Jump to content

Head-marking language: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Ceccee (talk | contribs)
m Hyphenated "head-marking", etc., thruout; linked to corrected graphic
Tags: Mobile edit Mobile web edit
 
(25 intermediate revisions by 19 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Grammar where inflection "agrees" with primary components of phrases}}
A language is '''head-marking''' if the [[grammar|grammatical]] marks showing [[Agreement (linguistics)|agreement]] between different words of a [[phrase]] tend to be placed on the [[Head (linguistics)|heads]] (or nuclei) of phrases, rather than on the modifiers or dependents. Many languages employ both head-marking and [[dependent-marking language|dependent-marking]], and some languages double up and are thus [[Double-marking language|double-marking]]. The concept of head/dependent-marking was proposed by [[Johanna Nichols]] in 1986 and has come to be widely used as a basic category in [[linguistic typology]].<ref>See Nichols (1986).</ref>


A language is '''head-marking''' if the [[grammar|grammatical]] marks showing [[Agreement (linguistics)|agreement]] between different words of a [[phrase]] tend to be placed on the [[Head (linguistics)|heads]] (or nuclei) of phrases, rather than on the [[Modifier (grammar)|modifiers]] or [[Dependent (grammar)|dependents]]. Many languages employ both head-marking and [[dependent-marking language|dependent-marking]], and some languages double up and are thus [[Double-marking language|double-marking]]. The concept of head/dependent-marking was proposed by [[Johanna Nichols]] in 1986 and has come to be widely used as a basic category in [[linguistic typology]].<ref>See Nichols (1986).</ref>
==Head-marking (and dependent-marking) in English==

The concepts of head-marking and dependent-marking are commonly applied to languages that have richer inflectional morphology than English. There are, however, a few types of agreement in English that can be used to illustrate these notions. The following graphic representations of a [[clause]], a [[noun phrase]], and a [[prepositional phrase]] involve agreement. The tree structures shown are those of a [[dependency grammar]] (as opposed to those of a [[phrase structure grammar]]):<ref>Dependency grammar trees similar to the ones shown here can be found in, for instance, Ágel et al. (2003/6).</ref>
==In English==
The concepts of head-marking and dependent-marking are commonly applied to languages that have richer inflectional morphology than [[English language|English]]. There are, however, a few types of agreement in English that can be used to illustrate those notions. The following graphic representations of a [[clause]], a [[noun phrase]], and a [[prepositional phrase]] involve agreement. The three tree structures shown are those of a [[dependency grammar]], as opposed to those of a [[phrase structure grammar]]:<ref>Dependency grammar trees similar to the ones that are shown can be found in, for instance, Ágel et al. (2003/6).</ref>


::[[File:Head-marking-he-not-I.png|Head-marking 1]]
::[[File:Head-marking-he-not-I.png|Head-marking 1]]


Heads and dependents are identified by the actual hierarchy of words, whereas the concepts of head-marking and dependent-marking are indicated with the arrows. Subject-verb agreement shown in the tree on the left is a case of head-marking because the singular subject ''John'' requires the inflectional suffix ''-s'' to appear on the finite verb ''cheats'', which is the head of the clause; the determiner-noun agreement shown in the tree in the middle is a case of dependent-marking because the plural noun ''houses'' requires the dependent determiner to appear in its plural form ''these'', not in its singular form ''this''; and the preposition-pronoun agreement of [[case government]] shown in the tree on the right is also an instance of dependent-marking because the head preposition ''with'' requires the dependent pronoun to appear in its object form ''him'', not in its subject form ''he''.
Heads and dependents are identified by the actual hierarchy of words, and the concepts of head-marking and dependent-marking are indicated with the arrows. Subject-verb agreement, shown in the tree on the left, is a case of head-marking because the singular subject ''John'' requires the inflectional suffix ''-s'' to appear on the finite verb ''cheats'', the head of the clause. The determiner-noun agreement, shown in the tree in the middle, is a case of dependent-marking because the plural noun ''houses'' requires the dependent determiner to appear in its plural form, ''these'', not in its singular form, ''this''. The preposition-pronoun agreement of [[case government]], shown in the tree on the right, is also an instance of dependent-marking because the head preposition ''with'' requires the dependent pronoun to appear in its object form, ''him'', not in its subject form, ''he''.


==Noun phrases and verb phrases==
==Noun phrases and verb phrases==
The distinction between head and dependent-marking shows up most in noun phrases and verb phrases, whereby there is significant variation across languages and within individual languages.<ref>The website [[World Atlas of Language Structures|World Atlas of Language Structure]]s is dedicated in part to documenting the distribution of head-marking and dependent-marking in noun and verb phrases among the world's languages.</ref>
The distinction between head-marking and dependent-marking shows the most in noun phrases and verb phrases, which have significant variation among and within languages.<ref>The [[World Atlas of Language Structures]] is dedicated in part to documenting the distribution of head-marking and dependent-marking in noun and verb phrases among the world's languages.</ref>


::{| class="wikitable"
::{| class="wikitable"
!Phrase type!!Head!!Dependents!!Global distribution map ([[World Atlas of Language Structures|WALS]])
!Phrase type!!Head!!Dependents!!Global distribution map ([[World Atlas of Language Structures|WALS]])
|-
|-
|[[Noun phrase]]||[[Noun]]s||[[adjective]]s, [[possessive]]s, [[relative clause]]s, etc.||[http://wals.info/feature/description/24 Marking in Possessive Noun Phrases]
|[[Noun phrase]]||[[Noun]]s||[[adjective]]s, [[possessive]]s, [[relative clause]]s, etc.||[http://wals.info/feature/24A Marking in Possessive Noun Phrases]
|-
|-
|[[Verb phrase]] (theory A)||[[Verb]]||[[verb argument]]s||[http://wals.info/feature/description/23 Marking in the Clause: Head-marking]
|[[Verb phrase]] (theory A)||[[Verb]]||[[verb argument]]s||[http://wals.info/feature/23A Marking in the Clause: Head-marking]
|-
|-
|[[Verb phrase]] (theory B)||[[Subject (grammar)|Subject]]||[[verb]]s||[http://wals.info/feature/description/23 Marking in the Clause: Dependent-marking]
|[[Verb phrase]] (theory B)||[[Subject (grammar)|Subject]]||[[verb]]s||[http://wals.info/feature/23A Marking in the Clause: Dependent-marking]
|}
|}


Languages may be head-marking in verb phrases and dependent-marking in noun phrases—such as most Bantu languages—or vice versa, and it has been argued that the subject rather than the verb is the head of a clause, so "head-marking" is not necessarily a coherent typology. Nonetheless, languages which are head-marking in both noun and verb phrases are common enough to make the term useful for typological description.
Languages may be head-marking in verb phrases and dependent-marking in noun phrases, such as most [[Bantu languages]], or vice versa, and it has been argued that the subject rather than the verb is the head of a clause so "head-marking" is not necessarily a coherent typology. Still, languages that are head-marking in both noun and verb phrases are common enough to make the term useful for typological description.


==Geographical distribution==
==Geographical distribution==
Head-marked possessive noun phrases are common in the Americas and Melanesia and infrequent elsewhere. Dependent-marked noun phrases have a complementary distribution: frequent in Africa, Eurasia, Australia, and New Guinea, the only area where the two types overlap appreciably. Double-marked possession is rare but found around the Eurasian periphery (such as [[Finnish language|Finnish]]), in the Himalayas, and along the Pacific coast of North America. Zero-marked possession is also uncommon with instances mostly found near the [[equator]], but does not form any true clusters.<ref>[http://wals.info/feature/description/24 WALS - Locus of Marking in Possessive Noun Phrases]</ref>
Head-marked possessive noun phrases are common in the Americas, Melanesia, [[Afro-Asiatic language|Afro-Asiatic languages]] ([[status constructus]]) and [[Turkic languages]] and infrequent elsewhere. Dependent-marked noun phrases have a complementary distribution and are frequent in [[Africa]], [[Eurasia]], [[Australia]], and [[New Guinea]], the only area in which both types overlap appreciably. Double-marked possession is rare but found in languages around the Eurasian periphery such as [[Finnish language|Finnish]], in the [[Himalayas]], and along the [[Pacific Coast]] of [[North America]]. [[Zero-marking language|Zero-marked]] possession is also uncommon, with instances mostly found near the [[equator]], but it does not form any true clusters.<ref>[http://wals.info/feature/description/24 WALS - Locus of Marking in Possessive Noun Phrases]</ref>


The head-marked [[clause]] is common in the Americas, Australia, New Guinea, among the Bantu languages, and very rare elsewhere. The dependent-marked clause is common in Eurasia and northern Africa, sparse in South America, and rare in North America. In New Guinea it clusters in the Eastern Highlands, and in Australia in the south, east, and interior, with the very old [[Pama-Nyungan]] family. Double-marking is moderately well attested in the Americas, Australia, and New Guinea, and the southern fringe of Eurasia (chiefly in the [[Languages of the Caucasus|Caucasian]] and Himalayan mountain enclaves), and particularly favored in Australia and the westernmost Americas. The zero-marked object is, unsurprisingly, common in Southeast Asia and western Africa, two centers of [[morphology (linguistics)|morphological]] simplicity, but also very common in New Guinea and moderately common in eastern Africa and Central and South America, among languages of average or higher morphological complexity.<ref>[http://wals.info/feature/description/23 WALS - Locus of Marking in the Clause]</ref><ref>See Nichols (1992).</ref>
The head-marked [[clause]] is common in the [[Americas]], Australia, New Guinea, and the Bantu languages but is very rare elsewhere. The dependent-marked clause is common in Eurasia and [[Northern Africa]], sparse in [[South America]], and rare in North America. In New Guinea, it clusters in the Eastern Highlands and in Australia in the south, east, and interior with the very old [[Pama-Nyungan]] family. Double-marking is moderately well attested in the Americas, Australia, and New Guinea, and the southern fringe of Eurasia (chiefly in the [[Caucasian languages]] and Himalayan mountain enclaves), and it is particularly favored in Australia and the westernmost Americas. The zero-marked object is unsurprisingly common in [[Southeast Asia]] and [[Western Africa]], two centers of [[morphology (linguistics)|morphological]] simplicity, but it is also very common in New Guinea and moderately common in [[Eastern Africa]] and [[Central America]] and South America, among languages of average or higher morphological complexity.<ref>[http://wals.info/feature/description/23 WALS - Locus of Marking in the Clause]</ref><ref>See Nichols (1992).</ref>


The Pacific Rim distribution of head-marking may reflect [[Models of migration to the New World|population movements beginning tens of thousands of years ago]] and [[founder effect]]s. [[Kusunda language|Traces in Himalayan]] or Caucasian enclaves may be remnants of [[linguistic typology|typology]] preceding spreads of [[Borean languages|interior Eurasian language families]]. The dependent-marking type is found everywhere but rare in the Americas, possibly another result of founder effects. In the Americas, all four types are found along the Pacific coast but in the East only head-marking is common. Whether the diversity of types along the Pacific coast reflects great age or overlay of more recent Eurasian colonizations on an earlier American stratum remains to be seen.<ref>[http://wals.info/feature/description/25 WALS - Locus of Marking: Whole-language Typology]</ref>
The [[Pacific Rim]] distribution of head-marking may reflect [[models of migration to the New World|population movements beginning tens of thousands of years ago]] and [[founder effects]]. [[Kusunda language|Kusunda]] has traces in the Himalayas, and there are Caucasian enclaves, both of which are perhaps remnants of [[linguistic typology|typology]] preceding the spreads of [[Eurasiatic languages|interior Eurasian language families]]. The dependent-marking type is found everywhere but rare in the Americas, possibly another result of founder effects. In the Americas, all four types are found along the Pacific Coast, but in the East, only head-marking is common. Whether the diversity of types along the Pacific Coast reflects a great age or an overlay of more recent Eurasian colonizations on an earlier American stratum remains to be seen.<ref>[http://wals.info/feature/description/25 WALS - Locus of Marking: Whole-language Typology]</ref>


== See also ==
== See also ==
{{div col|cols=3}}
{{div col|colwidth=22em}}
*[[Dependency grammar]]
*[[Dependency grammar]]
*[[Head (linguistics)|Head]]
*[[Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time]]
*[[Linguistic typology]]
*[[Dependent-marking language]]
*[[Dependent-marking language]]
*[[Double-marking language]]
*[[Double-marking language]]
*[[Head (linguistics)|Head]]
*''[[Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time]]''
*[[Linguistic typology]]
*[[Phrase]]
*[[Phrase]]
*[[Verb phrase]]
*[[Verb phrase]]
Line 44: Line 46:


==Notes==
==Notes==
{{Reflist|2}}
{{Reflist|30em}}


==References==
==References==
*Ágel, V., L. Eichinger, H.-W. Eroms, P. Hellwig, H. Heringer, and H. Lobin (eds.) 2003/6. Dependency and valency: An international handbook of contemporary research. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
*Ágel, V., L. Eichinger, H.-W. Eroms, P. Hellwig, H. Heringer, and H. Lobin (eds.) 2003/6. ''Dependency and Valency: An International Handbook of Contemporary Research.'' Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
*Nichols, J. 1986. Head-marking and dependent-marking grammar. Language 62, 1, 56-119.
*Nichols, J. 1986. "Head-marking and dependent-marking grammar," in ''Language'' 62, 1, 56-119.
*Nichols, J. 1992. Linguistic diversity in space and time. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
*Nichols, J. 1992. ''Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time.'' Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

[[Category:Linguistic typology]]
[[Category:Linguistic typology]]

Latest revision as of 09:57, 25 June 2024

A language is head-marking if the grammatical marks showing agreement between different words of a phrase tend to be placed on the heads (or nuclei) of phrases, rather than on the modifiers or dependents. Many languages employ both head-marking and dependent-marking, and some languages double up and are thus double-marking. The concept of head/dependent-marking was proposed by Johanna Nichols in 1986 and has come to be widely used as a basic category in linguistic typology.[1]

In English

[edit]

The concepts of head-marking and dependent-marking are commonly applied to languages that have richer inflectional morphology than English. There are, however, a few types of agreement in English that can be used to illustrate those notions. The following graphic representations of a clause, a noun phrase, and a prepositional phrase involve agreement. The three tree structures shown are those of a dependency grammar, as opposed to those of a phrase structure grammar:[2]

Head-marking 1

Heads and dependents are identified by the actual hierarchy of words, and the concepts of head-marking and dependent-marking are indicated with the arrows. Subject-verb agreement, shown in the tree on the left, is a case of head-marking because the singular subject John requires the inflectional suffix -s to appear on the finite verb cheats, the head of the clause. The determiner-noun agreement, shown in the tree in the middle, is a case of dependent-marking because the plural noun houses requires the dependent determiner to appear in its plural form, these, not in its singular form, this. The preposition-pronoun agreement of case government, shown in the tree on the right, is also an instance of dependent-marking because the head preposition with requires the dependent pronoun to appear in its object form, him, not in its subject form, he.

Noun phrases and verb phrases

[edit]

The distinction between head-marking and dependent-marking shows the most in noun phrases and verb phrases, which have significant variation among and within languages.[3]

Phrase type Head Dependents Global distribution map (WALS)
Noun phrase Nouns adjectives, possessives, relative clauses, etc. Marking in Possessive Noun Phrases
Verb phrase (theory A) Verb verb arguments Marking in the Clause: Head-marking
Verb phrase (theory B) Subject verbs Marking in the Clause: Dependent-marking

Languages may be head-marking in verb phrases and dependent-marking in noun phrases, such as most Bantu languages, or vice versa, and it has been argued that the subject rather than the verb is the head of a clause so "head-marking" is not necessarily a coherent typology. Still, languages that are head-marking in both noun and verb phrases are common enough to make the term useful for typological description.

Geographical distribution

[edit]

Head-marked possessive noun phrases are common in the Americas, Melanesia, Afro-Asiatic languages (status constructus) and Turkic languages and infrequent elsewhere. Dependent-marked noun phrases have a complementary distribution and are frequent in Africa, Eurasia, Australia, and New Guinea, the only area in which both types overlap appreciably. Double-marked possession is rare but found in languages around the Eurasian periphery such as Finnish, in the Himalayas, and along the Pacific Coast of North America. Zero-marked possession is also uncommon, with instances mostly found near the equator, but it does not form any true clusters.[4]

The head-marked clause is common in the Americas, Australia, New Guinea, and the Bantu languages but is very rare elsewhere. The dependent-marked clause is common in Eurasia and Northern Africa, sparse in South America, and rare in North America. In New Guinea, it clusters in the Eastern Highlands and in Australia in the south, east, and interior with the very old Pama-Nyungan family. Double-marking is moderately well attested in the Americas, Australia, and New Guinea, and the southern fringe of Eurasia (chiefly in the Caucasian languages and Himalayan mountain enclaves), and it is particularly favored in Australia and the westernmost Americas. The zero-marked object is unsurprisingly common in Southeast Asia and Western Africa, two centers of morphological simplicity, but it is also very common in New Guinea and moderately common in Eastern Africa and Central America and South America, among languages of average or higher morphological complexity.[5][6]

The Pacific Rim distribution of head-marking may reflect population movements beginning tens of thousands of years ago and founder effects. Kusunda has traces in the Himalayas, and there are Caucasian enclaves, both of which are perhaps remnants of typology preceding the spreads of interior Eurasian language families. The dependent-marking type is found everywhere but rare in the Americas, possibly another result of founder effects. In the Americas, all four types are found along the Pacific Coast, but in the East, only head-marking is common. Whether the diversity of types along the Pacific Coast reflects a great age or an overlay of more recent Eurasian colonizations on an earlier American stratum remains to be seen.[7]

See also

[edit]

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^ See Nichols (1986).
  2. ^ Dependency grammar trees similar to the ones that are shown can be found in, for instance, Ágel et al. (2003/6).
  3. ^ The World Atlas of Language Structures is dedicated in part to documenting the distribution of head-marking and dependent-marking in noun and verb phrases among the world's languages.
  4. ^ WALS - Locus of Marking in Possessive Noun Phrases
  5. ^ WALS - Locus of Marking in the Clause
  6. ^ See Nichols (1992).
  7. ^ WALS - Locus of Marking: Whole-language Typology

References

[edit]
  • Ágel, V., L. Eichinger, H.-W. Eroms, P. Hellwig, H. Heringer, and H. Lobin (eds.) 2003/6. Dependency and Valency: An International Handbook of Contemporary Research. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
  • Nichols, J. 1986. "Head-marking and dependent-marking grammar," in Language 62, 1, 56-119.
  • Nichols, J. 1992. Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.