.; Anderson, K.M.; Olin, I.W.; Jovanovic, T.; Kwon, C.; Warren, Victor T.; Bradley, B.; Bosshardt, Lauren; Sabree, Justin; Duncan, Erica J.; Rothbaum, Barbara O.; Bradley, Bekh (2011). "Versatility of fear-potentiated startle paradigms for assessing human conditioned fear extinction and return of fear". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 5: 77. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00077. PMC 3221285. PMID 22125516.
- ^ Grillon, C.; Ameli, R.; Woods, S. W.; Merikangas, K.; Davis, M. (1991). "Fear-potentiated startle in humans: Effects of anticipatory anxiety on the acoustic blink reflex". Psychophysiology. 28 (5): 588–595. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb01999.x. PMID 1758934.
- ^ Norrholm, S.; Jovanovic, Tanja (2011). "Translational fear inhibition models as indices of trauma-related psychopathology". Current Psychiatry Reviews. 7 (3): 194–204. doi:10.2174/157340011797183193.
- ^ Norrholm, S. D.; Jovanovic, T.; Olin, I. W.; Sands, L.; Karapanou, I.; Bradley, B.; Ressler, K. J. (2010). "Fear extinction in traumatized civilians with posttraumatic stress disorder: relation to symptom severity". Biological Psychiatry. 69 (6): 556–563. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.013. PMC 3052965. PMID 21035787.
- ^ Lang, P. J.; McTeague, L. M. (2009). "The anxiety disorder spectrum: Fear imagery, physiological reactivity, and differential diagnosis". Anxiety, Stress & Coping. 22 (1): 5–25. doi:10.1080/10615800802478247. PMC 2766521. PMID 19096959.
- ^ a b Cousens, G.A.; Kearns, A.; Laterza, F.; Tundidor, J. (2012). "Excitotoxic lesions of the medial amygdala attenuate olfactory fear-potentiated startle and conditioned freezing behavior". Behavioural Brain Research. 229 (2): 427–432. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.01.011. PMID 22249137. S2CID 25116098.
- ^ Jovanovic, T; Norrholm, Seth D.; Fennell, Jennifer E.; Keyes, Megan; Fiallos, Ana M.; Myers, Karyn M.; Davis, Michael; Duncan, Erica J. (15 May 2009). "Posttraumatic stress disorder may be associated with impaired fear inhibition: relation to symptom severity". Psychiatry Research. 167 (1–2): 151–160. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.12.014. PMC 2713500. PMID 19345420.
- ^ Picard, R. W.; Papert, S; Bender, W; Blumberg, B; Breazeal, C; Cavallo, D; Machover, T; Resnick, M; Roy, D; Strohecker, C (2004). "Affective Learning – a manifesto". BT Technology Journal. 22 (4): 253–269. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.110.3308. doi:10.1023/b:bttj.0000047603.37042.33. S2CID 518899.
- ^ Havas, D.A.; Glenberg, A.M.; Rinck, M. (2007). "Emotion simulation during language comprehension". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 14 (3): 436–441. doi:10.3758/bf03194085. hdl:2066/55501. PMID 17874584.
- ^ Wicker, B.; Keysers, Christian; Plailly, Jane; Royet, Jean-Pierre; Gallese, Vittorio; Rizzolatti, Giacomo (2003). "Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula: The Common Neural Basis of Seeing and Feeling Disgust". Neuron. 40 (3): 655–664. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2. PMID 14642287. S2CID 766157.
- ^ Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). "Embodying emotion". Science. 316 (5827): 1002–1005. Bibcode:2007Sci...316.1002N. doi:10.1126/science.1136930. PMID 17510358. S2CID 14537829.
- ^ a b c Celeghin, Alessia; Diano, Matteo; Bagnis, Arianna; Viola, Marco; Tamietto, Marco (2017). "Basic Emotions in Human Neuroscience: Neuroimaging and Beyond". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 1432. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 5573709. PMID 28883803. This article incorporates text by Alessia Celeghin, Matteo Diano, Arianna Bagnis, Marco Viola, and Marco Tamietto available under the CC BY 4.0 license.
- ^ a b c Ekman, P.; Cordaro, D. (2011). "What is meant by calling emotions basic". Emotion Review. 3 (4): 364–370. doi:10.1177/1754073911410740. S2CID 52833124.
- ^ a b c Panksepp, J.; Watt, D. (2011). "What is basic about basic emotions? Lasting lessons from affective neuroscience". Emotion Review. 3 (4): 387–396. doi:10.1177/1754073911410741. S2CID 144198152.
- ^ Izard, C.E. (2011). "Forms and functions in emotions: Matters of emotion-cognition interactions". Emotion Review. 3 (4): 371–378. doi:10.1177/1754073911410737. S2CID 144037625.
- ^ Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
- ^ a b c Barrett, L.F.; Wager, T. (2006). "The structure of emotion: Evidence from the neuroimaging of emotion". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 15 (2): 79–85. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.470.7762. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00411.x. S2CID 14489624.
- ^ a b Lindquist, K.; Wager, T.; Kober, Hedy; Bliss-Moreau, Eliza; Barrett, Lisa Feldman (2012). "The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 35 (3): 121–143. doi:10.1017/s0140525x11000446. PMC 4329228. PMID 22617651.
- ^ Fox, Andrew S.; Shackman, Alexander J. (February 2019). "The central extended amygdala in fear and anxiety: Closing the gap between mechanistic and neuroimaging research". Neuroscience Letters. 693: 58–67. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2017.11.056. ISSN 0304-3940. PMC 5976525. PMID 29195911.
- ^ a b Allard, Eric; Kensinger, Elizabeth (November 10, 2014). "Age-Related Differences in Functional Connectivity During Cognitive Emotion Regulation". The Journals of Gerontology. Retrieved 2023-10-25.
- ^ Suzuki, Yukihiro; Tanaka, Saori C. (2021-09-14). "Functions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion regulation under stress". Scientific Reports. 11 (1): 18225. Bibcode:2021NatSR..1118225S. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-97751-0. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 8440524. PMID 34521947.
- ^ a b Carstensen, Laura; Pasupathi, Monisha; Ulrich, Mayr; Nesselroade, John (2000). "Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 79 (4): 644–655. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.644. PMID 11045744. Retrieved 2023-10-26.
- ^ Urry, Heather L.; Gross, James J. (2010). "Emotion Regulation in Older Age". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 19 (6): 352–357. doi:10.1177/0963721410388395. ISSN 0963-7214. S2CID 1400335.
- ^ Blanchard-Fields, Fredda; Stein, Renee; Watson, Tonya (2004). "Age Differences in Emotion-Regulation Strategies in Handling Everyday Problems". The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 59 (6). The Journals of Gerontology: 261–9. doi:10.1093/geronb/59.6.p261. PMID 15576853. Retrieved 2023-10-26.
- ^ Carstensen, Laura L.; Fung, Helene H.; Charles, Susan T. (2003-06-01). "Socioemotional Selectivity Theory and the Regulation of Emotion in the Second Half of Life". Motivation and Emotion. 27 (2): 103–123. doi:10.1023/A:1024569803230. ISSN 1573-6644. S2CID 143149171.
- ^ Phan, K.L.; Wager, T.D.; Taylor, S.F.; Liberzon, I. (2002). "Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI". NeuroImage. 16 (2): 331–348. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. PMID 12030820. S2CID 7150871.
- ^ Murphy, F.C.; Nimmo-Smith, I.; Lawrence, A.D. (2003). "Functional Neuroanatomy: A meta-analysis". Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 3 (3): 207–233. doi:10.3758/cabn.3.3.207. PMID 14672157.
- ^ a b Wager, T.D.; Lindquist, M.; Kaplan, L. (2007). "Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data: Current and future directions". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2 (2): 150–158. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm015. PMC 2555451. PMID 18985131.
- ^ Kober, H.; Barrett, L.F.; Joseph, J.; Bliss-Moreau, E.; Lindquist, K.; Wager, T.D. (2008). "Functional grouping and cortical-subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies". NeuroImage. 42 (2): 998–1031. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.059. PMC 2752702. PMID 18579414.
- ^ Vytal, K.; Hamann, S. (2010). "Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 22 (12): 2864–2885. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21366. PMID 19929758. S2CID 18151469.
Further reading
[edit]- Davidson, R.J.; Irwin, W. (1999). "The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 3 (1): 11–21. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01265-0. PMID 10234222. S2CID 30912026.
- Panksepp, J. (1992). "A critical role for affective neuroscience in resolving what is basic about basic emotions". Psychological Review. 99 (3): 554–60. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.99.3.554. PMID 1502276.
- Harmon-Jones E, & Winkielman P. (Eds.) Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of Social Behavior. New York: Guilford Publications.
- Cacioppo, J.T., & Berntson, G.G. (2005). Social Neuroscience. Psychology Press.
- Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L.G., & Berntson, G.G. (2007). Handbook of Psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press.
- Panksepp J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions (Series in Affective Science). Oxford University Press, New York, New York.
- Brain and Cognition, Vol. 52, No. 1, pp. 1–133 (June, 2003). Special Issue on Affective Neuroscience.