Jump to content

Cross-race effect: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
MCJones20 (talk | contribs)
m Fixed typo (Graphs)
consistent citation formatting; templated cites; fixed date errors
Line 1: Line 1:
{{primary sources|date=April 2013}}
{{primary sources|date=April 2013}}
{{race}}
{{race}}
The '''cross-race effect''' (sometimes called '''cross-race bias''', '''other-race bias''' or '''own-race bias''') is the tendency to more easily [[Face perception|recognize faces]] that belong to one's own racial group. In social psychology, the cross-race effect is described as the "ingroup advantage," whereas in other fields, the effect can be seen as a specific form of the "ingroup advantage" since it is only applied in interracial or inter-ethnic situations.<ref>e.g. M. Beaupre (2006): An Ingroup Advantage for Confidence in Emotion Recognition Judgments: The Moderating Effect of Familiarity With the Expressions of Outgroup Members. ''Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin'', volume 32(1), pages 16–26.</ref> The cross-race affect is thought to contribute to difficulties in cross-race identification, as well as implicit racial bias.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Lebrecht|first=Sophie|last2=Pierce|first2=Lara J.|last3=Tarr|first3=Michael J.|last4=Tanaka|first4=James W.|date=2009-01-21|title=Perceptual Other-Race Training Reduces Implicit Racial Bias|url=https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0004215|journal=PLOS ONE|language=en|volume=4|issue=1|pages=e4215|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0004215|issn=1932-6203}}</ref>
The '''cross-race effect''' (sometimes called '''cross-race bias''', '''other-race bias''' or '''own-race bias''') is the tendency to more easily [[Face perception|recognize faces]] that belong to one's own racial group. In social psychology, the cross-race effect is described as the "ingroup advantage," whereas in other fields, the effect can be seen as a specific form of the "ingroup advantage" since it is only applied in interracial or inter-ethnic situations.<ref>e.g. M. Beaupre (2006): An Ingroup Advantage for Confidence in Emotion Recognition Judgments: The Moderating Effect of Familiarity With the Expressions of Outgroup Members. ''Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin'', volume 32(1), pages 16–26.</ref> The cross-race affect is thought to contribute to difficulties in cross-race identification, as well as implicit racial bias.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Lebrecht S, Pierce LJ, Tarr MJ, Tanaka JW | title = Perceptual other-race training reduces implicit racial bias | journal = PloS One | volume = 4 | issue = 1 | pages = e4215 | date = 2009-01-21 | pmid = 19156226 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0004215 }}</ref>


Multiple theories as to why the cross-race effect exists have been conceived, including social cognition and perceptual expertise. However, no model has been able to fully account for the full body of evidence, so multiple theories are still considered in the literature. <ref>{{Cite journal|last=Young|first=Steven G.|last2=Hugenberg|first2=Kurt|last3=Bernstein|first3=Michael J.|last4=Sacco|first4=Donald F.|date=2012-05-01|title=Perception and Motivation in Face Recognition: A Critical Review of Theories of the Cross-Race Effect|url=https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311418987|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Review|language=en|volume=16|issue=2|pages=116–142|doi=10.1177/1088868311418987|issn=1088-8683}}</ref> 
Multiple theories as to why the cross-race effect exists have been conceived, including social cognition and perceptual expertise. However, no model has been able to fully account for the full body of evidence, so multiple theories are still considered in the literature. <ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Young SG, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ, Sacco DF | title = Perception and motivation in face recognition: a critical review of theories of the Cross-Race Effect | journal = Personality and Social Psychology Review | volume = 16 | issue = 2 | pages = 116–42 | date = May 2012 | pmid = 21878608 | doi = 10.1177/1088868311418987 }}</ref> 


== History ==
== History ==
The first research study on the cross-race effect was published in 1914.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Feingold|first1=CA|year=1914|title=The influence of environment on identification of persons and things|url=https://scholarlycommons.law.northwestern.edu/jclc/vol5/iss1/6|journal=Journal of Criminal Law and Police Science|volume=5|issue=1|pages=39–51|doi=10.2307/1133283|jstor=1133283}}</ref> It stated that humans tend to perceive people of other races than themselves to all look alike. All else being equal, individuals of a given race are distinguishable from each other in proportion to their familiarity or contact with the race as a whole. Thus, to the uninitiated Caucasian, all East Asian people look alike, while to East Asian people, all Caucasian people look alike. This does not hold true when people of different races familiarize themselves with races different from their own.
The first research study on the cross-race effect was published in 1914.<ref>{{cite journal| vauthors = Feingold CA |year=1914|title=The influence of environment on identification of persons and things|url=https://scholarlycommons.law.northwestern.edu/jclc/vol5/iss1/6|journal=Journal of Criminal Law and Police Science|volume=5|issue=1|pages=39–51|doi=10.2307/1133283|jstor=1133283}}</ref> It stated that humans tend to perceive people of other races than themselves to all look alike. All else being equal, individuals of a given race are distinguishable from each other in proportion to their familiarity or contact with the race as a whole. Thus, to the uninitiated Caucasian, all East Asian people look alike, while to East Asian people, all Caucasian people look alike. This does not hold true when people of different races familiarize themselves with races different from their own.


As research into the cross-race effect continued, multiple theories arose as to why the effect exists, including the contact hypothesis, different processing mechanisms, social cognition, feature selection, and cognitive disregard. However, each of these theories have their own challenges and conflicting evidence.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Young|first=Steven G.|last2=Hugenberg|first2=Kurt|last3=Bernstein|first3=Michael J.|last4=Sacco|first4=Donald F.|date=2012-05-01|title=Perception and Motivation in Face Recognition: A Critical Review of Theories of the Cross-Race Effect|url=https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311418987|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Review|language=en|volume=16|issue=2|pages=116–142|doi=10.1177/1088868311418987|issn=1088-8683}}</ref>
As research into the cross-race effect continued, multiple theories arose as to why the effect exists, including the contact hypothesis, different processing mechanisms, social cognition, feature selection, and cognitive disregard. However, each of these theories have their own challenges and conflicting evidence.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Young SG, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ, Sacco DF | title = Perception and motivation in face recognition: a critical review of theories of the Cross-Race Effect | journal = Personality and Social Psychology Review | volume = 16 | issue = 2 | pages = 116–42 | date = May 2012 | pmid = 21878608 | doi = 10.1177/1088868311418987 }}</ref>


Numerous studies on the cross-race effect have occurred. However, improvements in technology has allowed for researchers to study the cross-race effect from a neural and/or computational lens. Despite both occurring under the umbrella of facial processing, researchers have found that different areas of the brain activate when processing own-race vs other-race faces.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Natu|first=Vaidehi|last2=O'Toole|first2=Alice J.|date=2013-09-01|title=Neural perspectives on the other-race effect|url=https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2013.811455|journal=Visual Cognition|volume=21|issue=9-10|pages=1081–1095|doi=10.1080/13506285.2013.811455|issn=1350-6285}}</ref> Research using computational models have found that the other race effect only occurs when facial processing and feature selection is influenced by biased experience.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=O'Toole|first=Alice J.|last2=Natu|first2=Vaidehi|date=2013-09-01|title=Computational perspectives on the other-race effect|url=https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2013.803505|journal=Visual Cognition|volume=21|issue=9-10|pages=1121–1137|doi=10.1080/13506285.2013.803505|issn=1350-6285}}</ref> However, as use of these methods to examine the cross-race effect is relatively new, further research is still needed.
Numerous studies on the cross-race effect have occurred. However, improvements in technology has allowed for researchers to study the cross-race effect from a neural and/or computational lens. Despite both occurring under the umbrella of facial processing, researchers have found that different areas of the brain activate when processing own-race vs other-race faces.<ref>{{Cite journal| vauthors = Natu V, O'Toole AJ |date=2013-09-01|title=Neural perspectives on the other-race effect |journal=Visual Cognition|volume=21|issue=9-10|pages=1081–1095|doi=10.1080/13506285.2013.811455 }}</ref> Research using computational models have found that the other race effect only occurs when facial processing and feature selection is influenced by biased experience.<ref>{{Cite journal| vauthors = O'Toole AJ, Natu V |date=2013-09-01|title=Computational perspectives on the other-race effect |journal=Visual Cognition|volume=21|issue=9-10|pages=1121–1137|doi=10.1080/13506285.2013.803505 }}</ref> However, as use of these methods to examine the cross-race effect is relatively new, further research is still needed.


==Theoretical approaches==
==Theoretical approaches==
Line 19: Line 19:


===Cross-race effect and emotion recognition===
===Cross-race effect and emotion recognition===
A [[meta-analysis]] of several studies about [[emotion recognition]] in facial expressions revealed that people could recognize and interpret the emotional facial expression of a person of their own race faster and better than of a person of another race. These findings apply to all races in the same way.<ref>Elfenbein, H.A. & Ambidi, N. (2002): On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. ''Psychological Bulletin'', Band 128(2), S. 203–235</ref> Some studies show that other races, compared to one's own race, have differently shaped faces and different details within a facial expression, making it difficult for members of other races to decode emotional expressions.<ref>Anthony, T.; Cooper, C. & Mullen, B. (1992): Cross-racial facial identification: A social cognitive integration. ''Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin'', Band 18, S. 296–301</ref><ref name=Sporer>{{cite journal|last=Sporer|first=Siegfried Ludwig|title=Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories.|journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law|date=1 January 2001|volume=7|issue=1|pages=36–97|doi=10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.36}}</ref> However, studies have shown that the mood of the observer does not affect the cross-race effect.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Teitelbaum|first=S.|author2=Geiselman, R. E.|title=Observer Mood and Cross-Racial Recognition of Faces|journal=Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology|date=1 January 1997|volume=28|issue=1|pages=93–106|doi=10.1177/0022022197281006}}</ref>
A [[meta-analysis]] of several studies about [[emotion recognition]] in facial expressions revealed that people could recognize and interpret the emotional facial expression of a person of their own race faster and better than of a person of another race. These findings apply to all races in the same way.<ref>Elfenbein, H.A. & Ambidi, N. (2002): On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. ''Psychological Bulletin'', Band 128(2), S. 203–235</ref> Some studies show that other races, compared to one's own race, have differently shaped faces and different details within a facial expression, making it difficult for members of other races to decode emotional expressions.<ref>Anthony, T.; Cooper, C. & Mullen, B. (1992): Cross-racial facial identification: A social cognitive integration. ''Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin'', Band 18, S. 296–301</ref><ref name=Sporer>{{cite journal|vauthors = Sporer SL |title=Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories.|journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law|date=1 January 2001|volume=7|issue=1|pages=36–97|doi=10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.36}}</ref> However, studies have shown that the mood of the observer does not affect the cross-race effect.<ref>{{cite journal| vauthors = Teitelbaum S, Geiselman RE |title=Observer Mood and Cross-Racial Recognition of Faces|journal=Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology|date=1 January 1997|volume=28|issue=1|pages=93–106|doi=10.1177/0022022197281006}}</ref>


===Social cognition===
===Social cognition===
[[File:Other Race Effect Evidence.webp|thumb|Figure 2 from "A Memory Computational Basis for the Other-Race Effect" (Yaros et al., 2019) that show evidence of the other race effect occurring when participants do a Mnemonic discrimination task but not a match to sample task, showing that mnemonic and proactive interference (old memories interfere with new) may contribute to the other-race effect (cross-race effect)<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Yaros|first=Jessica L.|last2=Salama|first2=Diana A.|last3=Delisle|first3=Derek|last4=Larson|first4=Myra S.|last5=Miranda|first5=Blake A.|last6=Yassa|first6=Michael A.|date=2019-12-18|title=A Memory Computational Basis for the Other-Race Effect|url=https://www.nature.com/articles/s41598-019-55350-0|journal=Scientific Reports|language=en|volume=9|issue=1|pages=19399|doi=10.1038/s41598-019-55350-0|issn=2045-2322}}</ref>]]
[[File:Other Race Effect Evidence.webp|thumb|Figure 2 from "A Memory Computational Basis for the Other-Race Effect" (Yaros et al., 2019) that show evidence of the other race effect occurring when participants do a Mnemonic discrimination task but not a match to sample task, showing that mnemonic and proactive interference (old memories interfere with new) may contribute to the other-race effect (cross-race effect)<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Yaros JL, Salama DA, Delisle D, Larson MS, Miranda BA, Yassa MA | title = A Memory Computational Basis for the Other-Race Effect | journal = Scientific Reports | volume = 9 | issue = 1 | pages = 19399 | date = December 2019 | pmid = 31853093 | doi = 10.1038/s41598-019-55350-0 }}</ref>]]
Research has shown that people tend to think more categorically about outgroup members and more individually about ingroup members.<ref name=You /> For example, outgroup members may associate specific facial features with a particular race or ethnicity, and do not notice the subtle variations in skin tone, lip size, or brow strength that ingroup members recognize. Categorical thinking happens more consistently for outgroup participants while individuation does the exact opposite.<ref name=You /> These different views between outgroup and ingroup members have been known to bias conceptual cognitive processes and show that the cross-race effect actually has less to do with race than with different levels of cognitive processing that occur for ingroup and outgroup members.<ref name=You />
Research has shown that people tend to think more categorically about outgroup members and more individually about ingroup members.<ref name=You /> For example, outgroup members may associate specific facial features with a particular race or ethnicity, and do not notice the subtle variations in skin tone, lip size, or brow strength that ingroup members recognize. Categorical thinking happens more consistently for outgroup participants while individuation does the exact opposite.<ref name=You /> These different views between outgroup and ingroup members have been known to bias conceptual cognitive processes and show that the cross-race effect actually has less to do with race than with different levels of cognitive processing that occur for ingroup and outgroup members.<ref name=You />


==== Cognitive disregard====
==== Cognitive disregard====
Another set of cognitive theories related to cross-race effect focuses on how social categorization and individuation biases face memory.<ref name=You>{{cite journal|last=Young|first=S. G.|author2=Hugenberg, K. |author3=Bernstein, M. J. |author4= Sacco, D. F. |title=Perception and Motivation in Face Recognition: A Critical Review of Theories of the Cross-Race Effect|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Review|date=30 August 2011|volume=16|issue=2|pages=116–142|doi=10.1177/1088868311418987|pmid=21878608}}</ref> Some researchers believe that the inability for ingroup members' to recognize differences in the features of outgroup members can be explained through cognitive disregard.<ref name=Sporer /> They find that the likelihood of falsely identifying a member of an out-group stems from an automatic encoding of a face without processing its unique features.<ref name=Sporer /> Thus, when presented with an out-group member who has a similar face to the one that was encoded, the in-group member automatically, but incorrectly determines that the face has been "seen" before.<ref name=Sporer /> These studies conclude that diminishing the cross race effect requires individuals to process ethnically-differing faces with the goal of encoding with individuation.<ref name=Sporer />
Another set of cognitive theories related to cross-race effect focuses on how social categorization and individuation biases face memory.<ref name=You>{{cite journal | vauthors = Young SG, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ, Sacco DF | title = Perception and motivation in face recognition: a critical review of theories of the Cross-Race Effect | journal = Personality and Social Psychology Review | volume = 16 | issue = 2 | pages = 116–42 | date = May 2012 | pmid = 21878608 | doi = 10.1177/1088868311418987 }}</ref> Some researchers believe that the inability for ingroup members' to recognize differences in the features of outgroup members can be explained through cognitive disregard.<ref name=Sporer /> They find that the likelihood of falsely identifying a member of an out-group stems from an automatic encoding of a face without processing its unique features.<ref name=Sporer /> Thus, when presented with an out-group member who has a similar face to the one that was encoded, the in-group member automatically, but incorrectly determines that the face has been "seen" before.<ref name=Sporer /> These studies conclude that diminishing the cross race effect requires individuals to process ethnically-differing faces with the goal of encoding with individuation.<ref name=Sporer />


====Depth of processing hypothesis====
====Depth of processing hypothesis====
Depth of processing also influences the presence of the cross race effect.<ref name=Meissner>{{cite journal|last=Meissner|first=Christian A.|author2=Brigham, John C. |author3=Butz, David A. |title=Memory for own- and other-race faces: a dual-process approach|journal=Applied Cognitive Psychology|date=1 July 2005|volume=19|issue=5|pages=545–567|doi=10.1002/acp.1097}}</ref> Same-race faces are more correctly and easily discerned because deep processing, than other-race faces are.<ref name=Meissner /> This hypothesis, however, is controversial because of its inability to be replicated within studies.<ref name=Meissner />
Depth of processing also influences the presence of the cross race effect.<ref name=Meissner>{{cite journal| vauthors = Meissner CA, Brigham JC, Butz DA |title=Memory for own- and other-race faces: a dual-process approach|journal=Applied Cognitive Psychology|date=1 July 2005|volume=19|issue=5|pages=545–567|doi=10.1002/acp.1097}}</ref> Same-race faces are more correctly and easily discerned because deep processing, than other-race faces are.<ref name=Meissner /> This hypothesis, however, is controversial because of its inability to be replicated within studies.<ref name=Meissner />


====Challenges for social cognition models====
====Challenges for social cognition models====
Line 45: Line 45:


===Effects of social cognition===
===Effects of social cognition===
Another reason the cross-race-effect may occur is that perceptions are often affected by motivations, expectations, and social cognition. Overall, the creation of norms has shaped and biased even simple perceptions such as line lengths. In terms of perception of faces, studies have shown that racially ambiguous faces that have been identified as one race or another based on their hairstyle are identified as having more features of the racial category represented by the hairstyle. Similarly, faces of an ambiguous but equal shade are interpreted as darker or lighter when accompanied by the label of either "black" or "white", respectively.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Corneille | first1 = O. | last2 = Potter | first2 = T. | last3 = Hugenberg | first3 = K. | year = 2007 | title = Applying the attractor field model to social cognition: perceptual discrimination is facilitated, but memory is impaired for faces displaying evaluatively congruent expressions | url = | journal = Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | volume = 93 | issue = 3| pages = 335–352 | doi=10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.335 | pmid=17723052| citeseerx = 10.1.1.586.1592 }}</ref>Other social cognitive biases may also have the ability to overpower the cross-race-effect. A study has shown that social perception of wealth has the ability to modulate the effect: when the targets were seen as impoverished, the difference in facial recognition disappeared.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Shriver|first=Edwin R.|last2=Young|first2=Steven G.|last3=Hugenberg|first3=Kurt|last4=Bernstein|first4=Michael J.|last5=Lanter|first5=Jason R.|date=2007-12-04|title=Class, Race, and the Face: Social Context Modulates the Cross-Race Effect in Face Recognition:|url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0146167207310455|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin|language=en|doi=10.1177/0146167207310455}}</ref>
Another reason the cross-race-effect may occur is that perceptions are often affected by motivations, expectations, and social cognition. Overall, the creation of norms has shaped and biased even simple perceptions such as line lengths. In terms of perception of faces, studies have shown that racially ambiguous faces that have been identified as one race or another based on their hairstyle are identified as having more features of the racial category represented by the hairstyle. Similarly, faces of an ambiguous but equal shade are interpreted as darker or lighter when accompanied by the label of either "black" or "white", respectively.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Corneille O, Hugenberg K, Potter T | title = Applying the attractor field model to social cognition: Perceptual discrimination is facilitated, but memory is impaired for faces displaying evaluatively congruent expressions | journal = Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | volume = 93 | issue = 3 | pages = 335–52 | date = September 2007 | pmid = 17723052 | doi = 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.335 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.586.1592 }}</ref>Other social cognitive biases may also have the ability to overpower the cross-race-effect. A study has shown that social perception of wealth has the ability to modulate the effect: when the targets were seen as impoverished, the difference in facial recognition disappeared.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Shriver ER, Young SG, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ, Lanter JR | title = Class, race, and the face: social context modulates the cross-race effect in face recognition | journal = Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin | volume = 34 | issue = 2 | pages = 260–74 | date = February 2008 | pmid = 18212334 | doi = 10.1177/0146167207310455 }}</ref>


===Integration of cross-race effect theories===
===Integration of cross-race effect theories===
Line 54: Line 54:
===Race-feature theory===
===Race-feature theory===
Deeper study of the cross-race effect has demonstrated two types of processing for the recognition of faces: featural and holistic. It has been found that holistic processing (which occurs beyond individual parts of the face) is more commonly used in same-race situations, but there is an experience effect, which means that as a person gains more experience with those of a particular race, he or she will begin to use more holistic processing. Featural processing is much more commonly used with an unfamiliar stimulus or face.<ref name="Tanaka">Tanaka, J. W., Kiefer, M., & Bukach, C. M. (2003). A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: evidence from a cross-cultural study. Elsevier.</ref>
Deeper study of the cross-race effect has demonstrated two types of processing for the recognition of faces: featural and holistic. It has been found that holistic processing (which occurs beyond individual parts of the face) is more commonly used in same-race situations, but there is an experience effect, which means that as a person gains more experience with those of a particular race, he or she will begin to use more holistic processing. Featural processing is much more commonly used with an unfamiliar stimulus or face.<ref name="Tanaka">Tanaka, J. W., Kiefer, M., & Bukach, C. M. (2003). A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: evidence from a cross-cultural study. Elsevier.</ref>
[[File:Cross-race effect study samples.jpg|alt=grid with 20 faces, center column shows the original face (first two faces in column are white, last two faces are Asian) without editing. The leftmost face has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 10 pixels smaller than the original (the middle face), the face located second from the left has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 5 pixels smaller than the original, the rightmost face has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 10 pixels larger than the original, and the face located second from the right has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 5 pixels larger than the original.|thumb|Sample of real and edited Caucasian and Asian faces used in study of the Cross-race effect<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Wang|first=Zhe|last2=Quinn|first2=Paul C.|last3=Tanaka|first3=James W.|last4=Yu|first4=Xiaoyang|last5=Sun|first5=Yu-Hao P.|last6=Liu|first6=Jiangang|last7=Pascalis|first7=Olivier|last8=Ge|first8=Liezhong|last9=Lee|first9=Kang|date=2015|title=An other-race effect for configural and featural processing of faces: upper and lower face regions play different roles|url=https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00559/full|journal=Frontiers in Psychology|language=English|volume=6|doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00559|issn=1664-1078}}</ref>]]
[[File:Cross-race effect study samples.jpg|alt=grid with 20 faces, center column shows the original face (first two faces in column are white, last two faces are Asian) without editing. The leftmost face has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 10 pixels smaller than the original (the middle face), the face located second from the left has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 5 pixels smaller than the original, the rightmost face has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 10 pixels larger than the original, and the face located second from the right has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 5 pixels larger than the original.|thumb|Sample of real and edited Caucasian and Asian faces used in study of the Cross-race effect<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Wang Z, Quinn PC, Tanaka JW, Yu X, Sun YH, Liu J, Pascalis O, Ge L, Lee K | display-authors = 6 | title = An other-race effect for configural and featural processing of faces: upper and lower face regions play different roles | language = English | journal = Frontiers in Psychology | volume = 6 | pages = 559 | date = 2015 | pmid = 26005427 | doi = 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00559 }}</ref>]]
In his 1996 study, researchers noticed that when looking at ethnicity, in-group faces are processed without acknowledgement of ethnic-specific details and features.<ref name=Sporer /> People code faces that deviate from the norm of their ethnic group through the absence or presence of distinctive ethnic features.<ref name=Sporer />
In his 1996 study, researchers noticed that when looking at ethnicity, in-group faces are processed without acknowledgement of ethnic-specific details and features.<ref name=Sporer /> People code faces that deviate from the norm of their ethnic group through the absence or presence of distinctive ethnic features.<ref name=Sporer />


This is supported by the finding that the classification of other-race faces tends to be faster than same-race faces. This suggests that race seems to be a more perceptually salient feature than other more discerning facial features when the face belongs to a different race.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Levin | first1 = D. T. | year = 1996 | title = Classifying faces by race. The structure of face categories | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition | volume = 22 | issue = 6| pages = 1364–1382 | doi=10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1364}}</ref> Some [[eye tracking]] studies found tentative evidence for such a hypothesis by demonstrating that people look at different facial features in same- versus other-race faces.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Brielmann | first1 = A. A. | last2 = Bülthoff | first2 = I. | last3 = Armann | first3 = R. | year = 2014 | title = Looking at faces from different angles: Europeans fixate different features in Asian and Caucasian faces | url = | journal = Vision Research | volume = 100 | issue = | pages = 105–112 | doi=10.1016/j.visres.2014.04.011 | pmid=24796509| doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Wheeler | first1 = A. | last2 = Anzures | first2 = G. | last3 = Quinn | first3 = P. C. | last4 = Pascalis | first4 = O. | last5 = Omrin | first5 = D. S. | last6 = Lee | first6 = K. | year = 2011 | title = Caucasian infants scan own- and other-race faces differently | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0018621 | pmid = 21533235 | pmc = 3076379 | journal = PLOS ONE | volume = 6 | issue = 4| page = e18621 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Goldinger | first1 = S. D. | last2 = He | first2 = Y. | last3 = Papesh | first3 = M. H. | year = 2009 | title = Deficits in cross-race face learning: Insights from eye movements and pupillometry | doi = 10.1037/a0016548 | pmid = 19686008 | pmc = 2871406 | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition | volume = 35 | issue = 5| pages = 1105–1122 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Fu | first1 = G. | last2 = Hu | first2 = C. S. | last3 = Wang | first3 = Q. | last4 = Quinn | first4 = P. C. | last5 = Lee | first5 = K. | year = 2012 | title = Adults scan own- and other-race faces differently | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0037688 | pmid = 22675486 | pmc = 3365898 | journal = PLOS ONE | volume = 7 | issue = 6| page = e37688 }}</ref> The general trend observed is that people fixate the eyes of a face with higher probability if it belongs to the same ethnic group as the observer her- or himself. However, other studies found just as pronounced differences between Asian and European ''observers′'' looking behavior and these differences were stable for both own- and other-race faces.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Blais | first1 = C. | last2 = Jack | first2 = R. E. | last3 = Scheepers | first3 = C. | last4 = Fiset | first4 = D. | last5 = Caldara | first5 = R. | year = 2008 | title = Culture shapes how we look at faces | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0003022 | pmid = 18714387 | pmc = 2515341 | journal = PLOS ONE | volume = 3 | issue = 8| page = e3022 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Kelly | first1 = D. J. | last2 = Miellet | first2 = S. | last3 = Caldara | first3 = R. | year = 2010 | title = Culture shapes eye movements for visually homogeneous objects | doi = 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00006 | pmid = 21833189 | pmc = 3153738 | journal = Frontiers in Psychology | volume = 1 | issue = | page = 6 }}</ref>
This is supported by the finding that the classification of other-race faces tends to be faster than same-race faces. This suggests that race seems to be a more perceptually salient feature than other more discerning facial features when the face belongs to a different race.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Levin DT | year = 1996 | title = Classifying faces by race. The structure of face categories | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition | volume = 22 | issue = 6| pages = 1364–1382 | doi=10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1364}}</ref> Some [[eye tracking]] studies found tentative evidence for such a hypothesis by demonstrating that people look at different facial features in same- versus other-race faces.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Brielmann AA, Bülthoff I, Armann R | title = Looking at faces from different angles: Europeans fixate different features in Asian and Caucasian faces | journal = Vision Research | volume = 100 | issue = | pages = 105–12 | date = July 2014 | pmid = 24796509 | doi = 10.1016/j.visres.2014.04.011 | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Wheeler A, Anzures G, Quinn PC, Pascalis O, Omrin DS, Lee K | title = Caucasian infants scan own- and other-race faces differently | journal = PloS One | volume = 6 | issue = 4 | pages = e18621 | date = April 2011 | pmid = 21533235 | pmc = 3076379 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0018621 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Goldinger SD, He Y, Papesh MH | title = Deficits in cross-race face learning: insights from eye movements and pupillometry | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition | volume = 35 | issue = 5 | pages = 1105–22 | date = September 2009 | pmid = 19686008 | pmc = 2871406 | doi = 10.1037/a0016548 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Fu G, Hu CS, Wang Q, Quinn PC, Lee K | title = Adults scan own- and other-race faces differently | journal = PloS One | volume = 7 | issue = 6 | pages = e37688 | year = 2012 | pmid = 22675486 | pmc = 3365898 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0037688 }}</ref> The general trend observed is that people fixate the eyes of a face with higher probability if it belongs to the same ethnic group as the observer her- or himself. However, other studies found just as pronounced differences between Asian and European ''observers′'' looking behavior and these differences were stable for both own- and other-race faces.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Blais C, Jack RE, Scheepers C, Fiset D, Caldara R | title = Culture shapes how we look at faces | journal = PloS One | volume = 3 | issue = 8 | pages = e3022 | date = August 2008 | pmid = 18714387 | pmc = 2515341 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0003022 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Kelly DJ, Miellet S, Caldara R | title = Culture shapes eye movements for visually homogeneous objects | journal = Frontiers in Psychology | volume = 1 | issue = | pages = 6 | year = 2010 | pmid = 21833189 | pmc = 3153738 | doi = 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00006 }}</ref>
This was previously explained as stemming from a more clustered density-gradient for other-race faces than same race faces.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Valentine | first1 = T. | last2 = Endo | first2 = M. | year = 1992 | title = Towards an exemplar model of face processing: The effects of race and distinctiveness | doi = 10.1080/14640749208401305 | journal = Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | volume = 44A | issue = 4| pages = 671–703 }}</ref> The reasoning is that this causes more nodes to become activated in reaction to an other-race face, resulting in faster classification, but less discriminability in terms of memory. However, these exemplar-based theories cannot explain why faces that are ambiguous in terms of social category information can influence recognition.
This was previously explained as stemming from a more clustered density-gradient for other-race faces than same race faces.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Valentine T, Endo M | title = Towards an exemplar model of face processing: the effects of race and distinctiveness | journal = The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology | volume = 44 | issue = 4 | pages = 671–703 | date = May 1992 | pmid = 1615169 | doi = 10.1080/14640749208401305 }}</ref> The reasoning is that this causes more nodes to become activated in reaction to an other-race face, resulting in faster classification, but less discriminability in terms of memory. However, these exemplar-based theories cannot explain why faces that are ambiguous in terms of social category information can influence recognition.


===Contact hypothesis theory===
===Contact hypothesis theory===
''See also: [[Contact hypothesis|Contact Hypothesis]]''
''See also: [[Contact hypothesis|Contact Hypothesis]]''


One method researchers have suggested to help mollify the prevalence of the cross race effect is through application of the contact hypothesis. Accurate recognition and identification of other-race faces, researchers have deduced, stems from a difference in learning experiences that relate to individual ethnic groups.<ref name=Combs>{{cite journal|last=Combs|first=G. M.|author2=Griffith, J.|title=An Examination of Interracial Contact:The Influence of Cross-Race Interpersonal Efficacy and Affect Regulation|journal=Human Resource Development Review|date=1 September 2007|volume=6|issue=3|pages=222–244|doi=10.1177/1534484307303990|url=http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1027&context=managementfacpub}}</ref> The cross race effect can be reduced by continual exposure ethnic groups that differ from one's own; the more positive interactions that occur between two ethnic groups, the more heterogeneous the ethnicities will seem to be.<ref name=Combs /> The type of contact experienced between the two ethnic groups also plays a major role in this hypothesis' effectiveness; the more intimate the contact, the higher the chances become of accurately recognizing a member of a different ethnicity than one's own<ref name=Combs /> As an example, research done on Asian and white students living in Singapore and Canada showed a significant cross race effect that was not able to be predicted by perceived familiarity with the other race.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Ng|first=Wei-Jen|last2=Lindsay|first2=R. C. L.|date=2016-07-27|title=Cross-Race Facial Recognition: Failure of the Contact Hypothesis|url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0022022194252004|journal=Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology|language=en|doi=10.1177/0022022194252004}}</ref> Previously seen as evidence against the contact hypothesis, it is now seen as evidence that the depth of contact is an important factor.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Wilson|first=John Paul|last2=Hugenberg|first2=Kurt|last3=Bernstein|first3=Michael J.|date=2013|title=The Cross-Race Effect and Eyewitness Identification: How to Improve Recognition and Reduce Decision Errors in Eyewitness Situations|url=https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1751-2409.2012.01044.x|journal=Social Issues and Policy Review|language=en|volume=7|issue=1|pages=83–113|doi=10.1111/j.1751-2409.2012.01044.x|issn=1751-2409}}</ref>
One method researchers have suggested to help mollify the prevalence of the cross race effect is through application of the contact hypothesis. Accurate recognition and identification of other-race faces, researchers have deduced, stems from a difference in learning experiences that relate to individual ethnic groups.<ref name=Combs>{{cite journal| vauthors = Combs GM, Griffith J |title=An Examination of Interracial Contact:The Influence of Cross-Race Interpersonal Efficacy and Affect Regulation|journal=Human Resource Development Review|date=1 September 2007|volume=6|issue=3|pages=222–244|doi=10.1177/1534484307303990|url=http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1027&context=managementfacpub}}</ref> The cross race effect can be reduced by continual exposure ethnic groups that differ from one's own; the more positive interactions that occur between two ethnic groups, the more heterogeneous the ethnicities will seem to be.<ref name=Combs /> The type of contact experienced between the two ethnic groups also plays a major role in this hypothesis' effectiveness; the more intimate the contact, the higher the chances become of accurately recognizing a member of a different ethnicity than one's own<ref name=Combs /> As an example, research done on Asian and white students living in Singapore and Canada showed a significant cross race effect that was not able to be predicted by perceived familiarity with the other race.<ref>{{Cite journal| vauthors = Ng WJ, Lindsay RC |date=2016-07-27|title=Cross-Race Facial Recognition: Failure of the Contact Hypothesis |journal=Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology |doi=10.1177/0022022194252004}}</ref> Previously seen as evidence against the contact hypothesis, it is now seen as evidence that the depth of contact is an important factor.<ref>{{Cite journal| vauthors = Wilson JP, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ |date=2013|title=The Cross-Race Effect and Eyewitness Identification: How to Improve Recognition and Reduce Decision Errors in Eyewitness Situations |journal=Social Issues and Policy Review|language=en|volume=7|issue=1|pages=83–113|doi=10.1111/j.1751-2409.2012.01044.x }}</ref>


==Empirical findings==
==Empirical findings==


===The cross-race effect across ethnic groups===
===The cross-race effect across ethnic groups===
Although most studies done about the cross race effect are with black and white participants, there are also numerous studies done with people of different ethnic backgrounds.<ref name=Brig>{{cite book|editor=Michael P. Toglia|title=Handbook of eyewitness psychology.|year=2007|publisher=Erlbaum|location=Mahwah, NJ [u.a.]|isbn=978-1-4106-1491-9|pages=257–81}}</ref> For example, there are studies done that compare Hispanic with black and white participants, black with white and Japanese participants, Chinese with Korean and white participants, Chinese with Indian and other East Asian ethnic participants,<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Perera-W.A.|first1=Hiran|title=The Effects of Memory Conformity and the Cross-Race Effect in Eyewitness Testimony|doi=10.2139/ssrn.2732189|year=2014}}</ref> Turkish and German participants, and finally a study has been done comparing Arab and Israeli Jews.<ref name=Brig /> The data from all of these studies have come to the same conclusion. The cross-race effect is evident among all people of all different races.<ref name=Brig />
Although most studies done about the cross race effect are with black and white participants, there are also numerous studies done with people of different ethnic backgrounds.<ref name=Brig>{{cite book| veditors = Toglia MP |title=Handbook of eyewitness psychology.|year=2007|publisher=Erlbaum|location=Mahwah, NJ [u.a.]|isbn=978-1-4106-1491-9|pages=257–81}}</ref> For example, there are studies done that compare Hispanic with black and white participants, black with white and Japanese participants, Chinese with Korean and white participants, Chinese with Indian and other East Asian ethnic participants,<ref>{{cite journal| vauthors = Perera-WA H |title=The Effects of Memory Conformity and the Cross-Race Effect in Eyewitness Testimony|doi=10.2139/ssrn.2732189|year=2014}}</ref> Turkish and German participants, and finally a study has been done comparing Arab and Israeli Jews.<ref name=Brig /> The data from all of these studies have come to the same conclusion. The cross-race effect is evident among all people of all different races.<ref name=Brig />


===Morphological basis===
===Morphological basis===
The cross race effect has a morphological basis: The facial appearance is morphologically different for different ethnic backgrounds. This has been established empirically,<ref>{{cite journal | last=Salah| first=Albert Ali|author2=Alyüz, Neşe |author3=Akarun, Lale |title=Registration of 3D Face Scans with Average Face Models | journal=Journal of Electronic Imaging|year=2008|volume=17 | issue = 1 |page=011006|doi= 10.1117/1.2896291}}</ref> wherein a large set of 3D scans of faces from different ethnic backgrounds was automatically clustered into groups. Only facial landmark distances were used in this grouping. The result was that gender, as well as ethnicity, emerged as primary factors of group membership.
The cross race effect has a morphological basis: The facial appearance is morphologically different for different ethnic backgrounds. This has been established empirically,<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Salah AA, Alyüz N, Akarun L |title=Registration of 3D Face Scans with Average Face Models | journal=Journal of Electronic Imaging|year=2008|volume=17 | issue = 1 |page=011006|doi= 10.1117/1.2896291}}</ref> wherein a large set of 3D scans of faces from different ethnic backgrounds was automatically clustered into groups. Only facial landmark distances were used in this grouping. The result was that gender, as well as ethnicity, emerged as primary factors of group membership.


While the cross-race effect has been observed for adult faces, research indicates that infant faces do not produce a cross-race effect; infant faces seem to automatically draw the viewer's attention with the ethnicity of the infant having no effect.<ref>Proverbio, Alice Mado, and Valeria De Gabriele. "The other-race effect does not apply to infant faces: An ERP attentional study." Neuropsychologia 126 (2019): 36-45.</ref>
While the cross-race effect has been observed for adult faces, research indicates that infant faces do not produce a cross-race effect; infant faces seem to automatically draw the viewer's attention with the ethnicity of the infant having no effect.<ref>Proverbio, Alice Mado, and Valeria De Gabriele. "The other-race effect does not apply to infant faces: An ERP attentional study." Neuropsychologia 126 (2019): 36-45.</ref>
Line 78: Line 78:


====Children and face identification====
====Children and face identification====
With the help of several conducted studies, researchers conclude that the accuracy of eyewitness memory is significantly affected by the ethnic identity of both the suspect and the eye-witness; an individual can more accurately recognize a face belonging to his or her race than an individual whose race differs from that of his or her own.<ref name=Pezdek>{{cite journal|last=Pezdek|first=Kathy|author2=Blandon-Gitlin, Iris |author3=Moore, Catherine |title=Children's face recognition memory: More evidence for the cross-race effect|journal=Journal of Applied Psychology|date=1 January 2003|volume=88|issue=4|pages=760–763|doi=10.1037/0021-9010.88.4.760|pmid=12940414|citeseerx=10.1.1.365.6517}}</ref> Previous studies have analyzed how the cross-race effect affects adults during eyewitness testimony but fails to address the possible existence of age-related confounding factors: On one hand, as an individual grows older and encounters more members of the other ethnic group in question, the novelty of the ethnic difference wears off and makes it less distracting, and the individual can pay higher absolute and relative amounts of attention to subtle distinctions between members of that group; on the other hand, time also increases the individual's exposure to biases prevalent in his/her own in-group, as well as compounding the effects of any self-reinforcement bias that the individual exhibits with respect to his/her preexisting opinions.<ref name=Pezdek /> The literature available on this topic is minute and conflicting; some researchers have found a prevalence of the cross-race effect in both white and black children,<ref name=Pezdek /> yet others have reported findings of children possessing the ability to discern other-race faces accurately.<ref name=Pezdek /> In their aim to identify developmental differences, researchers such as Pezdek et al.<ref name=Pezdek /> discovered that children recognize faces belonging to their own race more effectively than faces belonging to another race.<ref name=Pezdek />{{explain|reason=How did they show this and how does that resolve the conflict in findings?|date=September 2020}}
With the help of several conducted studies, researchers conclude that the accuracy of eyewitness memory is significantly affected by the ethnic identity of both the suspect and the eye-witness; an individual can more accurately recognize a face belonging to his or her race than an individual whose race differs from that of his or her own.<ref name=Pezdek>{{cite journal | vauthors = Pezdek K, Blandon-Gitlin I, Moore C | title = Children's face recognition memory: more evidence for the cross-race effect | journal = The Journal of Applied Psychology | volume = 88 | issue = 4 | pages = 760–3 | date = August 2003 | pmid = 12940414 | doi = 10.1037/0021-9010.88.4.760 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.365.6517 }}</ref> Previous studies have analyzed how the cross-race effect affects adults during eyewitness testimony but fails to address the possible existence of age-related confounding factors: On one hand, as an individual grows older and encounters more members of the other ethnic group in question, the novelty of the ethnic difference wears off and makes it less distracting, and the individual can pay higher absolute and relative amounts of attention to subtle distinctions between members of that group; on the other hand, time also increases the individual's exposure to biases prevalent in his/her own in-group, as well as compounding the effects of any self-reinforcement bias that the individual exhibits with respect to his/her preexisting opinions.<ref name=Pezdek /> The literature available on this topic is minute and conflicting; some researchers have found a prevalence of the cross-race effect in both white and black children,<ref name=Pezdek /> yet others have reported findings of children possessing the ability to discern other-race faces accurately.<ref name=Pezdek /> In their aim to identify developmental differences, researchers such as Pezdek et al.<ref name=Pezdek /> discovered that children recognize faces belonging to their own race more effectively than faces belonging to another race.<ref name=Pezdek />{{explain|reason=How did they show this and how does that resolve the conflict in findings?|date=September 2020}}


==Consequences==
==Consequences==
Line 84: Line 84:
===Cross-race identification bias===
===Cross-race identification bias===
[[File:Jennifer Thompson-Cannino and Ronald Cotton at PopTech 2010 in Camden, Maine.jpg|alt=Ronald Cotton (left) and Jennifer Thomson-Cannino (right) sitting in two rolling office chairs in front of large red and blue Pop!Tech logo.|thumb|Ronald Cotton with Jennifer Thompson-Cannino at PopTech 2010. After Thompson-Cannino mistakenly identified Cotton as her rapist, Cotton was convicted of rape in 1985. A decade later, DNA evidence exonerated him. The case is often used as a real world example of the cross-race effect and the potential dangers of eyewitness testimony and lineup identification.]]
[[File:Jennifer Thompson-Cannino and Ronald Cotton at PopTech 2010 in Camden, Maine.jpg|alt=Ronald Cotton (left) and Jennifer Thomson-Cannino (right) sitting in two rolling office chairs in front of large red and blue Pop!Tech logo.|thumb|Ronald Cotton with Jennifer Thompson-Cannino at PopTech 2010. After Thompson-Cannino mistakenly identified Cotton as her rapist, Cotton was convicted of rape in 1985. A decade later, DNA evidence exonerated him. The case is often used as a real world example of the cross-race effect and the potential dangers of eyewitness testimony and lineup identification.]]
This effect refers to the decreased ability of people of one race to recognize faces and facial expressions of people of another race. This differs from the cross-race bias because this effect is found mostly during [[eyewitness identification]] as well as identification of a suspect in a [[police lineup|line-up]]. In these situations, many people feel as if races other than their own look alike, and they have difficulty distinguishing between members of different ethnic groups. Cross-race identification bias is also known as the misinformation effect since people are considered to be misinformed about other races and have difficulty identifying them. A study was made which examined 271 real court cases. In photographic line-ups, 231 witnesses participated in cross-race versus same-race identification. In cross-race lineups, only 45% were correctly identified versus 60% for same-race identifications.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Behrman|first=Bruce W.|last2=Davey|first2=Sherrie L.|year=2001|title=Eyewitness identification in actual criminal cases: An archival analysis.|journal=Law and Human Behavior|volume=25|issue=5|pages=475–491|doi=10.1023/a:1012840831846}}</ref> In a study dealing with eyewitness testimony, investigators examined forty participants in a racially diverse area of the US. Participants watched a video of a property crime being committed, then in the next 24 hours came to pick the suspect out of a photo line-up. Most of the participants in the study either misidentified the suspect or stated the suspect was not in the line-up at all. Correct identification of the suspect occurred more often when the eyewitness and the suspect were of the same race.<ref>Josephson, S. & Holmes, M. (2008). Cross-race recognition deficit and visual attention: do they all look (at faces) alike?. ''Proceeding ETRA '08 Proceedings of the 2008 symposium on Eye tracking research & applications.'' ACM New York, NY, USA.</ref> In another study, 86 convenience store clerks were asked to identify three customers: one white, one black, and one Mexican, all of whom had purchased in the store earlier that day. The clerks tended to identify customers belonging to their own race accurately, but were more likely to make errors when attempting to identify other races members.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Platz | first1 = S. J. | last2 = Hosch | first2 = H. M. | year = 1988 | title = Cross-Racial/Ethnic Eyewitness Identification: A Field Study | url = | journal = Journal of Applied Social Psychology | volume = 18 | issue = 11| pages = 972–984 | doi=10.1111/j.1559-1816.1988.tb01187.x}}</ref> Meanwhile, another study found that "alcohol intoxication reduces the own-race bias in face recognition," albeit by impairing accurate perception and leaving in place or increasing random error rather than by improving facial recognition of members of other groups.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hilliar | first1 = K. | last2 = Kemp | first2 = R. | last3 = Denson | first3 = T. | year = 2010 | title = Now Everyone Looks The Same: Alcohol Intoxication Reduces The Own-Race Bias in Face Recognition | url = | journal = Law and Human Behavior | volume = 34 | issue = 5| pages = 367–368 | doi=10.1007/s10979-009-9204-x| pmid = 20130972 }}</ref>
This effect refers to the decreased ability of people of one race to recognize faces and facial expressions of people of another race. This differs from the cross-race bias because this effect is found mostly during [[eyewitness identification]] as well as identification of a suspect in a [[police lineup|line-up]]. In these situations, many people feel as if races other than their own look alike, and they have difficulty distinguishing between members of different ethnic groups. Cross-race identification bias is also known as the misinformation effect since people are considered to be misinformed about other races and have difficulty identifying them. A study was made which examined 271 real court cases. In photographic line-ups, 231 witnesses participated in cross-race versus same-race identification. In cross-race lineups, only 45% were correctly identified versus 60% for same-race identifications.<ref>{{Cite journal| vauthors = Behrman BW, Davey SL |year=2001|title=Eyewitness identification in actual criminal cases: An archival analysis.|journal=Law and Human Behavior|volume=25|issue=5|pages=475–491|doi=10.1023/a:1012840831846}}</ref> In a study dealing with eyewitness testimony, investigators examined forty participants in a racially diverse area of the US. Participants watched a video of a property crime being committed, then in the next 24 hours came to pick the suspect out of a photo line-up. Most of the participants in the study either misidentified the suspect or stated the suspect was not in the line-up at all. Correct identification of the suspect occurred more often when the eyewitness and the suspect were of the same race.<ref>Josephson, S. & Holmes, M. (2008). Cross-race recognition deficit and visual attention: do they all look (at faces) alike?. ''Proceeding ETRA '08 Proceedings of the 2008 symposium on Eye tracking research & applications.'' ACM New York, NY, USA.</ref> In another study, 86 convenience store clerks were asked to identify three customers: one white, one black, and one Mexican, all of whom had purchased in the store earlier that day. The clerks tended to identify customers belonging to their own race accurately, but were more likely to make errors when attempting to identify other races members.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Platz SJ, Hosch HM | year = 1988 | title = Cross-Racial/Ethnic Eyewitness Identification: A Field Study | journal = Journal of Applied Social Psychology | volume = 18 | issue = 11| pages = 972–984 | doi=10.1111/j.1559-1816.1988.tb01187.x}}</ref> Meanwhile, another study found that "alcohol intoxication reduces the own-race bias in face recognition," albeit by impairing accurate perception and leaving in place or increasing random error rather than by improving facial recognition of members of other groups.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Hilliar KF, Kemp RI, Denson TF | title = Now everyone looks the same: alcohol intoxication reduces the own-race bias in face recognition | journal = Law and Human Behavior | volume = 34 | issue = 5 | pages = 367–78 | date = October 2010 | pmid = 20130972 | doi = 10.1007/s10979-009-9204-x }}</ref>


There has been some disagreement about the consistency of the own-race bias. However, data gathered from multiple studies does show that the own-race bias is very consistent.<ref name=Both>{{cite journal|last=Bothwell|first=R. K.|author2=Brigham, J. C. |author3=Malpass, R. S. |title=Cross-Racial Identification|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin|date=1 March 1989|volume=15|issue=1|pages=19–25|doi=10.1177/0146167289151002}}</ref> Where it is not consistent, such as in a study done on white students in England and white and black students in South Africa, there are other factors associated. In this study the black South African students were slightly better at identifying white faces, but this is thought to be related to the significant contact the black students had with white students in University, as black non-students in South Africa were found to exhibit the own race bias<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Wright|first=Daniel B.|last2=Boyd|first2=Catherine E.|last3=Tredoux|first3=Colin G.|date=2003-04|title=Inter-racial contact and the own-race bias for face recognition in South Africa and England|url=http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/acp.898|journal=Applied Cognitive Psychology|language=en|volume=17|issue=3|pages=365–373|doi=10.1002/acp.898|issn=0888-4080}}</ref>. The own-race bias occurs in people of all races.<ref name=Both /> Since eyewitness identification can be problematic, researchers have started to conduct studies of own-race biases using more forensics.<ref name=Both /> This kind of research needs to pay more attention to a target's distinctive features and level of attractiveness.<ref name=Both /> If a target is very distinctive or very attractive, it could reduce the cross-race effect because that person would be easier to identify.<ref name=Both />
There has been some disagreement about the consistency of the own-race bias. However, data gathered from multiple studies does show that the own-race bias is very consistent.<ref name=Both>{{cite journal| vauthors = Bothwell RK, Brigham JC, Malpass RS |title=Cross-Racial Identification|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin|date=1 March 1989|volume=15|issue=1|pages=19–25|doi=10.1177/0146167289151002}}</ref> Where it is not consistent, such as in a study done on white students in England and white and black students in South Africa, there are other factors associated. In this study the black South African students were slightly better at identifying white faces, but this is thought to be related to the significant contact the black students had with white students in University, as black non-students in South Africa were found to exhibit the own race bias<ref>{{Cite journal| vauthors = Wright DB, Boyd CE, Tredoux CG |date= April 2003 |title=Inter-racial contact and the own-race bias for face recognition in South Africa and England |journal=Applied Cognitive Psychology|language=en|volume=17|issue=3|pages=365–373|doi=10.1002/acp.898|issn=0888-4080}}</ref>. The own-race bias occurs in people of all races.<ref name=Both /> Since eyewitness identification can be problematic, researchers have started to conduct studies of own-race biases using more forensics.<ref name=Both /> This kind of research needs to pay more attention to a target's distinctive features and level of attractiveness.<ref name=Both /> If a target is very distinctive or very attractive, it could reduce the cross-race effect because that person would be easier to identify.<ref name=Both />


Psychological experts all agree that the cross-race effect is a common occurrence during in-court testimony when an eyewitness is trying to remember a person.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Sporer|first=Siegfried Ludwig|title=The cross-race effect: Beyond recognition of faces in the laboratory.|journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law|date=1 January 2001|volume=7|issue=1|pages=170–200|doi=10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.170}}</ref> In order to reduce the cross-race effect there have been multiple changes to how policemen handle eyewitness identification.<ref name=Revlin /> For example, to reduce the cross-race identification bias Britain has a law that states police must include the suspect in a line up with at least eight other people who share similar characteristics to him or her.<ref name=Revlin>{{cite book|last=Revlin|first=Russell|title=Human Cognition : Theory and Practice.|year=2007|publisher=Worth Pub|location=New York, NY|isbn=9780716756675|pages=110–11}}</ref> This forces the eyewitness to use his or her memory of the suspects features, not the suspect's race, as a form of identification.<ref name=Revlin /> However, as evidence shows that cross race identification is more difficult when faces are viewed in a group, cross race identification still poses a risk.<ref>{{Cite book|last=Pezdek|first=Kathy|url=http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.365.7500|title=Cross-Race (but Not Same-Race) Face Identification Is Impaired by Presenting Faces in a Group Rather Than Individually|last2=Wasson|first2=Corey|last3=Pezdek|first3=Kathy|last4=Wasson|first4=Corey}}</ref>
Psychological experts all agree that the cross-race effect is a common occurrence during in-court testimony when an eyewitness is trying to remember a person.<ref>{{cite journal| vauthors = Sporer SL |title=The cross-race effect: Beyond recognition of faces in the laboratory.|journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law|date=1 January 2001|volume=7|issue=1|pages=170–200|doi=10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.170}}</ref> In order to reduce the cross-race effect there have been multiple changes to how policemen handle eyewitness identification.<ref name=Revlin /> For example, to reduce the cross-race identification bias Britain has a law that states police must include the suspect in a line up with at least eight other people who share similar characteristics to him or her.<ref name=Revlin>{{cite book| vauthors = Revlin R |title=Human Cognition : Theory and Practice.|year=2007|publisher=Worth Pub|location=New York, NY|isbn=9780716756675|pages=110–11}}</ref> This forces the eyewitness to use his or her memory of the suspects features, not the suspect's race, as a form of identification.<ref name=Revlin /> However, as evidence shows that cross race identification is more difficult when faces are viewed in a group, cross race identification still poses a risk.<ref>{{Cite book| vauthors = Pezdek K, Wasson C, Corey P, Pezdek K, Wasson C |url=http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.365.7500|title=Cross-Race (but Not Same-Race) Face Identification Is Impaired by Presenting Faces in a Group Rather Than Individually}}</ref>


===Economic===
===Economic===
Research has shown, that when making financial decisions, specific facial characteristics and implicit bias can influence the perceived trustworthiness of another person.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Stanley|first=Damian A.|last2=Sokol-Hessner|first2=Peter|last3=Banaji|first3=Mahzarin R.|last4=Phelps|first4=Elizabeth A.|date=2011-05-10|title=Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions|url=https://www.pnas.org/content/108/19/7710|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|language=en|volume=108|issue=19|pages=7710–7715|doi=10.1073/pnas.1014345108|issn=0027-8424|pmc=PMC3093479|pmid=21518877}}</ref>
Research has shown, that when making financial decisions, specific facial characteristics and implicit bias can influence the perceived trustworthiness of another person.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Stanley DA, Sokol-Hessner P, Banaji MR, Phelps EA | title = Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 108 | issue = 19 | pages = 7710–5 | date = May 2011 | pmid = 21518877 | pmc = 3093479 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.1014345108 }}</ref>
==Mitigation of cross-race effect==
==Mitigation of cross-race effect==
Studies beyond the [[contact hypothesis]] have also been done to mitigate the cross-race effect. A study was done in which participants were forewarned about cross-race effect. Results from this study showed that the cross-race effect could be reduced and sometimes even eliminated when participants were wary of it.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hugenberg | first1 = K. | last2 = Miller | first2 = J. | last3 = Claypool | first3 = H. M. | year = 2007 | title = Categorization and individuation in the cross-race recognition deficit: Toward a solution to an insidious problem | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Social Psychology | volume = 43 | issue = 2| pages = 334–340 | doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2006.02.010}}</ref>Research has also found that individuation training in which other race faces are given specific characteristics can mitigate the cross-race effect. In the study, white participants were trained to identify faces from one race (ex. African American) by specific letters and categorize another race (ex. Asian) using only one letter. Results found that participants were better able to distinguish other race faces that were individuated better than those that were categorized, despite equal exposure. <ref>{{Cite journal|last=Lebrecht|first=Sophie|last2=Pierce|first2=Lara J.|last3=Tarr|first3=Michael J.|last4=Tanaka|first4=James W.|date=2009-01-21|title=Perceptual Other-Race Training Reduces Implicit Racial Bias|url=https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0004215|journal=PLOS ONE|language=en|volume=4|issue=1|pages=e4215|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0004215|issn=1932-6203|pmc=PMC2627769|pmid=19156226}}</ref>In a study done on 43 white men, administering oxytocin before participants memorized faces was also found to reduce the cross-race effect. <ref>{{Cite journal|last=Blandón-Gitlin|first=Iris|last2=Pezdek|first2=Kathy|last3=Saldivar|first3=Sesar|last4=Steelman|first4=Erin|date=2014-09-11|title=Oxytocin eliminates the own-race bias in face recognition memory|url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0006899313009803|journal=Brain Research|series=Oxytocin in Human Social Behavior and Psychopathology|language=en|volume=1580|pages=180–187|doi=10.1016/j.brainres.2013.07.015|issn=0006-8993}}</ref>
Studies beyond the [[contact hypothesis]] have also been done to mitigate the cross-race effect. A study was done in which participants were forewarned about cross-race effect. Results from this study showed that the cross-race effect could be reduced and sometimes even eliminated when participants were wary of it.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Hugenberg K, Miller J, Claypool HM | year = 2007 | title = Categorization and individuation in the cross-race recognition deficit: Toward a solution to an insidious problem | journal = Journal of Experimental Social Psychology | volume = 43 | issue = 2| pages = 334–340 | doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2006.02.010}}</ref>Research has also found that individuation training in which other race faces are given specific characteristics can mitigate the cross-race effect. In the study, white participants were trained to identify faces from one race (ex. African American) by specific letters and categorize another race (ex. Asian) using only one letter. Results found that participants were better able to distinguish other race faces that were individuated better than those that were categorized, despite equal exposure. <ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Lebrecht S, Pierce LJ, Tarr MJ, Tanaka JW | title = Perceptual other-race training reduces implicit racial bias | journal = PloS One | volume = 4 | issue = 1 | pages = e4215 | date = 2009-01-21 | pmid = 19156226 | pmc = 2627769 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0004215 }}</ref>In a study done on 43 white men, administering oxytocin before participants memorized faces was also found to reduce the cross-race effect. <ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Blandón-Gitlin I, Pezdek K, Saldivar S, Steelman E | title = Oxytocin eliminates the own-race bias in face recognition memory | journal = Brain Research | volume = 1580 | pages = 180–7 | date = September 2014 | pmid = 23872107 | doi = 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.07.015 | series = Oxytocin in Human Social Behavior and Psychopathology }}</ref>


==Related biases==
==Related biases==
Similar biases have been found for aspects other than race. There is an own-gender bias, although evidence suggests that this comes down to hair style recognition.<ref>{{cite journal | doi=10.1016/S0001-6918(03)00052-0 | volume=114 | title=An own gender bias and the importance of hair in face recognition | journal=Acta Psychologica | pages=101–114 | year=2003 | last1 = Wright | first1 = Daniel B.}}</ref> Also, there is an own-age bias where people are better at recognising people of a similar age as themselves.<ref>{{cite journal|title=The own-age face recognition bias in children and adults|doi=10.1080/17470218.2010.537926|pmid = 21213196|journal=The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology|volume=64|issue=1|year=2011|author1=Peter J. Hills|author2=Michael B. Lewis|pages=17–23}}</ref>
Similar biases have been found for aspects other than race. There is an own-gender bias, although evidence suggests that this comes down to hair style recognition.<ref>{{cite journal | doi=10.1016/S0001-6918(03)00052-0 | volume=114 | title=An own gender bias and the importance of hair in face recognition | journal=Acta Psychologica | pages=101–114 | year=2003 | vauthors = Wright DB }}</ref> Also, there is an own-age bias where people are better at recognising people of a similar age as themselves.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Hills PJ, Lewis MB | title = The own-age face recognition bias in children and adults | journal = Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | volume = 64 | issue = 1 | pages = 17–23 | date = January 2011 | pmid = 21213196 | doi = 10.1080/17470218.2010.537926 }}</ref>


==See also==
== See also ==
{{cols|colwidth=16em}}
{{cols|colwidth=16em}}
* [[Discrimination]]
* [[Discrimination]]
Line 117: Line 117:
{{colend}}
{{colend}}


==References==
== References ==
{{Reflist}}
{{Reflist}}


==Further reading==
== Further reading ==
{{refbegin}}
*Brigham, J. C., Bennett, L. B., Meissner, C. A., & Mitchell, T. L. (2006). The influence of race on eyewitness memory. In R. Lindsay, D. Ross, J. Read, & M. Toglia, (Eds). ''Handbook of Eyewitness Psychology'': Memory for People, (pp.&nbsp;257–281). Lawrence Erlbaum & Associates.
* {{cite book | vauthors = Brigham JC, Bennett LB, Meissner CA, Mitchell TL | date = 2006 | title = The influence of race on eyewitness memory. | veditors = Lindsay R, Ross D, Read J, Toglia M | title = Handbook of Eyewitness Psychology: Memory for People | pages = 257–281 | publisher = Lawrence Erlbaum & Associates }}
*Marcon, J. L., Meissner, C. A., & Malpass, R. S. (in press). Cross-race effect in eyewitness identification. In B. Cutler's (Ed.), ''Encyclopedia of Psychology & Law''. Sage Publications
*Marcon, J. L., Meissner, C. A., & Malpass, R. S. (in press). Cross-race effect in eyewitness identification. In B. Cutler's (Ed.), ''Encyclopedia of Psychology & Law''. Sage Publications
*{{cite journal | last1 = Meissner | first1 = C. A. | last2 = Brigham | first2 = J. C. | year = 2001 | title = Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review | url = | journal = Psychology, Public Policy, and Law | volume = 7 | issue = | pages = 3–35 | doi=10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.3| citeseerx = 10.1.1.468.7169 }}
* {{cite journal | vauthors = Meissner CA, Brigham JC | year = 2001 | title = Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review | journal = Psychology, Public Policy, and Law | volume = 7 | issue = | pages = 3–35 | doi=10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.3| citeseerx = 10.1.1.468.7169 }}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Sporer | first1 = S. L. | year = 2001 | title = Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories | url = | journal = Psychology, Public Policy, and Law | volume = 7 | issue = | pages = 36–97 | doi=10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.36}}
* {{cite journal | vauthors = Sporer SL | year = 2001 | title = Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories | journal = Psychology, Public Policy, and Law | volume = 7 | issue = | pages = 36–97 | doi=10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.36}}
{{refend}}


{{ethnicity}}
{{ethnicity}}

Revision as of 09:18, 18 November 2020

The cross-race effect (sometimes called cross-race bias, other-race bias or own-race bias) is the tendency to more easily recognize faces that belong to one's own racial group. In social psychology, the cross-race effect is described as the "ingroup advantage," whereas in other fields, the effect can be seen as a specific form of the "ingroup advantage" since it is only applied in interracial or inter-ethnic situations.[1] The cross-race affect is thought to contribute to difficulties in cross-race identification, as well as implicit racial bias.[2]

Multiple theories as to why the cross-race effect exists have been conceived, including social cognition and perceptual expertise. However, no model has been able to fully account for the full body of evidence, so multiple theories are still considered in the literature. [3] 

History

The first research study on the cross-race effect was published in 1914.[4] It stated that humans tend to perceive people of other races than themselves to all look alike. All else being equal, individuals of a given race are distinguishable from each other in proportion to their familiarity or contact with the race as a whole. Thus, to the uninitiated Caucasian, all East Asian people look alike, while to East Asian people, all Caucasian people look alike. This does not hold true when people of different races familiarize themselves with races different from their own.

As research into the cross-race effect continued, multiple theories arose as to why the effect exists, including the contact hypothesis, different processing mechanisms, social cognition, feature selection, and cognitive disregard. However, each of these theories have their own challenges and conflicting evidence.[5]

Numerous studies on the cross-race effect have occurred. However, improvements in technology has allowed for researchers to study the cross-race effect from a neural and/or computational lens. Despite both occurring under the umbrella of facial processing, researchers have found that different areas of the brain activate when processing own-race vs other-race faces.[6] Research using computational models have found that the other race effect only occurs when facial processing and feature selection is influenced by biased experience.[7] However, as use of these methods to examine the cross-race effect is relatively new, further research is still needed.

Theoretical approaches

Ingroup advantage

Cross-race effect has a strong connection with the ingroup advantage phenomenon. With ingroup advantage, people evaluate and judge members of their own self-defined group as being better and fairer than members of other groups (outgroup disadvantage). Social psychologists have demonstrated in the last 30 years that even the smallest aspect of differentiation, like preference for flavor of ice cream or style of music, can trigger ingroup advantage. If the group-building factor is a person's race, then cross-race effect appears. The favoritism of ingroup members also results from the decreased inborn motivation to read the face of a person of another group or culture. Hess, Senecal & Kirouac[8] showed in 1996 that the motivation to decode the emotional facial expression instantly decreased when the experimental subject realized that the face belonged to a person of another race.

Cross-race effect and emotion recognition

A meta-analysis of several studies about emotion recognition in facial expressions revealed that people could recognize and interpret the emotional facial expression of a person of their own race faster and better than of a person of another race. These findings apply to all races in the same way.[9] Some studies show that other races, compared to one's own race, have differently shaped faces and different details within a facial expression, making it difficult for members of other races to decode emotional expressions.[10][11] However, studies have shown that the mood of the observer does not affect the cross-race effect.[12]

Social cognition

Figure 2 from "A Memory Computational Basis for the Other-Race Effect" (Yaros et al., 2019) that show evidence of the other race effect occurring when participants do a Mnemonic discrimination task but not a match to sample task, showing that mnemonic and proactive interference (old memories interfere with new) may contribute to the other-race effect (cross-race effect)[13]

Research has shown that people tend to think more categorically about outgroup members and more individually about ingroup members.[14] For example, outgroup members may associate specific facial features with a particular race or ethnicity, and do not notice the subtle variations in skin tone, lip size, or brow strength that ingroup members recognize. Categorical thinking happens more consistently for outgroup participants while individuation does the exact opposite.[14] These different views between outgroup and ingroup members have been known to bias conceptual cognitive processes and show that the cross-race effect actually has less to do with race than with different levels of cognitive processing that occur for ingroup and outgroup members.[14]

Cognitive disregard

Another set of cognitive theories related to cross-race effect focuses on how social categorization and individuation biases face memory.[14] Some researchers believe that the inability for ingroup members' to recognize differences in the features of outgroup members can be explained through cognitive disregard.[11] They find that the likelihood of falsely identifying a member of an out-group stems from an automatic encoding of a face without processing its unique features.[11] Thus, when presented with an out-group member who has a similar face to the one that was encoded, the in-group member automatically, but incorrectly determines that the face has been "seen" before.[11] These studies conclude that diminishing the cross race effect requires individuals to process ethnically-differing faces with the goal of encoding with individuation.[11]

Depth of processing hypothesis

Depth of processing also influences the presence of the cross race effect.[15] Same-race faces are more correctly and easily discerned because deep processing, than other-race faces are.[15] This hypothesis, however, is controversial because of its inability to be replicated within studies.[15]

Challenges for social cognition models

There are two challenges to the social cognition models (a) mixed evidence dealing with race accessibility, face perception, and memory and (b) the effects of development and training on the cross-race effect.[14] Regarding the mixed evidence, the popular belief is that the more someone is exposed to people of different races the less likely they will be affected by the cross-race effect.[14] There have been studies that support this theory, but other research has shown mixed results.[14] For example, the results of studies done where the accessibility, as in how easy or not it is for a person to be around people of difference races, to different races is manipulated, showed that this does not always affect face memory.[14] Second regarding the development and training effects, just because someone shows improvement with dealing with the cross-race effect due to exposure to cross race training or experience, it is not a direct prediction of a good social cognitive model.[14] For the social cognitive model to start explaining such effects there would have to be evidence that ingroup and outgroup distinctions occur developmentally at the exact time the cross-race effect emerges in a child.[14] There is some evidence showing when the cross-race effect first emerges, but there is little research directly testing the onset of ingroup and outgroup recognition biases in young children.[14]

While social cognition models indicate a lack of effort to individuate other-race faces explains the cross-race effect, some research has challenged this by arguing that individuals do not spend less effort looking at other-race faces than same-race ones.[16]

Perceptual expertise hypothesis

The perceptual expertise theory also suggest that if we identify the perceptual learning mechanisms that control perceptual expertise with face and non-face stimuli we will understand the cross-race effect.[14] There are many models that deal with perceptual expertise, but all of these models share the idea that a human's face processing ability does not generalize equally to all faces.[14] Hence, these theories propose that racial segregation results in people developing better expertise in distinguishing between faces of our own race or of a different race.[14] Research around these theories also suggests that the more a child is exposed to cross-race face processing the lower the cross-race effect.[14] However, if the child is not exposed to a lot of cross-race face processing the cross-race effect can increase.[14] Furthermore, there is evidence that long term and short term exposure to cross-race face processing can improve recognition.[14] In this view, the cross-race effect may not actually be a distinct, individual effect but rather an example of a larger difficulty in humans with the capacity to recognise unfamiliar groups and categories (such as unfamiliar sounds, animals, car models etc).[17]

Challenges for perceptual expertise models

Challenges to the perceptual expertise models are the mixed evidence for the interracial contact hypothesis, and the varied results of the training effect.[14] The mixed evidence shows that although there is some support to the theory that the more interracial contact a person has the better a person is at cross-race recognition, all the evidence gathered does not come to the same conclusion.[14] This mixture of results causes the relationship between cross-race exposure and recognition ability to weaken.[14] However, there may also be a third factor that contributes to these inconsistent findings.[14] There is some evidence that the quality of cross-race contact has an effect on this relationship.[14] For example, research supports the position that to be able to recognize cross-race faces one has to be attentive and effortful when encoding the face into memory.[14] Training individuals has been shown to reduce the cross race effect in people, however this quick onset is coupled with a quick off set of the ability.[14] Although, this short term training can translate into long term training, it is not the same as actually having real life experience with the cross-race effect.[14] Finally, there are also other processes besides perceptual expertise that can influence cross-race recognition.[14]

Effects of social cognition

Another reason the cross-race-effect may occur is that perceptions are often affected by motivations, expectations, and social cognition. Overall, the creation of norms has shaped and biased even simple perceptions such as line lengths. In terms of perception of faces, studies have shown that racially ambiguous faces that have been identified as one race or another based on their hairstyle are identified as having more features of the racial category represented by the hairstyle. Similarly, faces of an ambiguous but equal shade are interpreted as darker or lighter when accompanied by the label of either "black" or "white", respectively.[18]Other social cognitive biases may also have the ability to overpower the cross-race-effect. A study has shown that social perception of wealth has the ability to modulate the effect: when the targets were seen as impoverished, the difference in facial recognition disappeared.[19]

Integration of cross-race effect theories

Prototypes

Individuals develop and store a face prototype each time they encounter a face unique to ones he or she has previously encountered (usually ones that differ in features compared to his or her ethnic group).[11] From their studies, researchers have concluded that when an individual belonging to an ethnicity that differs from his or her own, he or she forms a prototype and reserves it for future use, if and when necessary.[11] The prototype view raises concern, however, because individuals storing these unique faces may ignore the fact that everyone has features that may be only special to his or her makeup, and may not apply to everyone belonging to that particular ethnic group or race; thus, this results in more false alarms during eyewitness testimony or identifying perpetrators in lineups.[11]

Race-feature theory

Deeper study of the cross-race effect has demonstrated two types of processing for the recognition of faces: featural and holistic. It has been found that holistic processing (which occurs beyond individual parts of the face) is more commonly used in same-race situations, but there is an experience effect, which means that as a person gains more experience with those of a particular race, he or she will begin to use more holistic processing. Featural processing is much more commonly used with an unfamiliar stimulus or face.[20]

grid with 20 faces, center column shows the original face (first two faces in column are white, last two faces are Asian) without editing. The leftmost face has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 10 pixels smaller than the original (the middle face), the face located second from the left has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 5 pixels smaller than the original, the rightmost face has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 10 pixels larger than the original, and the face located second from the right has the distance between eyes or between nose and mouth 5 pixels larger than the original.
Sample of real and edited Caucasian and Asian faces used in study of the Cross-race effect[21]

In his 1996 study, researchers noticed that when looking at ethnicity, in-group faces are processed without acknowledgement of ethnic-specific details and features.[11] People code faces that deviate from the norm of their ethnic group through the absence or presence of distinctive ethnic features.[11]

This is supported by the finding that the classification of other-race faces tends to be faster than same-race faces. This suggests that race seems to be a more perceptually salient feature than other more discerning facial features when the face belongs to a different race.[22] Some eye tracking studies found tentative evidence for such a hypothesis by demonstrating that people look at different facial features in same- versus other-race faces.[23][24][25][26] The general trend observed is that people fixate the eyes of a face with higher probability if it belongs to the same ethnic group as the observer her- or himself. However, other studies found just as pronounced differences between Asian and European observers′ looking behavior and these differences were stable for both own- and other-race faces.[27][28] This was previously explained as stemming from a more clustered density-gradient for other-race faces than same race faces.[29] The reasoning is that this causes more nodes to become activated in reaction to an other-race face, resulting in faster classification, but less discriminability in terms of memory. However, these exemplar-based theories cannot explain why faces that are ambiguous in terms of social category information can influence recognition.

Contact hypothesis theory

See also: Contact Hypothesis

One method researchers have suggested to help mollify the prevalence of the cross race effect is through application of the contact hypothesis. Accurate recognition and identification of other-race faces, researchers have deduced, stems from a difference in learning experiences that relate to individual ethnic groups.[30] The cross race effect can be reduced by continual exposure ethnic groups that differ from one's own; the more positive interactions that occur between two ethnic groups, the more heterogeneous the ethnicities will seem to be.[30] The type of contact experienced between the two ethnic groups also plays a major role in this hypothesis' effectiveness; the more intimate the contact, the higher the chances become of accurately recognizing a member of a different ethnicity than one's own[30] As an example, research done on Asian and white students living in Singapore and Canada showed a significant cross race effect that was not able to be predicted by perceived familiarity with the other race.[31] Previously seen as evidence against the contact hypothesis, it is now seen as evidence that the depth of contact is an important factor.[32]

Empirical findings

The cross-race effect across ethnic groups

Although most studies done about the cross race effect are with black and white participants, there are also numerous studies done with people of different ethnic backgrounds.[33] For example, there are studies done that compare Hispanic with black and white participants, black with white and Japanese participants, Chinese with Korean and white participants, Chinese with Indian and other East Asian ethnic participants,[34] Turkish and German participants, and finally a study has been done comparing Arab and Israeli Jews.[33] The data from all of these studies have come to the same conclusion. The cross-race effect is evident among all people of all different races.[33]

Morphological basis

The cross race effect has a morphological basis: The facial appearance is morphologically different for different ethnic backgrounds. This has been established empirically,[35] wherein a large set of 3D scans of faces from different ethnic backgrounds was automatically clustered into groups. Only facial landmark distances were used in this grouping. The result was that gender, as well as ethnicity, emerged as primary factors of group membership.

While the cross-race effect has been observed for adult faces, research indicates that infant faces do not produce a cross-race effect; infant faces seem to automatically draw the viewer's attention with the ethnicity of the infant having no effect.[36]

Immersion vs. upbringing

Children and face identification

With the help of several conducted studies, researchers conclude that the accuracy of eyewitness memory is significantly affected by the ethnic identity of both the suspect and the eye-witness; an individual can more accurately recognize a face belonging to his or her race than an individual whose race differs from that of his or her own.[37] Previous studies have analyzed how the cross-race effect affects adults during eyewitness testimony but fails to address the possible existence of age-related confounding factors: On one hand, as an individual grows older and encounters more members of the other ethnic group in question, the novelty of the ethnic difference wears off and makes it less distracting, and the individual can pay higher absolute and relative amounts of attention to subtle distinctions between members of that group; on the other hand, time also increases the individual's exposure to biases prevalent in his/her own in-group, as well as compounding the effects of any self-reinforcement bias that the individual exhibits with respect to his/her preexisting opinions.[37] The literature available on this topic is minute and conflicting; some researchers have found a prevalence of the cross-race effect in both white and black children,[37] yet others have reported findings of children possessing the ability to discern other-race faces accurately.[37] In their aim to identify developmental differences, researchers such as Pezdek et al.[37] discovered that children recognize faces belonging to their own race more effectively than faces belonging to another race.[37][further explanation needed]

Consequences

Cross-race identification bias

Ronald Cotton (left) and Jennifer Thomson-Cannino (right) sitting in two rolling office chairs in front of large red and blue Pop!Tech logo.
Ronald Cotton with Jennifer Thompson-Cannino at PopTech 2010. After Thompson-Cannino mistakenly identified Cotton as her rapist, Cotton was convicted of rape in 1985. A decade later, DNA evidence exonerated him. The case is often used as a real world example of the cross-race effect and the potential dangers of eyewitness testimony and lineup identification.

This effect refers to the decreased ability of people of one race to recognize faces and facial expressions of people of another race. This differs from the cross-race bias because this effect is found mostly during eyewitness identification as well as identification of a suspect in a line-up. In these situations, many people feel as if races other than their own look alike, and they have difficulty distinguishing between members of different ethnic groups. Cross-race identification bias is also known as the misinformation effect since people are considered to be misinformed about other races and have difficulty identifying them. A study was made which examined 271 real court cases. In photographic line-ups, 231 witnesses participated in cross-race versus same-race identification. In cross-race lineups, only 45% were correctly identified versus 60% for same-race identifications.[38] In a study dealing with eyewitness testimony, investigators examined forty participants in a racially diverse area of the US. Participants watched a video of a property crime being committed, then in the next 24 hours came to pick the suspect out of a photo line-up. Most of the participants in the study either misidentified the suspect or stated the suspect was not in the line-up at all. Correct identification of the suspect occurred more often when the eyewitness and the suspect were of the same race.[39] In another study, 86 convenience store clerks were asked to identify three customers: one white, one black, and one Mexican, all of whom had purchased in the store earlier that day. The clerks tended to identify customers belonging to their own race accurately, but were more likely to make errors when attempting to identify other races members.[40] Meanwhile, another study found that "alcohol intoxication reduces the own-race bias in face recognition," albeit by impairing accurate perception and leaving in place or increasing random error rather than by improving facial recognition of members of other groups.[41]

There has been some disagreement about the consistency of the own-race bias. However, data gathered from multiple studies does show that the own-race bias is very consistent.[42] Where it is not consistent, such as in a study done on white students in England and white and black students in South Africa, there are other factors associated. In this study the black South African students were slightly better at identifying white faces, but this is thought to be related to the significant contact the black students had with white students in University, as black non-students in South Africa were found to exhibit the own race bias[43]. The own-race bias occurs in people of all races.[42] Since eyewitness identification can be problematic, researchers have started to conduct studies of own-race biases using more forensics.[42] This kind of research needs to pay more attention to a target's distinctive features and level of attractiveness.[42] If a target is very distinctive or very attractive, it could reduce the cross-race effect because that person would be easier to identify.[42]

Psychological experts all agree that the cross-race effect is a common occurrence during in-court testimony when an eyewitness is trying to remember a person.[44] In order to reduce the cross-race effect there have been multiple changes to how policemen handle eyewitness identification.[45] For example, to reduce the cross-race identification bias Britain has a law that states police must include the suspect in a line up with at least eight other people who share similar characteristics to him or her.[45] This forces the eyewitness to use his or her memory of the suspects features, not the suspect's race, as a form of identification.[45] However, as evidence shows that cross race identification is more difficult when faces are viewed in a group, cross race identification still poses a risk.[46]

Economic

Research has shown, that when making financial decisions, specific facial characteristics and implicit bias can influence the perceived trustworthiness of another person.[47]

Mitigation of cross-race effect

Studies beyond the contact hypothesis have also been done to mitigate the cross-race effect. A study was done in which participants were forewarned about cross-race effect. Results from this study showed that the cross-race effect could be reduced and sometimes even eliminated when participants were wary of it.[48]Research has also found that individuation training in which other race faces are given specific characteristics can mitigate the cross-race effect. In the study, white participants were trained to identify faces from one race (ex. African American) by specific letters and categorize another race (ex. Asian) using only one letter. Results found that participants were better able to distinguish other race faces that were individuated better than those that were categorized, despite equal exposure. [49]In a study done on 43 white men, administering oxytocin before participants memorized faces was also found to reduce the cross-race effect. [50]

Similar biases have been found for aspects other than race. There is an own-gender bias, although evidence suggests that this comes down to hair style recognition.[51] Also, there is an own-age bias where people are better at recognising people of a similar age as themselves.[52]

See also

References

  1. ^ e.g. M. Beaupre (2006): An Ingroup Advantage for Confidence in Emotion Recognition Judgments: The Moderating Effect of Familiarity With the Expressions of Outgroup Members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, volume 32(1), pages 16–26.
  2. ^ Lebrecht S, Pierce LJ, Tarr MJ, Tanaka JW (2009-01-21). "Perceptual other-race training reduces implicit racial bias". PloS One. 4 (1): e4215. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004215. PMID 19156226.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  3. ^ Young SG, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ, Sacco DF (May 2012). "Perception and motivation in face recognition: a critical review of theories of the Cross-Race Effect". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 16 (2): 116–42. doi:10.1177/1088868311418987. PMID 21878608.
  4. ^ Feingold CA (1914). "The influence of environment on identification of persons and things". Journal of Criminal Law and Police Science. 5 (1): 39–51. doi:10.2307/1133283. JSTOR 1133283.
  5. ^ Young SG, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ, Sacco DF (May 2012). "Perception and motivation in face recognition: a critical review of theories of the Cross-Race Effect". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 16 (2): 116–42. doi:10.1177/1088868311418987. PMID 21878608.
  6. ^ Natu V, O'Toole AJ (2013-09-01). "Neural perspectives on the other-race effect". Visual Cognition. 21 (9–10): 1081–1095. doi:10.1080/13506285.2013.811455.
  7. ^ O'Toole AJ, Natu V (2013-09-01). "Computational perspectives on the other-race effect". Visual Cognition. 21 (9–10): 1121–1137. doi:10.1080/13506285.2013.803505.
  8. ^ Hess, U.; Kappas, A.; Bause, R. (1995): The intensity of facial expression is determined by underlying affective states and social situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Band 69(2), S. 280-288
  9. ^ Elfenbein, H.A. & Ambidi, N. (2002): On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, Band 128(2), S. 203–235
  10. ^ Anthony, T.; Cooper, C. & Mullen, B. (1992): Cross-racial facial identification: A social cognitive integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Band 18, S. 296–301
  11. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Sporer SL (1 January 2001). "Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 7 (1): 36–97. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.36.
  12. ^ Teitelbaum S, Geiselman RE (1 January 1997). "Observer Mood and Cross-Racial Recognition of Faces". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 28 (1): 93–106. doi:10.1177/0022022197281006.
  13. ^ Yaros JL, Salama DA, Delisle D, Larson MS, Miranda BA, Yassa MA (December 2019). "A Memory Computational Basis for the Other-Race Effect". Scientific Reports. 9 (1): 19399. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-55350-0. PMID 31853093.
  14. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z Young SG, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ, Sacco DF (May 2012). "Perception and motivation in face recognition: a critical review of theories of the Cross-Race Effect". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 16 (2): 116–42. doi:10.1177/1088868311418987. PMID 21878608.
  15. ^ a b c Meissner CA, Brigham JC, Butz DA (1 July 2005). "Memory for own- and other-race faces: a dual-process approach". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 19 (5): 545–567. doi:10.1002/acp.1097.
  16. ^ Crookes, Kate, and Gillian Rhodes. "Poor recognition of other-race faces cannot always be explained by a lack of effort." Visual Cognition 25, no. 4-6 (2017): 430-441.
  17. ^ Malinowska, Joanna K. "Cultural neuroscience and the category of race: the case of the other-race effect." Synthese 193, no. 12 (2016): 3865-3887.
  18. ^ Corneille O, Hugenberg K, Potter T (September 2007). "Applying the attractor field model to social cognition: Perceptual discrimination is facilitated, but memory is impaired for faces displaying evaluatively congruent expressions". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (3): 335–52. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.586.1592. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.335. PMID 17723052.
  19. ^ Shriver ER, Young SG, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ, Lanter JR (February 2008). "Class, race, and the face: social context modulates the cross-race effect in face recognition". Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 34 (2): 260–74. doi:10.1177/0146167207310455. PMID 18212334.
  20. ^ Tanaka, J. W., Kiefer, M., & Bukach, C. M. (2003). A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: evidence from a cross-cultural study. Elsevier.
  21. ^ Wang Z, Quinn PC, Tanaka JW, Yu X, Sun YH, Liu J, et al. (2015). "An other-race effect for configural and featural processing of faces: upper and lower face regions play different roles". Frontiers in Psychology. 6: 559. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00559. PMID 26005427.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  22. ^ Levin DT (1996). "Classifying faces by race. The structure of face categories". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 22 (6): 1364–1382. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1364.
  23. ^ Brielmann AA, Bülthoff I, Armann R (July 2014). "Looking at faces from different angles: Europeans fixate different features in Asian and Caucasian faces". Vision Research. 100: 105–12. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.04.011. PMID 24796509.
  24. ^ Wheeler A, Anzures G, Quinn PC, Pascalis O, Omrin DS, Lee K (April 2011). "Caucasian infants scan own- and other-race faces differently". PloS One. 6 (4): e18621. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018621. PMC 3076379. PMID 21533235.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  25. ^ Goldinger SD, He Y, Papesh MH (September 2009). "Deficits in cross-race face learning: insights from eye movements and pupillometry". Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 35 (5): 1105–22. doi:10.1037/a0016548. PMC 2871406. PMID 19686008.
  26. ^ Fu G, Hu CS, Wang Q, Quinn PC, Lee K (2012). "Adults scan own- and other-race faces differently". PloS One. 7 (6): e37688. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037688. PMC 3365898. PMID 22675486.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  27. ^ Blais C, Jack RE, Scheepers C, Fiset D, Caldara R (August 2008). "Culture shapes how we look at faces". PloS One. 3 (8): e3022. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003022. PMC 2515341. PMID 18714387.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  28. ^ Kelly DJ, Miellet S, Caldara R (2010). "Culture shapes eye movements for visually homogeneous objects". Frontiers in Psychology. 1: 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00006. PMC 3153738. PMID 21833189.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  29. ^ Valentine T, Endo M (May 1992). "Towards an exemplar model of face processing: the effects of race and distinctiveness". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology. 44 (4): 671–703. doi:10.1080/14640749208401305. PMID 1615169.
  30. ^ a b c Combs GM, Griffith J (1 September 2007). "An Examination of Interracial Contact:The Influence of Cross-Race Interpersonal Efficacy and Affect Regulation". Human Resource Development Review. 6 (3): 222–244. doi:10.1177/1534484307303990.
  31. ^ Ng WJ, Lindsay RC (2016-07-27). "Cross-Race Facial Recognition: Failure of the Contact Hypothesis". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. doi:10.1177/0022022194252004.
  32. ^ Wilson JP, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ (2013). "The Cross-Race Effect and Eyewitness Identification: How to Improve Recognition and Reduce Decision Errors in Eyewitness Situations". Social Issues and Policy Review. 7 (1): 83–113. doi:10.1111/j.1751-2409.2012.01044.x.
  33. ^ a b c Toglia MP, ed. (2007). Handbook of eyewitness psychology. Mahwah, NJ [u.a.]: Erlbaum. pp. 257–81. ISBN 978-1-4106-1491-9.
  34. ^ Perera-WA H (2014). "The Effects of Memory Conformity and the Cross-Race Effect in Eyewitness Testimony". doi:10.2139/ssrn.2732189. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  35. ^ Salah AA, Alyüz N, Akarun L (2008). "Registration of 3D Face Scans with Average Face Models". Journal of Electronic Imaging. 17 (1): 011006. doi:10.1117/1.2896291.
  36. ^ Proverbio, Alice Mado, and Valeria De Gabriele. "The other-race effect does not apply to infant faces: An ERP attentional study." Neuropsychologia 126 (2019): 36-45.
  37. ^ a b c d e f Pezdek K, Blandon-Gitlin I, Moore C (August 2003). "Children's face recognition memory: more evidence for the cross-race effect". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 88 (4): 760–3. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.365.6517. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.4.760. PMID 12940414.
  38. ^ Behrman BW, Davey SL (2001). "Eyewitness identification in actual criminal cases: An archival analysis". Law and Human Behavior. 25 (5): 475–491. doi:10.1023/a:1012840831846.
  39. ^ Josephson, S. & Holmes, M. (2008). Cross-race recognition deficit and visual attention: do they all look (at faces) alike?. Proceeding ETRA '08 Proceedings of the 2008 symposium on Eye tracking research & applications. ACM New York, NY, USA.
  40. ^ Platz SJ, Hosch HM (1988). "Cross-Racial/Ethnic Eyewitness Identification: A Field Study". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 18 (11): 972–984. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1988.tb01187.x.
  41. ^ Hilliar KF, Kemp RI, Denson TF (October 2010). "Now everyone looks the same: alcohol intoxication reduces the own-race bias in face recognition". Law and Human Behavior. 34 (5): 367–78. doi:10.1007/s10979-009-9204-x. PMID 20130972.
  42. ^ a b c d e Bothwell RK, Brigham JC, Malpass RS (1 March 1989). "Cross-Racial Identification". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 15 (1): 19–25. doi:10.1177/0146167289151002.
  43. ^ Wright DB, Boyd CE, Tredoux CG (April 2003). "Inter-racial contact and the own-race bias for face recognition in South Africa and England". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 17 (3): 365–373. doi:10.1002/acp.898. ISSN 0888-4080.
  44. ^ Sporer SL (1 January 2001). "The cross-race effect: Beyond recognition of faces in the laboratory". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 7 (1): 170–200. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.170.
  45. ^ a b c Revlin R (2007). Human Cognition : Theory and Practice. New York, NY: Worth Pub. pp. 110–11. ISBN 9780716756675.
  46. ^ Pezdek K, Wasson C, Corey P, Pezdek K, Wasson C. Cross-Race (but Not Same-Race) Face Identification Is Impaired by Presenting Faces in a Group Rather Than Individually.
  47. ^ Stanley DA, Sokol-Hessner P, Banaji MR, Phelps EA (May 2011). "Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (19): 7710–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.1014345108. PMC 3093479. PMID 21518877.
  48. ^ Hugenberg K, Miller J, Claypool HM (2007). "Categorization and individuation in the cross-race recognition deficit: Toward a solution to an insidious problem". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 43 (2): 334–340. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.02.010.
  49. ^ Lebrecht S, Pierce LJ, Tarr MJ, Tanaka JW (2009-01-21). "Perceptual other-race training reduces implicit racial bias". PloS One. 4 (1): e4215. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004215. PMC 2627769. PMID 19156226.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  50. ^ Blandón-Gitlin I, Pezdek K, Saldivar S, Steelman E (September 2014). "Oxytocin eliminates the own-race bias in face recognition memory". Brain Research. Oxytocin in Human Social Behavior and Psychopathology. 1580: 180–7. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2013.07.015. PMID 23872107.
  51. ^ Wright DB (2003). "An own gender bias and the importance of hair in face recognition". Acta Psychologica. 114: 101–114. doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(03)00052-0.
  52. ^ Hills PJ, Lewis MB (January 2011). "The own-age face recognition bias in children and adults". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 64 (1): 17–23. doi:10.1080/17470218.2010.537926. PMID 21213196.

Further reading

  • Brigham JC, Bennett LB, Meissner CA, Mitchell TL (2006). Lindsay R, Ross D, Read J, Toglia M (eds.). Handbook of Eyewitness Psychology: Memory for People. Lawrence Erlbaum & Associates. pp. 257–281.
  • Marcon, J. L., Meissner, C. A., & Malpass, R. S. (in press). Cross-race effect in eyewitness identification. In B. Cutler's (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Psychology & Law. Sage Publications
  • Meissner CA, Brigham JC (2001). "Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 7: 3–35. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.468.7169. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.3.
  • Sporer SL (2001). "Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 7: 36–97. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.36.